Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 111

q

,r

//
.i li i

i -' ..i i, :r
i :)
!r :, I li ii ir, Jf: tt .1i

| ./
1 ta:

':j :

.. i .: i :l i i 11
r't". ;l ,

!-
'.:

,!
1",..
| :,'
-', I- .;
:i l.i | ) ll f i , .a.i
i; ;1 ,. t: 1i.,
: .

t
l
tl "{

:
"t: a

:.:
:: l
:

!.l
|
:t
'-,,
tl
' ll
i'. ', jI
ir l
!
:
il :i
ri :.:
'I :: 1,
i i, 'f\

qll:ifIll4
4xlsts
The (Jniuersiu of Chicago Press
W
l1T+r
,c-{ g

ror S
PRE,F'ACE,

Every musician, whether composer, performer, or theorist will agree that "In
the beginning was rhythm." For the shaping power of rhythm and, more broadly
speaking, of the temporal organization of music, is a sine qua nln of the art. An
understanding of rhythm is important for performer as well as composer, for
historiart as well as music theorist. Yet the study of this aspect of music has been
almost totally neglected in the formal training of musicians since the Renaissance.
There are many textbooks on harmony and counterpoint but none on rhythm.
Although theorists have frequently written about the temporal organization of
music, their concern has generally been with meter and phrase structure rather
than with rhythm. Every music school requires students to take courses in
harmony and counterpoint, but few give more than passing notice to the
rhythmic structure of music.
A book dealing with rhythm must therefore perform two functions at once.
It must, on the one hand, organize and develop a conceptual framework-a
theory of rhythmic structure. And it must, on the other hand, present through
discussion, example, and precept, analytical methods and compositional pro-
cedures.
This book does not presume to answer all questions in the field of rhythm. Nor
af* does it pretend to cover all possible rhythmic problems. It is a first book on the
OF CHICAGO subj6ct, a text intended for students not too advanced in theoretical studies but
TIBRARY already familiar with harmony and counterpoint. It is also a beginning in the
sense that it will, we hope, lead to further study of rhythm and better, more
comprehensive texts.
Since there are at present very few courses in rhythm, this book is designed to
Library oJ Congres Catalog Numbet: @-14068 be used in conjunction with courses in harmony or counterpoint, courses in
interpretation, and courses in analysis. It has been successfully used in these
Tne lJxrvnnsrrv or Cnrc,rco Pnrss, CHtclco 37

The University of Toronto Press, Toronto 5, Canada ways at the University of Chicago. We hope that, as a result of its existence,
courses dealing with rhythm will begin to be offered as part of the music cur-
Chicago. Publishcd 1960
@ lgfu b7 Thc tlniacrsit2 of riculum.
ComPoscd and Printcd bY
Tnr IJr.lrvsnsrrv or Cntceco Pnsss We are well aware of the fact that some readers will take exception either to
Chicago, Illinois, U,S'A.
a
HAN
^I
t
ai Preface

thegeneralconceptualframeworkadvancedhereortoparticularanalyses.
that dissenting voices will provoke
With regard to the ro*.,, we can but hope
a fruitful discussion on a subject which
ftu" Uy seemingly common consent' been
falli-
neglected by music theoristJ. with regard to the latter, we can but plead
present undertaking is the method
bility and .*pf,urlr. it at what is crucial i.t the
and generat ,ri.*poi.rt rather than their exemplifications in any particular
t?ilil-;""k
is the resurr of many fruitful, and some
fruitless, discussions-and CONTE,NTS
and with
sometimes u.g',,'.*l*hich we have had both with our colleagues
ourstudents'Thedebtwhichweowetothemisfreelyacknowledged'Inpar- helpful sugges- ::
u'hose
ticular we wish to irr."r. Mrs. Jeanne Bamberger, -many ..i Defnitions and Principles 7
way into this book'
tionsand cogent ..iii"i*- havJfound their
Architectonic Levels 2
Pulse 3
Meter 4

Rhythm 6
Accent 7
Stress 8
Grouping 8
i il'
)t t, Rlrythms on Llwer Architectonic Leuels 72
Trochee and Iamb in Duple Meter 12

Articulation and Structure /7


Amphibrach, Anapest, and Dactyl in Duple Meter 18
Dactyl, Anapest, and Amphibrach in Triple Meter 23
The Character of Particular Rhythms 26
Iamb and Trochee in Triple Meter 29
Rhythmic Ambiguity 32
The Influence of Rhythm on Melodic Motion 36
Recapitulationand Illustration 37
Exercises 57

il li t
More Complex Rh2thmic Structures 60
Theoretical Considerations 61

The Coherence of Higher-Level Rhythms 62

an
Contents ix
uiii Contents - r_ ':!' :r:
\s a
:!
!r .a
,:

Groupings on Higher Levels 68 Y :i":i il Extended Examples l,83


Analysis of a Whole Section 83 Chopin, Prelude in E-flat, Op.24 185

Exercises 87 Beethoven, Symphony No. B, First Movement 788

-',,
it ;'

"f
i Y Rhltthm and Meter 88 List of Symbols 204
Meter and the Bar Line 88
Non-congruence 89
Index 205
The lnfluence of Stress upon Meter
98

Syncopations, Suspensions, and Ties 99

Metric Crossing 106 Index of Music 208


Exercises 715

Y Rhlthm, Mobititlt, and Tension 117

Rhythm and MobilitY 117


Rhythm and Tension 125
An Extended Anacrusis 129

An Accented Rest 737


SummarY ExamPle 140
Exercises 142

Rhltthm, ContinuitY, and Form 744


Rhythm, Form, and Morphological
Lengths 744

ContinuitY and Form 147

Themes, Non-themes, and Continuity 153

RhYthm and Texture 160

Exercises 167

': '! i "i t'

'"
'i.'

,,- - Rhlthmic DeaeloPment 168


Ambiguous RhYthm 168
Rhythmic Vagueness 171

174
Rhythmic Transformation
Anacrustic DeveloPment 777

Exercises 182
DE,FINITIONS
AND
PRINCIPLE,S

To study rhythm is to study all of music. Rhythm both organizes, and is itself
organized by, all the elements which create and shape musical processes.
/, Just as a melody is more than simply a series of pitches, so rhythm is more than
a mere sequence of durational proportions. To experience rhythm is to group
separate sounds into structured patterns. Such grouping is the result of the inter-
action among thevarious aspects of the materials of music: pitch, intensity, timbre,
texture, and harmony-as well as duration.
It is the intimate and intricate interaction of temporal organization with all
the other shaping forces of music which makes the study of rhythm both a re-
warding task and, at times, a difficult and perplexing one. The task is rewarding
not only because the subject is itself intrinsically interesting but also because, by
adding a new dimension to our understanding of related fields such as melody,
harmony, counterpoint, and orchestration, it makes possible a more precise and
penetrating analysis of those processes.
The study of rhythm is rewarding in a practical way as well. An understand.
ing of rhythm is as important to the performer as it is to the composer and to the
theorist. Indeed, as will be apparent throughout this book, a considerable part
of what is usually called "interpretation" depends upon the performer's sensitiv-
ity to and awareness of rhythmic struiture.
Because the complex and delicate interaction among the elements of music
precludes the use of easy "rules of thumb" and pat, simplistic answers, the
analysis of rhythm tends to be complic3ted and, at times, uncertain. These
difficulties are in part responsible for the neglect which the field of rhythm has
suffered in recent writings on music theory.
fn part, however, the development of a fruitful approach to the study of
rhythm has been hampered by a failure to distinguish clearly among the several
aspects of temporal organization itself. The resulting confusion has created a
correlative ambiguity of terminology. Since clear distinctions and unequivocal
terminology are necessary if the analysis of the rhythmic structure of music is to
move beyond its present moribund state, our first task must be one of definition.

7
Defnitions and Prirrciples 3
2 The Rhythmic Structure of Music
in what follows may seem un- inferior metric level is organized in twos (3) and the superior metric level is
Some of the distinctions and definitions presented duple-that is, 2Xf.
with us, trusting that also
usual or contrary to current use. We ask the reader to bear
yield will justify
he will find that the insights which the distinctions ultimately Three basic modes of temporal organization can be differentiated. They are
the inconvenience of noveltY. pulse, meter, and rhythm. 'Iempo, though it qualifies and modifies these, is not
itself a mode of organization. Thus a rhythm or theme will be recognizably the
ARCHITECTONIC LEVELS same whether played faster or slower. And while changes in tempo will alter the
in its character of the music and perhaps influence our impression of what the basic
Most of the music with which we shall be concerned is architectonic
beat is (since the beat tends to be perceived as being moderate in speed), tempo
organization,Thatis,justaslettersarecombinedintowords,wordsintosen-
individual tones be- is not a relationship. It is not an organizing force.l
tences, sentences into iaragraphs, and so on' so in music
into periods' etc' This
come grouped into -oiirr.r, -otives into phrases, phrases
structure' It is
is a familiar concept in the analysis of harmonic and melodic PULSE
, equally important in the analysis of rhythm and meter' A pulse is one of a series of regularly recurring, precisely equivalent stimuli.
Asapieceofmusicunfolds,itsrhythmicstructureisperceivednotasaseries Like the ticks of a metronome or a watch, pulses mark off equal units in the
way
of discrete independent units strung together in a mechanical' additivewhile temporal continuum. Though generally established and supported by objective
motives'
like beads, but as an organic p.o".r* in wfrictr smaller rhythmic stimuli (sounds), the sense of pulse may exist subjectively. A sense of regular
function as integral parts of
possessing a shape and s"tructure of their own' also pulses, once established, tends to be continued in the mind and musculature of
42) for instance' the motive
a larger rhythmic organization. In Exam ple 53a \V' 'group' When this motive the listener, even though the sound has stopped. For instance, objective pulses
of the first measure forms a small, "pu"tt rhythmic may cease or may fail for a time to coincide with the previously established pulse
are perceived
is repeated in the second measure, the motive and its repetition r series. When this occurs, the human need for the security of an actual stimulus
They form a rhythm on a
as constituting a more extended rhythmic pattern. I or for simplicity of response generally makes such passages seem to point toward
higher architectonic level.
which the re-establishment of objective pulses or to a return to pulse coincidence.
The lowest level on which a complete rhythmic group is realized-upon
All pulses in a series are by definition exactly alike. However, preferring clear
astrongbeatandoneor,,'o,.*.ukbeut,aregroupedtogether-willbecalled
of the primary level in and definite patterns to such an unorganized and potentially infinite series, the
the priiar. rh2thmic leael. As is often the case, the rhythm
form a subsidiary' human mind tends to impose some sort of organization upon such equal pulses.2
' E*u-pt.-SZa is itself made up of smaller note values which
partial rhythmic motive. such partial patterns create what
will be called inJerior As we listen to the ticks of a clock or the clicks of a railroad car passing over the
level, tlne subprimaryt leael' When tracks, we tend to arrange the equal pulses into intelligible units of finite dura-
t rhythmic leuels or, where there is only one such
o' tn" primary rhythmic level are themselves organized into longer, tion or into even more obviously structured groups. Thus, although pulse can
;;;;;
' Iornporrnd patterns' superior rhythmic leuels ate created' theoretically exist without either meter or rhythm, the nature of the human mind
level will
In the analyses gi"." i" thiJ book the schematization of the primary is such that this is a rare occurrence in music.
of increasing length,
be indicated by al arabic "1." Superior levels, in order While pulse is seldom heard in a pure state (as a series of undifferentiated
,,2,,, ,,3,,, etc. Inferior rhythmic levels, in order of decreasing stimuli), this does not mean that it is not an important aspect of musical ex-
will be labeled
length, will be indicated by small roman numerals: "i"' "ii"'etc'
(see Examples
I It is important to recognize that tempo is a psychological fact as well as a physical one. Thus
23 and 50, PP' 23,40). eighth-notes in two pieces of music may move at the same absolute speed, but one of the pieces
meter differs from
Metric structure is similarly architectonic. For instance, a f may seem faster than the other. This is possible because the psychological tempo, which we shall
call "pace," depends upon how time is filled-upon how many patterns arise in a given span of
of a lower level $ meter'
a $ meter in that the former is made up of three units time. See, for instance, the increase in pace which takes place at measure 48 in the second move-
meter' And either a {
while the latter is made up of two units of a lower level $ ment of Mozart's Piano Concerto in D Minor (K. 466).
the same level to form
or a $ meter may itself be combined with metric units on r 2 That the mind tends to impose patterns upon even a random
series of stimuli has been clearly
demonstrated by experiments. See John Cohen, "Subjective Probability," Scicntifc American,
moreextensive,higher-levelmeters.ThusinExample53athemeterofthe XCCVII, No. 5 (November, 7957), 736.
in threes. The
primary level-thelevel on which beats are felt and counted-is
Defnitions and Priruiples 5
4 Ttw Rhltthmic Structure of Music
meter' but it generally, into larger metric units, some of which will be accented relative to others, it
I perience. Not only is pulse necessary,for the existence of follows that most compositions present a hierarchy of metric organizations. For
rhythmic experience'
i it orrgn ,rot ul*ays, underlies and reinforces instance,.the units of a f; meter might be divided or compounded as in Example
1. Needless to say, other combinations are possible on all architectonic levels.
METER
Meter is the measurement of the number of pulses between
more or less regu- ) )
some of the pulses
larly recurring accents.s Therefore, in order for meter to exist, ) ))))
in a series must be acpented-marked for consciousness-relative to others'a
Whenpulsesarethuscountedwithinametriccontext'theyarereferredtoas
beats.Beatswhich are accented are called "strong"i
those which are unaccented
))
LJ IJ
))
aaaa aaaaaa
l-1.,1 lJ
JT
-8-
aaaaaa
I

LIJ IIJ
rT 3

aaaaaa
!uL.l
t'weak." 88
are called
to establish the
While there can be no meter without an underlying pulse EXAMPLE 1

meter without any clearly


units of measurement, there can, as we shall see, be - only one metric organization, the one
We are inclined to think of there being
Jefinable rhythm (see pp' 7-8)' Conversely, th-9-19 can
be rhythm without
designated in the time signature and measured by the bar lines. This is because
meter-as in the ,,free" .hythm of some Oriental Jd iolk music and in what has
tonal harmony and homophony, with their emphasis upon vertical coincidence,
6".n .utt.a the "measured rhythm" of Gregorian chant'b
without destroy' and da4ce music, with its basic motor patterns, have for the past two hundred
Allh-oqg! meter tends 1o -pe-legular, irregularitle; may occur
such iiie-fularities years made for the dominance of what we have called the "primary metric
ing thg sense of -".rf";;t;;i"uiio1 Uruu'ny tftis is because
u

Often too]whit is irregular on


,-.i l.*porary.'frigfr.r one architectonic level becomes level." Until recently this primary level has dominated metric experience.
(or lower) one. Thus if a unit of three quarter-notes is fol- Changes on other metric levels which can be and are referred to the regularity
regular tn r
lowedbyaunitoftwoandthetempoisquitefast,lhe'oild,tendingtoperceive of the primary level are treated with an almost casual freedom.
the pattern
a pattern in the simplest, most regula. *uy possible, will _organize (measures But this has not always been the case. In the polyphonic music of the later
into a composite group of five quarter-notes, as in Act III' scene 2 Middle Ages and the Renaissanbe the relationships of the several metric levels
31 tr.) of Wagner,, T,i,ton und Isolde. This is also
the case with the hemiole both within each voice and between voices was a very important facet of style.
against two groups
rhythm in which the opposition of three groups of two played The organization of these metric levels was recognized by the theorists of the
state as a
of three (,tritr) is resolved after six beats. Indeed, one might time and was specified by the time signatures of the period. Such terms as
otganize itself-be tttempus," "prolatio,"
general law that'the dominant or primary meter will ,."q P "perfectus," and so forth indicated the organization on
the lowest architectonic level on which it exhibits
regularity' particular metric levels.
ierceived-on
Asnotedabove,meter'likeotheraspectsofmusicalorganization,isarchi- Of course some time signatures do indicate the organization of inferior metric
the meter designated
tectonic in nature. That is, since the beats which measure levels. Thus f implies-but only implies-that the subsidiary metric organization
units or compounded
in the time signature may the-relve, be divided into equal is to be in twos. One can easily move from this organization in twos,
the measurement of regu- J7 J7 n ,
a Although theorists, both Renaissance and modern, have referred.to it would seem that only
to the one in threes, ry
larly recurring u.".rr,r'*-,,rhythm,,, it is not so by our definition.
calling aspects of temporal
And
organization which measure' "rhyth- 47 Jl.1 , as Schubert, for instance, was so fond of
confusion has resulted from those doing. There are only-a few instances in the literature of music since 1600 in
mic." They are metric. which the composer has specified what the metric organization of higher archi-
a For further discussion of "accent," see pp' 11 ff'
tectonic levels is to be. The example which comes to mind most readily occurs in
6see willi Apel, Haruard Dictionar\ oJ Music (cambridge, Mass': Harvard university
Press'

1945), p. 640. the scherzo (measures 1s0-240) of Beethoven's Symphony No. 9, where the
6 often such irregularities are not apparent in the time signature
of the music' This is the case composer indicates that the higher rhythmic organization is to be in threes or in
,'rhythm," meter Lay ul be more regular than its
with the hemiole for instance. Conversely, ltt-t
Meyer, Emotion and Meaning fours by writing "ritmo di tre battute" or "ritmo di quattro battute" (see Ex-
ssg I eonard B.
notation would lead .rrio ."p..,. For such rr, .*urrrii.,
;, Uurll (Chicago: Universiiy of Chicago Press, 1956)' pp' 719-21' ample 95, p. 80).
Defnitions and Principles 7
6 The Rhlthmic Structure of Music
given abov., I J i u'a I :) J,, ,uth.. than as separate, ind.ependent
RHTTHM
patterns. In addition to limiting the number of classifiable patterns, this pro-
Rhythmmaybedefinedasthewayinwhichoneormoreunaccentedbeats
one. The five basic rhythmic groupings cedure has the advantage of employing a singre method for anaryzing group
are grouped in relation to an accented
with prosody: formation on all architectonic levels.
may be differentiat.J uy terms traditionally associated
a. lanb 'v -
The use of poetic feet to analyze rhythmic patterns is somewhat unusual.T
b. anaPestvu- Rhythmic groupings have generally been treated as if they were metric units.
i: llXtli"--.""w-v However, since these groups can be found in various different meters they are not
e. aurphibrach themselves the same as meters. An account of the theoretical basis for the view-
point advanced here is beyond the scope of this book.8 We can but ask the reader,
Since,asnotedabove,rhythmicorganizationisarchitectonic,moreextensive at least provisionally, to accept this viewpoint and hope that he will find it
rhythmic structures-phrure*, perioJs, etc.-as
well as shorter, more obviously
justified by the understanding which it yields.
t rhythmic motives exhibit these basic patterns'
senses. EIgt, rhythm can exist
Rhythm is independent of meter in two separate
without there being a regular meter, as it does
in the case of Gregorian chant or ACCENT
be grouped in relation to an
recitativo secco. rirat ii unaccented notes may Though the concept of accent is obviously of central importance in the theory
accented one without there being regularly
recurring accents measuring metric
and analysis of rhythm, an ultimate definition in terms of psychological causes
theoretically independent of
units of equal duration. Indeea, ,nyiftm is at least does not seem possible with our present knowledge. That is, one cannot at
rhythm is independent of
nulse. Seeond. and more i-po'iu"i for our purposes'
'Y----E present state unequivocally what makes one tone seem accented and another
meterinthesensethatu'yo.'.oftherhythmicgroupingsgivenabovecanoccur not. For while such factors as duration, intensity, melodic contour, regularity,
iambic grouping can occur
in any type of metric organization' Fo1 instance' an and so forth obviously play a part in creating an impression of accent, none of
vary within a given metric
in duple or triple meter."In other words, rhythm can them appears to be an g1v444ble and necessary concomitant of accent.e Accents
chapters ?*ply illustrate'
orgurri"utiorr, as the examples in the following -or may occur on short notes as well as long, on soft notes as well as loud, on lower
Though rhythm may vary independentty *it.., this does not mean that notes as well as higher ones, and irregularly as well as regularly. In short, since
,nytfr.rri, not influenc.a Uyifr. -"tri" organizatiol
uld, conversely, that meter
accent appears to be a product of a number of variables whose interaction is not
is not in a very important dtpt"dent upon rhythm' As we shall see' some precisely known, it must for our purposes remain a basic, axiomatic concept
"t"
rhythmicg,o,,pi'g^,aremoredifficutttorealizeinagivenmeterthanothers.on which is understandable as an experience but undefined in terms of causes.
accents generally coincide with metric
the other hand, prJ"ir.fy because rhythmic However, while we cannot stipulate precisely what makes a tone seem ac-
ones, it should u. .^piurized that the bar
lines, which serve to mark off metric
cented, we can define accent in terms of its operation within the musical context
is. Rhythmic groups are not
units, do not indicate *hut th" rhythmic organization t,
and point out many of its characteristics. fn order for a tone to appear accented
respecters of bar tirer. 1.hey crors the* mo-.e
ofte' than not; and one of the first it must be set off from other tones of the series in some way. If all notes are alike,
things that the reader m,,st lea,,' is that the
bar line will tell him little about
there will be no accents. At the same time, however, the accented tone must be
rhythmic grouPing. similar and near enough to other tones of the series that it can be related to
Sincearhythmicgroupcanbeapprehendedonlywhenitselementsaredis. these-that it does not become an isolated sound. In other words, accent is a
above, always involves an inter-
tinguished from one lrro,h.., rhythmf as defined 7As Apel (op. cit., p. 639) points out,
and either one or two un- "Itwould be a hopeless task to search for a definition of
relltionship between a single, accented (strong) beat .
fhythm which would prove acceptable even to a small minority of musicians and write rs on music.',
accented (weak) beats. Hlrrce neither a series
of undifferentiated strong beats t.s._..Y_.I"r, op. cit., pp. 83-93 and 102 ff.; andJames
weak beats -. I. Mursell, The psychology oJ Music (New
(- -, etc.), the so-called spondee foot, nor a series of undifferentiated York: W. W. Norton, 1937), chaps. iv and v.
rhythms
(--, etc.), the pyrrhic foot, can be true rhythms' They are incomplete
weak beats, such as
e While accents
may be distinguished according to whether they are produced by stress (dy-
and
i,=;r,. ,?'tr.). ijther possible combinations of strong groups
namic),_duration (agogic), or melodic change (tonic), their function in organizing rhythmic
groups does not depend upon their origin and we have therefore decided to tre;t them as a single
n ) .h o, J Jw J , wilt be analyzed as combinations of the basic
tv u vl l- -l
aspect of rhythmic experience. See Apel, op. cit., p. 6.
-
8 The Rh2thmic Structure of Music
Defnitions and Priruiples I
if there are unaccents (weak wilt be minimal. Its purpose, which is practical rather than theoretical, is to
i relational concept' There can be accents only
no such thing as a series ofaccents or a make the reader aware of a few of the basic concepts and procedures used in this
I beats) and vice versa. In this sense there is
alike, there is oniy 1 series of pulses' book and thus help him to understand what follows and aid him in his own
series of weak beats. If all stimuli are
which is markedJor conscious- analyses.
An accent, then, is a stimulus (in a series oi stimuti) Rhythmic grouping is a mental fact, not a physical one. There are no hard and
ness Lnsome way. It is set off from other
stimuli because of differences in duration'
are the unaccented beats thus dis- fast rules for calculating what in any particular instance the grouping is. Sensi-
, intensity, pitch, timbre, etc' But in a sense so
tinguished. The difference between accented
and unaccented beats lies in the tive, well-trained musicians may differ. Indeed, it is this that makes performance
the nucleus of the rhythm, aloun$ an art-that makes different phrasings and different interpretations of a piece of
fact that the accented beat is the focal point, music possible. Furthermore, grouping may at times be purposefully ambiguous
in relation to which they are heard'
which the unaccented beats ur. g.o.tp.i and and must be thus understood rather than forced into a clear decisive pattern.
group. may have several Y"u:-
This is clear when it is noticed that a rhythmic In brief, the interpretation of music-and this is what analysis should be-is an
the aclent (or both), but a rhythmic
cented beats, which may precede or follow art requiring experience, understanding, and sensitivity.
groupmayhavebutoneaccentonanygivenarchitectoniclevel.Furthermore,
as we shall see presently, u" t"'t""t"iei
btut may belong to more-1fn on'e Grouping on all architectonic levels is a product of similarity and difference,
proximity and separation of the sounds perceived by the senses and organized
rhythmicgroup'whiteanaccent,becauseitisthefocalpointaboutwhichweak by the mind. If tones are in no way differentiated from one another-whether in
one rhythmic group'
beats are arranged, generally belongs to only '
placement
. :-.
ln pitch, range, instrumentation and timbre, duration, intensity, texture, or dy-
differ from unaccented ones in that their
. Finally, accented Leats
specifically, in order to obtain the namics-then there can be no rhythm or grouping. For the mind will have no
the series of beats tends to be fixed and stable. basis for perceiving one tone as accented and others as unaccented. There will be
of,.., slightly displaces unaccented
desired impression of grouping, the perform.. uniform pulses, nothing more, unless of course the grouping is completely subjec.
are closer in time to the accents
beats in the temporui .orrli.rrr,rm so that they tive. On the other hand, if the successive stimuli are so different from one an-
had played them with rigid pre-
with which they are to be grouped than if he other or so separate in time or pitch that the mind cannot relate them to one
cision.(TherubatostyleofplayingwouldSeemtobeaninstanceofsuchdis. another, there will be no impression of rhythm. The stimuli will then be per-
So displaced'
placement.) But accented beats seem never to be ceived as separate, isolated tones.
In general, sounds or groups of sounds which are similar (in timbre, volume,
'szREs^t ,, --^^r i- +L,' etc.) and near to each other (in time, pitch, etc.) form strongly unified rhythmic
term "stress,t' as- used in this
Accent must not be confused with stress. The patterns. Difference and distance between sounds or groups of sounds tend to
book,meansthe_dynamicintensificationofabeat,whetheraccentedorun. separate rhythmic patterns. However, though similariry tends to create cohesion,
accented.Thusastress,nomatterhowforceful,placedonaweakbeatwillnot repetition usually makes for the separation of groups. The case is somewhat dif-
make that beat accented' ferent when a single pitch is repeated. For though such repetition may at first
beats, it may and often does
While stress does not change the function of I give rise to group cohesion, as it continues, change is expected. Consequently the
change tn. gro,',p1,tg oi tf" beits-the rhythm'
In general it appears that sffess' final note or notes of the series will seem like anacruses (upbeats) and will be-
the beginning of a group' Thus'
whether on a weak or strong beat, tends to mark come grouped with the new tone to which they move.
as we shall see, a norrnally end-accented
(iamb or anapest) temporal otganiza- Except on the lowest architectonic level, a grouping is seldom the result of the
tionmaybemadebeginning-accented(trocheeordactyl)byplacingastresson action of all the elements of music (duration, pitch, harmony, instrumentation,
21), while a normally beginning-
the accent (see Examples 19 and 20, pp. 20, etc.). Both within and between groups, some of the elements of music will tend to
end-accented one by placing a stress
accented group;; ;. converted into a., produce group coherence, others will tend to produce group separation. For
upon a *!uk b.ut (see Example 21, pp' 21)' instance, in measure 1 of Example 2 (p. 12) the grouping of beats 1 and 2 to-
I
i
gether and beats 3 and 4 together-the lowest architectonic level-is a result of
GR,UPING I the similarity and proximity of pitch, duration, and harmony. These two groups
'^L io in fqnr ronce. r grouping-for that is combine to form a group on the next architectonic level. This larger group is
Sincethisbookisinfactconcernedthroughoutwitl held together by temporal proximity and harmony, but it is separated into two
what rhythm is-the following discussion of
tle general principles of grouping
Defnitions and Principles 1/
10 The Rh2thmic Structure of Music
if they were on the seventh degree of the scale and moved to the tonic on the fol-
subgroupsbynon-proximityofpitchanddifierenceoftimbre,sincethelatter lowing beat-then they would be grouped with the tone to which they move. In
element tends to .n""st;ii tnly
slightly' with the change in pitch' this sense, the grouping depends upon whether tones are moving from a goal or
the several elements of music in which
some
This divisio., or rr_rrltio, u*orrg which at times make it are moving toward one. rn the second place, if the mind imposes an end-
produce unity and separati-on is one of the things accented grouping upon this durational disposition-if this is to be heard as an
";; grouping is' In aly case rs
difficult to know what the dominant 'particular tem- anapest-then the last two notes will be performed or imagined ever so slightly
segregation marked enough
to outweigl harmonic similarity and
melodic nearer to the accent with which they are to be grouped.l0 The notation remains
poralproximity?oo-i,,,t,.'*entaldiffer.,'"",dominategroupinginanother
it is partly the the same. What changes is the interpretation of the grouping. And this change
dominate? And so forth. Again,
,i*lu.ity
case, or does harmorrl" problem of the affects the placement of the beats and consequently the listener's impression of
can be established to solve this
fact that no hard uJ-tur, rules than a science' the grouping as well as the performer's expression of it. This is a crucial fact in
1'u'iuUl' thut -uttt' analysis an art rather
orecedence of t:: however' one funda- the analysis and performance of music and one to which we shall find ourselves
groupi"g
In this admittedli;fn";it problem oi -thtt". groups: Durational refening throughout this book.
izationor itutio.r;ips within
mental set of rules f..;;;;;g;" of some stimuli 10 For experimental confirmation, R. B. Stetson, "A Motor Theory of Rhythm and Discrete
result in the tempo'al proximity
see
differences, which t"tt"u'iiy group- succession," Pslchological Reuiew, xII
(1905), 250-70,293-350; and H. woodrow, ,,A
therefor. i" th; ;;ulu,io. of others, te.rd io produce end-accented tive Study of Rhythm," Archiues oJ Ps2chology, XIV (1909), 1-66.
euantita-
and to produce beginning-
ings (iambs ."d i*.rriry differentiation tends combination of
"";;;l;; u''i .au"tyts); and- the proper middle-accented
accented gro.rpi"gJ'6;t*
durational difference *ltt int..rrity
ain t'.".e tends to pioduce
groupings (amPhibrachs)' beginning
spaced beats and every third one,
Thus if we have a series of evenly
will be dactylic:
with the first, is accented, the rhythm
i tt ll I :
vvll-vvr

Nowifwekeepintensityconstantand'graduallylengthentherelativeduration follows
of the accented o.r;;;.kt"f
,i. *.ut beats cl,oser to the accent which end-
is reached, the grouping will become
them, until this ,.-p.r^r lis"position
accented, that is, anaPestic:

i+j1t=nil
I ,v v
-t
tuv
tr
-l

placement of the final tone so that


it is
However, if we change the temporal as an
follows it, the organization will
be perceived
closer to the accent^;ii;h
lrr[iiu.^"r, grouping, that is' as middle-accented:
olJ. JJr rL.l'lJ.J.l'lJ
i,lri: ' - -tt- I I'l
can be
as in the first case, the grouping
But even if there is durational equality,
heardasend-accented.Therearetwo,.^,o,,,forthis.Inthefirstplace,duration
isnottheonlyfactordetermininggrorrpirrg;anend-accentedgroupmighteasily
in pitch to-th3 beat which follows
beats were
arise if the second and third "tJr.t final beats
which precedesihem.,Or if the two
them than tt.y *"..1o in" U.u, instance'
beat in tot'al futtction and harmony-for
were separated from the first
Rlrythms on Lower Architectonic Leuels l J
which are not, this does not constitute an analysis of the rhythm. What is re-
RHYTHMS quired is an indication of how the beats are grouped. The grouping in this ex-
ample results from the proximity and similarity of the elements of sound. That
ON LOWE,R is, the two quarter-notes of each trochaic group are proximate in time and
identical in pitch, temporal placement, timbre, dynamics, and so forth.
ARCHITE,CTONIC Although the dominant organization of this tune is trochaic, it is important to
realizc that an end-accented, iambic grouping is not completely absent. It is
the latent organization and helps to connect and join the series of trochees to one
LEVE.LS another. Such latent organization will be indicated, as in this example, by the
use of inverted brackets.l

of relatively simple rhythms on the THE INFLUENCE OF DURATION


This chapter is concerned with the analysis
lowest architectoni" l;;;i. It is hoped
that after the reader has studied it with If one or more of these elements is changed, the grouping will tend to change
care,performingtt"-t"u*pft'u"ddoingtheexerciseqhewilthavelearned too. For instance, if the temporal placement is altered so that the first note of the
about (1) the nature and organ-
through example as well as prescript ro*.ihing group is lengthened, making the second note closer to the note which follows it,
izationofrhythmic*."p''czltheinfluenc.*of.hu''gesofpitch,duration,in- the grouping will become iambic (Example 3). This example illustrates several
(3) the relationship of
grouping-;
tensity, orchestratioi, urra- phrasing .upon
o'gani'ation; (4i the differences which may exist
rhythmic groups to metric
(5) the general merhods and procedures
within one type or rnytt -i. grlrp; and tv -t tv -t tv -r rV -llv -tlv
employed in the analysis of rhythm'
folk song in duple meter and illustrate EXAMPLE
We shall n.gin *ith fo"t "ttu"'res of a
3
many factors as possible are kept
by a proces, of ,i*i. .'u'iutio", in, which as points important to rhythmic grouping: (1) The grouping is most clearly iambic
groups and f; influence of changes
constant, both the ,:;;"i types of rhythmic where both temporal and melodic proximity exists between groups, as from
another folk tune in triple meter
in the elements of music on such groups. Later measure 2 on. (2) The iambic character of the rhythm is emphasized if an eighth-
willbeusedtoexemplifyotheraspectsofgroupingandoftherelationshipbe. note upbeet (either G or D) is placed (or imagined) before the first measure or
in-order to-show how differences in the
tween rhythm urra -it.r. Following this, when the tune is repeated with the upbeat D, as shown. This occurs because,
relationships of the elements of music ,o
ottt another within a single type of once a rhythm becomes established, it tends, if possible, to be continued in the
groupingmodifytheeffectandtendencyofparticular.rhythms,severalthemes mind of the listener-it tends, that is, to organize later patterns in its own image.
Finally, there will
having the same ru,;. ,r,ytr,mic groupi"g *irr
be analyzed.
This is true even where the natural grouping would seem to be otherwise. For
of rhythmic organization on melodic move-
be a brief discussion of the influence instance, if the reader begins Example 2 with an upbeat D, he will hear that the
ment. iambic organization seems to continue throughout the example. (3) Bar Iines
do not determine or delimit grouping. only beginning-accented groups on the
TROCHEE AND IAMB IN DUPLE METER lowest architectonic level stay within them.
Examples 2 and 3 demonstrate the ability of temporal proximity to influence
Thegroupingofthequarter-notesinthefolktunegiveninExample2is
should,note that while grouping. The more marked such proximity is, the more patent the grouping
The reader
trochaic, as is indicated by the lower brackets.
will be. And necessarily following from this, the more tones are separated, the
itisobviouslynecessarytodiscoverandindicatewhichbeatsareaccentedand greater the tendency for them to belong to different groups. For instance, if Ex-
ample 2 had a temporal organization throughout such ur
.|? t J'! I etc., its
1 However, when the
analysis is placed aboue the music in some later examples, the upper
brackets indicate the dominant organization.
EXAMPLE 2

12
Rh2thms on Lower Architectonic Leuels 75
The Rhlthmic Structure of
Music
14
THE INFLUENCE OF BEAT PLACEMENT
trochaicgroupingwouldbeevenmoremarked;orifExample3werewrittenas
more patent'
i;;i" organization would be even It is mostimportant to realize that the performer's understanding of a rhythm
'.1
;i'j:;:fil: t;-
influences his placement of the weak beats. In Bxample 4, for instance, the per-
former would unconsciously place beats 2 and 4 slightly closer to beats 3 and 1
THE INFLUENCE OF MELODY
(of the following measure) than he would in Example 2, where an apparently
Melodicchangescanalsoaltergrouping.Ifthetemporal-organizationiskept
details.of the melody rather similar set of temporal relationships is heard as trochaic. The rhythm of the tune
constant (is theru*.1, i., trr" original tun?) a.rdinthethe particular way shown in would be changed, even without melodic and durational alterations, were it
ur. -oaih.d
than its basic melodic structure heard as iambic. thit g.onping
occurs be- phrased as in Example 6. Here not only would the performer indicate the group-
4, the rhy;;-;;"ds to be
Example ing by a legato manner of playing, but he would also move the weak beats slight-
ly closer to the accents which follow and he might also alter duration by cutting
the length of the accented beats (as indicated by the commas), thus separating
Al----J
=-9-=-t,u -rrv -l ru -t Ul--:l tll----:J the groups. Observe that the particular effect of Example 6 is in part dependent
EXAMPLE 4
<
elements together-
equality' tle mind group: Pt:*iYtt
cause, given durational bar' and so <
the C with the B i";;;
i, tfr. D-shlrp witL the E across thetend to arise
rv -'rv -r gtc.
groups
the other way a'ou"d' separate
forth' Or, to put the matter C is separated from the G which
EXAMPLE 6

where there is
"tutJt "o"-proximity-ln"
from the B'' et3' upon the fact that, particularly in the first measure, an iambic grouping is im-
;;.*.t it, the D-sharP tt. articulation of this variant of the tune is the im- posed upon a melodic-temporal organization which would otherwise give rise to
Also of importanJio goal-its
plied harmony' Each leading
to"t t"ot"' to and is grouped with its a trochaic rhythm.
As indicated in Example 6, the performer will also articulate the desired group-
temporary tonlc' ^r^^r +lrp o"nrrninp, of the last two measures
the grouping' ing by making a slight crescendo on the upbeat. This is followed by a "piano"
But even without this leading-tone effect' "f *' 2' For
Ut if there is a cudJ""t o" ittt D in measure accent so that the next group can be performed the same way without creating
will tend to ""a-uttt"ita of first measure and because it resolves an an over-all crescendo. Such crescendos whether over one note or a group ofnotes
since this i, tt" tt*ittttt *"tf the follows must
appoggiatura (E), tttclol;l
t'"i tttt first larger group' The B which it tends
are an important way in which groupings can be made clear. They indicate the

tt"" g'o"p iiself with the C' Once this pattern is established' tendency, the leading toward a goal, of a tone or a group of tones. That is, the
of necessity
i*'rr'ififfi;^;;ilt;;'*'' ir the s is omitted so that the C crescendo creates an expectation that an accent will follow, and the tone bearing
to perpetua,' heard as trochaic
the.final'measures can be the crescendo is heard as leading toward, and grouping with, the expected accent.
is without ur, .ru..Jiri'.".;i..r, of the two-
this pattern partly because each Because the more a tone seems to be oriented toward a goal, the more it tends
(Example Sf)' fft"v it Ft'toftJl] to function as an anacrusis, rising melodic lines, particularly conjunct ones,
harmony'
,to,. grorrp* belongs to the same tend to become anacrustic. The energy and striving implicit in a rising line make
b.
a. each successive tone move touarl the one which follows it, rather than from the
EIII one preceding it. A rising melodic line feels very much like a crescendo. Indeed,
-l ls :l ,- ul r-vl L
E-9|-J r-.vr -, lv illg-:llv o, most people perceive it as such. This is shown not only by the tendency of per-
EXAMPLE 5 formers to crescendo in rising passages and of composers to indicate crescendos
over rising passages much more frequently than over descending ones, but also
Noticethatthefirstmeasurehasalsobeenchanged.Thisillustratesthefact by the fact that people actually hear higher pitches as louder, even though
thatpatternrepetitionleadstogroupseparation'Indeed'inthiscasebecause intensity remains constant. Conversely, descending melodic lines, or those in-
u,t o';;;;;"iui' tht disjunction is marked by the repeat- volving the repetition of one or two tones, tend generally to be heard with
both groups ou"*
ed B.
I

\ Rhytlms on Lower Architectonic Leuels 77


16 Ttu Rtrythmic Structure of Music
each tone moving from the
one
(trochees or amphibrachs), THE INFLUENCE OF ACCOMPANIMENT
feminine endings
Clearly harmony and whatever counterpoint or accompaniment figure is
before it.
present, may play a decisive role in articulating grouping. Thus after a somewhat
ambiguous initial measure the rhythm of Example 10 becomes iambic on the
THE INFLUENCE OF INSTRUMENTATION
in instru- primary level once the shaping forces of harmony and voice-leading are brought
emphasized' or altered-by changes
Groupings can also be shaped' original trochaic organization
of the into play in measure 2.
mentadon and timbie. r", i"r,^"".,'ttr. groups on different
emphasized by pliying 3lternaS # -7-?--Z----
tune (Exampf. Zl "o"falt u, irr" B*^-ple 7, which might be ffi

instruments in a ki.r; oirro"t t t..t.riq.r., a)

Oboe ----+
etc.
nu
---#-

EXAMPLE 7
EXAMPLE 10

by two violins'
But ev;1.if the parts were played
played by an oboe and a violin' the ARTICULATION AND STRUCTURE
putti"ttt*ty it' u live performance where
lhe groupirrgs would be apparent' accompanied by a different Although it would be quite easy io employ all the elements of music to empha-
the violins would be
difference in timbre u.,*.L., size a particular type of grouping and achieve a kind of unmitigated iambic,
play every other group
locationofthesoundSource.Asingleinstrurnent,too,mightarticulatethegroup- trochaic, or other grouping, this is in point of fact a rare occurrence in music
by differences i" ,ir"ur". For example, a violin might
ings literature. For if all the elements are used in such a single-minded manner on the
; g an i z e'|he
r;n :;J#'#il'::: #::
lowest architectonic level, the resulting patterns, though themselves impregnable,
i:T1?:'11"H'l' 1e used'fo or Example
g' (fnt crosses in 9 become so segregated from one another that there can be no higher levels of
iambic grouping as in Examplts I and
beats in an rhythmic organization. There will be no rhythms between groups within the
or muting on the trumpet')
represent pizzicatoon a viotin measure and none between measures, because no elements are left to form a

--+--1l-f--l-
-t-r--=-T basis for such larger groups. This is the case in Example 11, where the groups,

D- Antlarlte

--H'#r-
nlr P
ffi--F-, T-n + F- a)

Pizz,
EXAMPLE 8
---'ltr:-:)-----|---:i----t
Cello

+ +
---=----H
f
-YY-A etc. EXAMPLE 11

though clearly iambic, are almost completely isolated from one another and
EXAMPLE 9
hence there is no over-all rhythmic-melodic coherence.
Looking at these matters from another point of view, it should be observed that
Dynamicstoomightbeusedtoarticulatedifferenttypesofgroupings'Thusif
and 1 forte' etc'' there most groupings are only partially segregated from those which precede or foliow
played ft'it' f""t' 2 and' 3 piu"o' beats 4
beat 1 were if beats 1 and 2 were them. And when we speak about the organization of a particular group, we are
iambic rhythm to arise' While
would be a tendentr;;;;; be emphasized' referring to its dominant or manifest rhythm, but not to all the rhythmic rela-
played forte and u"l + piu"o' ihe trochaic rhythm would
Utut'l
Rhythms on Lluer Architectonic Leuels lg
18 Ttw Rlgtthmic Structure of Music
Of course, the performer, interpreting this as an amphibrach rhythm, will
tionshipsinwhichthegroupisinvolved.Thus,concurrentwiththemanifest automatically phrase in such a way that this grouping will be clear. He will, as
3-6, there exists a latent trochaic organization
iambic organizatio" l; i;;;ples indicated in Example 14, make minute temporal adjustments, bringingthe un-
which plays an important role in linking
(indicated by the i";;r;J brackets) accented beats closer to the accent and cutting some of the value of the afterbeat
theiambicgroupstooneanothera.,dc.euti.,ganover-allmelodic.rhythmic which by playing it staccato. This will create both coherence within groups and separa-
Datrern. And it is in part the
obliteration of this latent trochaic rhythm
tion between them. Particularly in cases such as this where the upbeat is closer
#ff;";';;;;;'iv ihuoti' atomization present in Example 11'
Boththecomposerandtheperformermustthereforebewareofoverarticulat. + etc.
ingthelowerarchitectoniclevelsattheexpenseofthehigherones'oneshould the musical in- \JttV
as much u, i, ,r.".*rury to make
lV -
articulate the smalter units onty music
while it will change ih. chu.ucter of the EXAMPLE 13
tention clear. Addedarticulation, --+ >
(andthisirof"o.,,"importantinone'sinterpretation)'maywellweakenthe of the
appreciably sirengthening the grouping # etc.
phrasing of larger p*ts without
smaller ones EXAMPLE
-) 14

DACTTL IN DUPLE METER in pitch to the accent than the afterbeat is, the performer will, by slightly stressing
AMPHIBRACH, ANAPEST, AND
the accent, tie the afterbeat to the accent. And finally he will probably group the
Becausethefolktunebeingusedisorganizedintotwo.unitgroups(trochees accent and the beat which follows it by playing them legato.
rhythms (anapests, dactyls, or amphi-
or iambs), it cannot give rise to three-unii employed
temporal alterations than have been
brachs) without -or! rubstuntial
to be three-unit groups in f meter, two
heretofore. That is, in order for there
THE INFLUENCE OF HARMONY
by a single half-note' thus:
of the quarter-nott' "ttt"be relllcel Example 15, an anapestic rhythm, is of particular interest because it illustrates
) ) a, )""')'l)'' the influence of rhythmic grouping on the melodic-harmonic organization of
Z'al.''i-'-
'a vt ruv-l
music. Observe, first of all, that one does not feel relaxed and easy when the
tune is performed in this way. The tension present in performing Example 15
THE INFLUENCE OF MELODY
grouping' The upbeat is necessary'
Example 12 is an instance of amphibrach with
since without it the ;;ffi;; *o.rld probabl| be interpreted as anapestic' -rtV U -ilV V -l tvv
once
because' as lr-as been mentioned'
it, the grouping i, ;;;i;;"Jn 'ntot'grtout' EXAMPLE 15
arhythmisestablished,ittendstoperpetuateitself.However'despitetheup. of
is rather unstable. For the final note
beat, rhe initial u,,'p-iii*.f, groupi.rg group itself
oi similarity and proximity, to
the rhythm (the D) ;;;uiu.r* l-v u --J
with the G with which it properly belongs
lvv - | l(/ v - |

with the D which rorr,o*, .utt er than EXAMPLE 16


lut from which it is separated in pitch'
+ is quite apparent if we compare it with Example 16, which uses the same pitches
------# and the same grouping but in a different order. Example 15 seems relatively
rV Vtrv - I rv "l
- strained (and of course this may be precisely what the composer desires) because
EXAMPLE 12
it emphasizes a melodic-harmonic pattern which is at odds with the simple tonic
character of the tune. That is, the rhythm emphasizes the subdominant side of
Theimpressionofamphibrachgroupingisthereforeappreciablystrengthened
from D to B' Not only is the after- G major by making the tones E, C, and A structural points in the melody. Ex-
if, as in Example t:'ir'Jurtt'btuiis tr'ut'"gta segregated ample 16, on the other hand, seems relaxed and natural because its rhythm sup-
the *tJotd group is more clearly
beat now closer to the accent' but ports the tonic character of the tune, emphasizing the tones D, B, and G.
from the first.
Rhythms on Lower Architectonic Leuels 2l
20 The Rhltthmic Structure of Music
the tune would for melodic reasons normally be interpreted as iambic, the group-
Noticethatthoughtheoriginalbarringofthetunehasbeenmaintainedin ing can be changed from end-accented to beginning-accented if a stress is placed
melodic emphasis
ir, ,rri t - and thJ resulting change in
Example 16, the on the accented beats.
"nu.g;
have created u.nung;i" "ttt'it organization'
The opening tones should now be
written as upbeats to the D, as in Example 17'
,,f d
etc.
.tf
"f
'v v -r etc.
EXAMPLE 20
EXAMPLE 17

Conversely, in order to achieve the iambic grouping indicated in Example 6,


----a--?-' )---1j- the reader probably found himself stressing the weak beats which mark the be-
v vl v vt ginning of each group. In fact, were the tune written with stresses, as shown in
v vr
Example 27, the iambic rhythm would probably be clear without the detailed
EXAMPLE 18
expression marks indicated in Example 6.

Actuallythetemporal-melodicarrangementgiveninExamplel5canbemade
changing the rhythmi-c
to seem relaxed and natural harmonically silply by 18' Now the half- .tf
grouping from an anapest to a dactyl, u"ho*oin Example
which other notes move' but as ap- EXAMPLE
notes are heard not as structural points toward
21

tend toward structural tones, and thus the funda-


;;;;;,".^, which themselves Observe that in each of these instances the reversal or inversion of the group-
ItJrituffy triadic character of the folk tune is maintained' ing gives a special flavor or character to the tune. It feels somewhat tense and
constrained because without the stress the melodic-temporal organization would
THE INFLUENCE OF STRESS give rise to a different, more natural, rhythmic pattern. The stress is, so to speak,
does not change the function an external force imposed upon the pattern, changing its normal behavior.
It was pointed out in chapter i that while a stress
ofabeatwithinthepulsecontinuum-anunaccentedbeatremainsunaccented
on accented or unaccented
even if stressed witt a sforzando-stress, whether THE INFLUENCE OF ORNAMENTATION
the grouping of the unaccented beats. More
specifically,
beats, generally Thus far stress has been considered as a product of dynamic intensification.
"t,u,,g., group'2
a stress'tends to indicate the beginning of a
But there is another way in which a beat may receive stress, namely, through
.---l---1---t)- etc.
ornamentation. Indeed, in many cases ornamentation seems to function like
stress in the articulation of rhythmic groupings.
,rtf
"f
"a----:-:t ua----:---y) Ornaments or embellishments act to emphasize tones in several ways. First,
EXAMPT.E 19 if an accented note is a non-chord tone, as in the case of the appoggiatura, its
tendency to move to a consonance makes it feel "heavy" or stressed. This is the
Indeed,thereadermayhavenoticedthatinperformingExamplelB]re case with the E in Example 12 and the first C-sharp in Example 31. It is inter-
stressedthehalf.notes.Withoutstressestomarkthebeginningofgroups,du- esting to note in the latter example that the appoggiatura effect continues even
tend to produce end-accented
rational differences rrr"h u' are present here would where, as in measure 4, the "appoggiatura" eJ actually a chord tone. Indeed the
the desired dactylic grouping
rhythms (see pp. f O-iil. it ,r, or,. *ay of indicating appoggiatura pattern as a melodic-rhythmic phenomenon is so firmly estab-
sforzando, as in
to the performer *orrid be to mark the accented half-notes lished in this style that it may arise in the absence of dissonance simply because a
Example 19. note has been preceded by an anticipation-because it sounds like a prepared
SimilarlyinExample20,whichresemblesExample4'thoughtherhythmof dissonance-as in Example 300.
,,/,, be used to indicate stress. Thus
/ indicates a stressed accent and i indi- A grace note may stress a substantive note because it approaches the substan-
z The sign will
cates a stressed weak beat.
22 The Rhltthmic Structure of Music Rh2thms 0n Lower Architectonic Leuels 2J
tive note by a skip-landing more heavily upon it than would a conjunct grace upon the weak beats and establish a relationship between them,, creating a
note. This is the case in Example 22. Note that here too the embellishments, if trochaic subgroup which is anacrustic to the main accent, thus: lA )
played in strict time, tend to delay the arrival of the substantive tone, making The articulation of the grouping in such a composite upbeat ffi
it ,..- stressed when it is actually presented. It should also be observed that empha-
sized by giving the anacrusis a distinctive melodic-temporal shape-a rhythmic
organization of its own-as in Example 23a and, D. Indeed, in Example izt tt.
--*# anacrusis is even further subdivided.
EXAMPLE 22 rc-----=-l
b r=-il
embellishments perform a melodic function-articulating the direction of etc.
melodic expectation. Thus in Example 22 the structural gaps created by the tvl rr.l-wtl-vvr
r V V
grace notes emphasize the tendencyof the melody to descend' Lastly, embellish- -r, i. etc

ments may serve to mark off groupings. They often have an articulatory function'
1.

EXAMPLE 23
This is clear in Example 22 (see also Examples 566, 63, 65' 69, 98) '
These changes not only modify the character of the rhythm within the upbeat
COMPOSITE SUBGROUPS
group itself but they also influence the quality of the downbeat D, which now
appears more strongly emphasized than before. This occurs because the more
Bxamples 15-19 were designated as instances of anapestic and dacytlic marked the temporal differentiation is-the shorter the note values of the upbeat
rhythms. But they are so only in a qualified sense. The mind tries to group relative to the downbeat-the stronger the stress on the accented note. Notice
stimuli in the simplest possible way and tends whenever possible to equalize the also that in Example 23b the final two sixteenth-notes are both part of the upbeat
the
accented and unaccented parts of a rhythm. Because of this tendency toward group and form a sort of extra anacrusis to the D. They are, so to speak, an up-
equalization of elements in a group, which we shall call the Principle of Metric beat within an upbeat and, as we shall presentry see, they serve in addition as a
Equivalence, the groups, in Example 15 might also be analyzed as iambs with rhythmic pivot welding the anacrustic group to the accent.
ltll
divided ,rpb.atr-,!. J, I /,-rather than as anapests. Similarly the SrouPs, in The force of the accent can be still further intensified if the anacrusis is incom-
Example ta *iglrtH-co*idered trochees with divided afterbeats- I
) ,1,1,- plete, as in Example 24. Since the chief accent of the anacrusis is missing, there is
rather than as dactYls.
- u 'l no stable trochaic organization as in Example 23. The group is incomplete and.
hence rushes toward the downbeat which serves as the god of the upbeat group.
Thus in analyzing rhythmic organization, the most fundamental and general
classification of structures would be that of end-, beginning-, and middle-ac-
cented.. Changes within these types, say from iamb to anapest' are as a
rule much
less striking than are changes between them as long as meter is constant' How- t-----JJ

ever, neither changes within such classifications nor the special organization of a ' v -retC
given grouping should be slighted. Indeed, what for the musician is most im- EXAMPLE 24

fo.tu.rt of * is precisely the unique interrelationship of melody, temporal organ- Through the intensification of the anacrustic function of the upbeat, the accent
^ization,
stress, and other factors which give a particular rhythm its peculiar becomes particularly marked because durational differences are strongly empha-
character and mode of progression. sized by the brevity of the upbeat relative to the accent.
Ffow an upbeat is comprehended-whether as a single unit or as subdivided
and articulated-depends upon tempo as well as upon the particular organiza- DACTTL, ANAPEST, AND AMPHIBRACH
tion of the musical materials. For instance, if Example 17 is played at a rapid IN TRIPLE METER
tempo, then the weak beats will tend to be heard as a pair of upbeats to a single Looking back over the changes to which an innocent tune has been subjected,
downbeat-that is, the whole group will be perceived as an anapest. If' on the the reader will observe that every type of rhythm is possible in duple meter.
other hand, the tempo is very slow, the mind will tend to impose an organization However, not all are achieved with equal ease. Because of the operation of the
24 The Rhythmic Strutture of Music
Rlrytltms on Lower Architectonic Leuels 25
unequivocal anapests rc 2 and from measure 2 to 3. The grouping becomes ambiguous and might
Principle of Metric Equivalence, it was difficult to create
a three-unit be middle-accented (as in a), or end-accented (as in D), as well as beginning-
and dactyls in duple Leter. Furthermore, we noted that because
and such dif- accented (as in Example 25). The interpretation of the performer wouldhere 6e
group in duple time necessarily requires durational differentiation
the deciding factor.
ferencestendtoproduce.,,d-u"...'t.dgroups,dactylicgroupingscouldarise
To study these But even without this modification, the tune can be heard as a series of ana-
only if the accenied beat was strongly stressed (Example 19)'
well to explore still pests. If it is heard in this way, the reader will probably find himself placing some
rhythms in what might be called their "natural habitat" as
as

intricacies and subtleties of grouping, we turn now to a


tune in triple stress on the second beat of the measure and making a crescendo to the accent,
further the
since such stress will articulate the grouping and the crescendo will indicate the
meter.
goal of the group. Had the composer desired this rhythm he might have written
the tune with a sforzando on the first of these notes and a crescendo. Of course an
THE INFLUENCE OF MELODY' DURATION' AND STRESS
anapestic grouping could be achieved in other ways as well-by assigning each
in Example 25'
The naturalness of a dactyl grouping in triple meter is apparent group to a different instrument, by providing an accompaniment figure which
is a result of several aspects of
The beginning-accented grorrf,i.t*.of tftis example had such a rhythm, by playing one group forte and another piano, and so forth.
(1) The note foilowing the accent belongs to the same harmony
afr.
".gl"iratiln: (2) The falling inflection is
It is again important to observe that, while such a grouping is possible, the
as the accent and tends therefore to group with it. particular character of the rhythm is a result of the fact that the anapest foot is,
so to speak, imposed upon a melodic organization which is more naturally
dactylic' The strain in the grouping is particularly noticeable between measures
vl l-v vl
t---14 r-v 2 and 3, which we have already discussed, and between measures 3 and.4, where
EXAMPLE 25 the marked pitch segregation tends to make for rhythmic separation. Thus the
on the first beat of each character of the anapestic grouping can be made to seem more natural and re-
heard as strong-weak. (3) The basic melodic line comes laxed by changing the melodic motion between measures 1 and 2, so that the
figure,
measure-D-B-A-B-and the weak beats are heard almost as a separate following measure proceeds in a like manner, and by changing the melodic
,,umpah,,, beat accompaniment. Hence the G is not heard as moving
a kind of progression from measure 3 to 4 so that proximate pitch progression makes
to the A, but as hanging from the B' grouping more relax€d. This is done in Example 27 . The anapest effect will be
is the melodic
Another i-portani fu""to, in articulating the groups as dactyls
though there is less pitch segregation
disjunction beiw.e' measures 2 and 3. For -t----1--)-
of direction is neverthe-
between measures 2 and 3 than within them, this change
separation. That is, be- v -F---]-
Iess a point of melodic, and consequently of rhythmic,
rV-t tV -rrv v

measure 1 to measure 2 is descending, the turning back to


ih. motion from EXAMPLE 27
"urrr.
further strengthened if the initial foot is an anapest-if two quarter-notes, G,
+ are imagined as preceding the opening D.
a _=) tv-vl19-VllV Although no special articulation (sforzando, instrumental change, etc.) is now
r v -llv --!1,-----3 tu
u -r ll/__y:_-
b. --=J required to project the desired grouping, the performer will, as we have seen be-
EXAMPLE 26 fore, make the anapest clear in his performance. He will tend to place the up-
Furthermore, because the beats closer to the accent than he would in articulating a dactylic grouping,
the A is heard as a break in the melodic continuum. thus making the separation between groups more apparent, and he will-very
the anacrustic function of the
motion from measure 1 to measure 2 is descending, likely make a slight crescendo on the weak beats so that their relation to the
be the case'
final eighth-notes of measure 1 is not as forceful as might otherwise accent which follows is unmistakable.
Theimportanceofthemelodicstructureofthefirstmeasurebecomesap- rhythmic groups of this tune can also be heard as amphibrachs (Example
motion from
parent if ii is rewritten as in Example 26. Now there is a rising ^ _Th.
2B). In this case, however, there is a tendency to stress the second tone of the
tends to order subse-
,.r.ur,rr. 1 to 2 and the anacrustic function of the eighth-notes group, the accent, rather than the initial upbeat. This is done because,
motion from measure 1
quent patterns because of the parallelism between the while the
Rhythms on Lower Architectonic Leuels 27
26 The Rhltthmic Structure of Music
ample 28, for instance, tends to be thumping and somewhat ponderous, like a
melodic-temporalproximityoftheupbeattotheaccentmakesthispartofthe peasant dance, because, if the amphibrach rhythm is to predominate, the ac-
were it not for the stress' for the non-
grouping clear, there wot'li be a tendency' cents must be heavily stressed. Example 300 seems, by comparison, light and al-
proximateafterbeatstog,o,,pthemselves*i.r'tr'.nextupbeattowhichtheyare most lilting, for the groupings are not strained and require no special stress for
closer in pitch' their articulation. On the other hand, the brevity of the anacrusis relative to the
remainder of the group makes Example 30a seem uneven and rather coarse. The
ul
simplicity and regularity of the melodic-harmonic structure of this tune calls for
,v -
an even beat, and the abbreviated anacrusis seems both contrived, overarticulat-
EXAMPLE 28 ing the pattern, and hurried, because it arrives too late. Also contributing to the
rather blatant character of this organization is the undue emphasis which the
Inothersituationswheretheproximityoftheafterbeatmakesitsrelationship accent receives as a result of the shortened upbeat. The passage is obvious, almost
totheaccentclear,butinwhichthefunctionoftheupbeatmightbeindoubt, vulgar.
the stress is placed on the beginning
tf t-lt group' Thisis. wh1 takes place in the By way of illustrating how very different the effects arising within one type of
of H;;;,, Sy*prr"o'y No."to+l"
D Major (Example 29). Here,
third movement rhythmic grouping can be, let us examine another of Haydn's themes, one from
Allegro the Finale of the "Oxford" Symphony in G Major (Example 31). This example
'---* Presto
(s)
J

EXAMPLE 29 p
t\_-/ - vt t \-/
EXAMPLE 31
wereitnotforthesforzandosplacedontheupbeats,thegroupingsmight,inspite
oftheinitialanacrusis,havebeenheardasdactyls.. is somewhat more complex than those analyzed heretofore. On the lower archi-
Withoutphraseordynamicmarkings"o*"sicit"wouldplayBxample25as tectonic levels the weak beats, functioning as pivots (as both afterbeats and up-
anamphibrach,foritsmelodicshapemakesdactylicrhythmalmostanecessity. beats), so fuse the first five eighth-notes that decisive groupings cannot be made
be'apprehenrled as amphibrachs'
only if the tune is modified will iis rhythmsupbeai of each group closer in time without doing violence to the pattern. This is true of the quarter-note motion
the
This is done in n*u*ft" 30aby making (Example 32, analysis D) as well as of the eighth-note motion (Example 32,
b. analysis a). Because these eighth-notes are apprehended as a cohesive group, the
a,
manifest rhythm is that of the whole motive and, as indicated in Example 31, the
tv - vl rv - w 1
grouping of this rhythm is an amphibrach.
UrrU vl
-

EXAMPLE 30

the end of one group from the beginning


to its downbeat, and this also separates
groups ut:::u^tY:ully because the up-
a.
of the next' In P*"-pit"ma u*pftibtuth the b.
beats are now closer in pitch to
the do#nbeats which follow and because EXAMPLE 32
is made clear. obviously these
parallelism between ,t.'nr" two amphibrachs one another'
be combined' reinforcing The witty and somewhat impertinent-almost brusque-character of this
modes of articulatio"
"o"ta theme is in large part a'result of the durational disproportion among the units of
RHTTHMS the main rhythmic group. The extended anacrusis leads one to expect a longer
THE CHARACTER OF PARTICULAR final unit. Not only is the accent too short, in relation to the upbeat group, but
28' 30a' and 300 one becomes aware
As one sings or plays over Examples which can
of progression
the weak afterbeat is even shorter. The second motive seems to begin too soon,
again of the diversitflrrt yrt rrric character and mode interrupting, as it were, the natural duration of the initial motive. Thus each
arisewithinthesamegroupingandeventhesamebasicmusicalstructure.Ex-
Rlrytlms on Lower Architectonic Leuels 29
28 The Rtrythmic Structure of Music
IAMB AND TROCHEE IN TRIPLE METER
groupembodiesinitselfakindofcomposedaccelerando,eachunitofthegroup.
this quickening of the pace
one which pr.J., it. And
t;;;h" THE INFLUENCE OF MELODY, DURATION, AND sTREss
ing being shorter
iscarriedthroughintothelasttwomeasures'wheretheamphibrachgrouptngs Let us now return to our variations on the tune ,,Ach du lieber Augustin."
come twice as fast' An iambic version can be created simply by making temporal differentiation
ExamPle 31 with the tune, "Twinkle,
If one compares the theme given in melodic such that the upbeat is clearly proximate to the accent (Example 34a). once
that both have the same fundamental
Twinkle, Little Star, " one finds 33, in which
measures. This is shown in Example
a. b.
motion' save in the final two (with an anacrusis added) for
an amphibrach rhYthm
the folk tune is written rn between the effects of the two tunes
vast difference
the sake of comParison' The b
t\',-trw-rtV-rtw r v - tetc,
a. EXAMPLB 34

again, however, the large skip between measures 3 and,4 makes the end-accented
b grouping seem rather strained. The rhythmic pattern is more relaxed if the
* melodic and harmonic groupings are congruent, as in Example 34b. Note, too,
that the end-accented impression is improved if the tune is imagined with an
etc upbeat G in front of the first measure.
EXAMPLE 33
The reader will remember that it was difficult to achieve a dactylic grouping
in f, time because here a three-unit group requires temporal differentiatio.r-anJ
arisesfromtheasymmetricalproportionsoftheHaydncomparedtothesymmet- hence tends to be end-accented. A similar situation obtains with regard to the
ricalproportionsoftt"fofftt""t'u'*af^i-"'thevital*tlodi"differences'for
2 of the Haydn' trochee in triple meter. When a triple meter is organized into two-unit groups,
example, the appoggiatura
in measure
and thus con- there will necessarily be temporal differentiation and the resulting rhythm wili
the folk tune reach a semicadence
Notice that in o.ai, to make
has been rewritten'
form to the motion oi ,rr.Haydn trr.*., it. final measure tend to be end-accented, as in Example 34. However, as in the case or ihe dactyl
in duple meter, this naturally end-accented organi zatjon can be made beginning-
AswiththeHaydn,thisfinalmeasure,'o*,..-,fasterthantheearlieronesbe.
To put the matter somewhat if the accented note is sufficiently
cause more takes place in the ru*. u*o.rr;;;i*.. accented stressed (Example 35).
lifeless regularity of
composei^,rt", ra avoids the
differently, Haydn,s tune is so if t*::-* a cadence on the
motion and the letdown of expectatit" in;l-""tttt =-----\-
of the Haydn been written as
in Example ----#
tonic. And had the ;;;i;;t 'ittu""t' (- w) r- w I

%'ffi LTi,Tl'JJ'":l'';'n::ii:;::rastanarvseshaveo*"i"li:1:11
ze two considerations. First, the primary
focus of attentron
EXAMPLE 35

to emphasi *."Er^r otganization being


shoutd be upon ,nJj"ai"ia"ality of ,t.'f.riJ"iar rtr'ythmicmeans toward more INVERTED GROUPS
presented
?" their
analyzed'.fnt tntotie' u"d "tu"ifications for
? own *kt:::1:: There is, however, another way in which a trochaic group can be found in
musical understanding, not ."ar'f"m"ed triple meter-namely, through what we shall call "group inversion,,, where the
sensitive by a comparrson
seen' be made more apparent accented beat is shorter than the unaccented one (Example 36). Such rhythms
while differ.,"t* -ui,";;;;; are sometimes referred to as ',Scotch snaps.,,
oftuneswhichareinsomerespectssimilar,thedifferencebetweenrhythmsis
muchmoreimportantthantheirsimilari.,-.r..fact,forinstance'thatbothare in temporal a.
amphibrach rhythms. Second,
tfr. gr..t i;pori^.r".oi small changes {i-.\
b

,t o,rfJ *uke boih tt. p.rfo.ri.. utd the critic acutely conscious of ----# #
relationship,
thedelicatedecisionsthatmustbemadeintheinterpretationandanalysisof sensitivity' ex- ,- ,i ,otc. r-vt l-vl
often it ;;ty subtle atJ requites
music. Grouping, or phrasing' EXAMPLE 36

perience, and understanding'


30 The Rhythmic Structure of Music Rlytthms on Lnwer Architectonic
Leaels Jl
Here the longer weak beat acts to complete the group' And
if the unaccented come anacruses. This tendency would have been considerabh
temporal separation makel
beat is followed by a rest, as in Bxamptez6t and c, the
one tends to place added
the sense of harmonic chu'g., which
*:l*:* ;;;;t;:::e.::'ff.1*:1
the groupings even moreincisive. Noiice that once again such patterns are particularly common-in*Bu.oq.r. ;";;'
where, apparentlv
stress upon the beginning of the group, the accent,
in order to make the rhythm because of their precision, they are used frequentty,
in relation to the under- bott *.r,r..,
as clear as possible. ArrJa, the accent becomes shorter form of ornaments such as the Schteifer and the mordent. "J,;i-i;',;:
the stress placed
Iying pulse, segregation of groups becomes more marked and The inverted temporal rerationships present in Example
l
I

36 might be stilr fur_


upon the accent heavier. ther varied by placing a stress followld by a crescendo
l

normal trochee' does on the


,
The weak beat of an inverted trochee, unlike that of a thus creating'an inverted iamb (Example 3B).
As b"fr; G;;xld;:T#n
of a trochee and
not tend to become a pivot, functioning first as the afterbeat melodic line of the first measure has beln varied
to emphasize the end_accented
is.partly because of its
then as the upbeat of an iamb or an amphibrach. This grouping. However, because the rather gauche,
lopsided humor of the unstable
separation from
relative proximity to the preceding u"".rri (and its concomitant iamb was what was desired, the final *Jur.r.., have
not been modified (as they
emphasized by
it. foUo*i.rg beat) and partly beiarrse it is both stabilized andwell as to the fact
were in Example 34b) to make them more naturally
iambic.
its relative terrgtlr. Furthermore, owing to this emphasis as
though it ends the
that it is not part of a cadential progression, the weak beat,
are thus peculiarly
group, doer nlt feel like a feminine ending. Inverted trochees
cohesive and' as a result, it is the total group rather
than any one of its com- I V
-"
U

clear if we compare
ponents which acts as the unit of motion. This becomes very
EXAMPLE 38
in B-flat
th. op..ri.rg of the third movement of Beethoven's Piano concerto a,
>
b. c.
the weak beat is
with its inverted trochees (Example 37a) to a variant in which #
shorter than the accent (i*u*pi. 37 b). ln the former
the groups are clearly --a---f----f-
groups are blurred,
defined and precisely articulat.d, *hil. in the latter
the r- vl
t- vt

flowing into one another EXAMPLE 39


Molto Allegro b
a. l It is more difficurt to invert a dactyr,
a three-unit group, in duple meter be_
cause the desire for metric equivalenc! iends
to make"the teat which is divided
(and one of them must necessarily be
,-6,,-6, so).seem fiil;G;;up of a rarger two-
unit rhythm. That is, the whole group will appear
c. d e. to be a ?ro"rr.. with a divided
first unit rather than a genuine thr.e_.r.rit aactyf
- etc
etc.
only by forcing the mind to perceive the g.oup 1e*u_pi"-alo;.
u, u'J.".-.,nit one will we
etc. find an unequivocar inverted dactyr arising in
t- Vt r- Vr ,- L,,t=--i:!r- vr--14 etc'
r=g_L+g_E#r etc Jupre meter. As shown in Example
390, this can be accomprished through syncopation.
EXAMPLE 37 However, even with this
temporal organization a dactylic rhythm wilr
arise onry if the first note of the
the accented
Related to the inverted trochee is the "closed" trochee in which group receives the stress necessary to tie the
following u.ut" to ir. For if the initial
is made up of
part of the group, though equal in duration to the weak part, beat is not thus stressed, the finar note of the
*"ur.rr.'r.rrJ, i"-rpri, off and become
grouped with the folrowing accent, forming
several short notes- ffi Because it is composed of short notes moving to the a partially i.rrr.r,.a amphibrach
L- _-l/J (Example 39c).
the weak beat' And
weak beat, th. u"".nt-il-i*de proximate to and emphasizes The dactylic feering of Example 39a can be intensified
by subdividing the first
unit of motion' Thus
the closed group, rather than any part of it, constitutes the two beats of the group, making them into more
crearly sepa.ute entities (Ex-
from the "Laudamus
while the trochaic grouping is very clear in the fragment ample 40a). But even here it is possible to perceive
the rhythm as trochaic. How_
variants (d
Te,,of Bach,s Mass in g Niirro. (d*ampte 37c),itis less clear in the ever, if the tune is written in tripre meter (Exampre
ioa;-,rr"n the inverted
and, e) in which the weak beats have an "on-going"
motion-a tendency to be- dactyl is more patent.
Rlrythms on Lower Architectonic
32 The Rttltthmic Structure of Music Leuels JJ
Example3gcwassaidtobe..partiallyinverted',becauseonlythesecondand "Ach du lieber Augustin," the melodic uniformity of the second and third
In a fully inverted measures makes it possible to interpret the groups as amphibrachs, dactyls,
third units of the group are in reverse order-short-long' and the downbeat or
perhaps even anapests. Thus if the rhythrn is thought of as being dactylic, the per_
amphibrach the upbeaiwould be longer
than the downbeat
40c)' former will unconsciously play the tune in such a way as to mike this interpreta_
*hort., than the afterbeat (Example
rise to an. inverted anapest (Ex- tion clear, articulating the groups by means of stress, temporal displacement,
A properly a""ir.l 'y""oputio., will also give
to be perceived as an anapest' the dynamics, and the like. But if the rhythm is thought of as being amphilrach,
ample 41a). Howe'r.., i" o'de' fo' this variairt per_
formance will make this grouping the manifest one.
b. c
a,
H (s)

'- v
671 -
vr eto.
Ot a,t V - vttv - vilv lv
Of ,- Vr etc, w t r-v vr t-
-j
v vtt_*J_
b.t-
EXAMPLE 40
Q0)
b. Andante
a. Vivace

EXAMPLE 42
'rJ u -r etc tv,
The equivocal grouping on the primary level is partly a result of
the character
EXAMPLE 41 of the melodic line. specifically, the focal tone of the third measure
might be G,
because with the first A an appoggiatura and the second A a passing-note to be B.
must be rather fast' otherwise' In this
beat must be felt in twos and the tempo case the rhythm would probably be amphibrach, a, indiclted
simple as possible, a subgroup will arise in analysis a. or
of the desire to make the organization as the focal tone might be the A, the harmony on the dominant,
the G .
withintheanacrusis,makingtheover-allrhythmseemtobeiambicwitha
(Example 410)'
note' The rhythm would then be dactylic, as in analysis D. An ""gi;_
anapestic "tg.o,;i;g
Ji ria"a upbeat rather than rially anapestic is unlikely because the second B in measure 2 is heard as an
that, for purposes of analysis' two aftert.ut of th.'firri
These considerations would seem to indicaie P and does not easily form the beginning of a new group. only
distinguished' on the one hand, in by .epracing this
different aspects of syncopation should be B with a c (as in the second phrase, measure o) mightihe rhythm
b."o-Ju.ru_
patterns occurring between
syncopations of welljarticulated rhythmic-melodic pestic. Notice, however, that this change also strengthens
involved are primarily metric' on the the dactyl impression
voices, the relationships and problerns because measure 6, like measure 7, now ha, a morJ definitive
shape. oi
such as we have been
other hand, i' p".ioaii 'y"toputio"s within
a single
"oi""' these ambiguities could be resorved by a decisive harmonization.
involv"ed are primarily rhythmic' of
"o,r.r.
discussing, the relationships and problems suppressed accents
The ambiguity of the second measure of the tune is, at least from
such as those in which a negative
course, some contin.to"* point of view, also the result of the indefinite rhythmic organization
'y""opuiio't', of rhythm' Needless to say' the of thl first
produce extended uttutttl"', u'e ulso aspects measure, whose second half becomes anacrustic in the miadle
of a beat. rrrat ir,
interpretationofa'v"."p^.i""-whetherasrhythmic,metric,oracombination were the grouping of the first measure decisive, it would influence
since it -will modify the that of the
of both-has an important effect upon perfo,*u.'.., following measures, because, as we have noted, once a grouping
of stress' the phrasing' and so is estabrished it
placement of beats, ,h. u-o""t and placement tends to be continued. Indeed, the influence of prior orjurriration
i, purti.rtu.if
more fully in chapter iv.)
forth. (The problem oiry.r.opution is discussed strong in this case precisely because of the ambiguity of the
second and third
measures. For instance, the presence of the upbeat, G,
to measure 5 makes the
RHTTHMIC AMBIGUITT second phrase seem much more end- or middle-accented
than the first phrase.
Thus if the first measure is organized so that only the final
Intheexamplesthusfarpresentedtherhythmicorganization,howevercom- beat becomes an
anacrusis, then the rhythm will be amphibrach (Example
plex,wasclearandincisive'Butthisisnotinvariablythecase'oftentherhythm 43a). Notice that ttre
amphibrach impression is considerabry strengtherred wire'
isambiguousanditisdifficulttodecidewhichgroupingisthedominant an upbeat (G or D)
or manifest one. For instance, in Example 42'
a tune similar in many ways to is imagined before the first measure, ih,r,
-ukirrg the initial group a complete
34 The Rhlthmic Structure of Music Rlythms on Lower Architectonic Leuels Js
amphibrach. Or if the sense of upbeat is eliminated and if there is separation in it should be performed?what is rhe correct way? In the right of what has just
pitch between the first two measures, isolating the groups, then the rhythm of the been said, ttre answerin this case would seem to be that thJ parrage should
not
second measure will appear to be dactylic (Example 43b). Finally, if the first be decisively "interpreted" as any particular grouping. However, though
the
measure is written so that the last two quarter-notes form an anacrusis to measure articulation of the rhythm should not be made as put.trt and forecful as i-hat
of
2, then the following measures tend to be heard as anapests (Example 43c). "Ach du lieber Augustin," groupings are nonetheress possible and ,r...rrury
b._ because, were the phrase performed with exaggerated uniformity, the
effect of
the sequence in the second part of the tune, whose raison d,\tri is uniformity,
v
would unquestionably be weakened.
-9tv -vl lv - vtlw -r t-v wttl-fl-flJl:-Y-l4r-t tv -tlvv -llvv -l
The amphibrach grouping, given in Exampre 42 (anarysis a) is possible
EXAMPLE 43 be-
cause the repeated B creates a desire for motion and change and
the performer,
knowing that such a change is about to take place, wilr g"roup the
Observe that were the same changes made in the first measure of "Ach du final beat of
measure 2 with the following tones-that is, he will maie
lieber Augustin," their influence upon the organization of the succeeding meas- the B an anacrusis.
once this grouping is estabrished, it tends to be continued and consequently
ures would be noticeably smaller. Because the rhythmic-melodic shapes of "Ach de_
termines the grouping of the following measure. The discontinuity
du lieber Augustin" are more decisively and patently structured, they are less ance which would be created by a change of rhythm is out
u.ra airi.,ru_
readily influenced by prior rhythmic groupings than the tune in Example 42, prevailing regularity and simplicity of this melody.
of keeping with the
which, at least on the lowest architectonic level, is without a very strong rhyth- A dactylic grouping is arso possible. such a grouping would
mic personality or profile. of course alter the
t'weak and ambiguous" should character of the tune, making the accent somewhat more strongry
The words "well-structured and decisive" or this is not necessarily bad. The choice of interpretation is up
stressed. But
not be understood to imply any valuations. Ambiguous rhythms have their own to the performer.
-more
There is no right or wrong here-just differences. However,
character and function and play just as important a role in shaping musical one conception of a musical phrase or passage may be correct,
dtnorrgrr than
experience as do unambiguous incisive rhythmic shapes. The smoothly Iyrical some are clearly
wrong. For instance, an anapestic grouping of this tune
line of the tune in Example 42 is a result of the fact that the rhythmic groups flow would definitely be in
poor taste, since it would require a noticeable stress upon
into one another. So is the correlative feeling that each phrase of the tune con- the weak second beat
and a crescendo to the following accent. Such an alteration
stitutes a single, unbroken musical pattern. Conversely, the heavily marked, would conflict with
the childlike directness of the melody.
dancelike thumping of "Ach du lieber Augustin" results partly from the sharp While articulation of grouping is desirable in the first two phrases
separation of the groups from one another. The tune seems for this reason to be of this tune,
decisive articulation is out of prace in the middle part (Exampre
a compound of alrnost independent motives. 45a).Arry at-
a b d.

.- u V !,- V v rr- V v ll- V v I


tv -.vltv -.Vltv -l-1lv - I

EXAMPLE 44 EXAMPLE 45

Furthermore, these differences are not confined to the initial phrases of each tempt to divide the passage into separate rhythmic groups
does viorence to
tune. They become even more emphatic as the tunes progress. Thus in the second musical sense. Thus a clear amphibrach disturbs the continuity
of the phrase
part of "Ach du lieber" the separateness of quasi-independent motives becomes (Example 45b); so does an unambiguous anapest (Example
45c); while a marked
the basis for musical progression (Example 44), while in the second part of the dac.trl obviously distorts the general anacrustic motion oi the
sequence (Example
other tune the ambiguous overlapping of groups is, as we shall see, the principle 45d).The phrase is uniform, running without break from beginning
to end. This
of construction. passage is an example of a well-structured metric
or.ganizltion which lacks a
Now the reader may ask: if I have a choice-if I can interpret the rhythm of definable rhythm.
a tune as an anapest, an amphibrach, or a dactyl-how do I decide which way From a theoretical point of view, what happens is that the
second and per-
Rhytlms on Lower Architectonic Leuels J7
36 Tlu Rhythmic Strutture of Music
both as the end which is naturally end-accented has been forced to become beginning-accented.
haps the third beat of each measure act as pivots, functioning Because the pattern is trochaic, we are made to hear the G mo"i"lg
45a)' The succes- to th. A,
of an amphibrach and the beginning of an anapest (Example rather than the other way around. That is, we hear the melodic-rhythlic group
series whose articula-
sive groups are thus linked together in a smooth, unbroken move upward. It points in a definite direction; and when the melody do", irorr.,
tion takes place only *i.n in. goal of the series-the D-is
reached' Though
groups, it fulfils our expectations. It rises.
frequently used in ihi, *uy to create ambiguity by fusing successive
In the fourth Prelude, on the other hand, the accent is stated in the melody
function in the
rhythmic pivots have, as we shall see, another no less important and consequently the group is able to follow its natural course-to become end_
structuring of higher architectonic levels' accented. This iambic group, moving across the bar line from c to B rather
of a song'
we have been treating this tune as an instrumental melody instead the reverse, begins a descending motion which we expect to be continued.
than
of und Gretel,
obviously when it i, ,rrig to a text, as it is in the first act And
Hiinsel
is to a separate after some delay it is. T'he iambic grouping in this prelude is, of course, supported
note set
articulation is almost i.reriituble, particularly since each by the initial anacrusis, just as the trochaic grouping of the first preiud. i, ,,rp_
the articulation in the second half of the
syllable. In fact, in order to strenlgthen ported by the lack of one.
notes on the accented
tune, Humperdinck feels it is necessary to place grace There are of course other factors involved in shaping our expectations about
beats. melodic motion. For instance, the skip at the beginning of the fourth prelude
leads us to expect a descending line which will fill in this structural gap.
And the
THE INFLUENCE OF RHTTHM ON MELODIC MOTION difference in mode makes it unlikely that prelude No. 4 will ascend, will over_
Throughoutthischaptertheinfluenceofmelodicorganizationuponrhythmic come the downward tendency of the semitone. conversely, once the motion
important to note to
grorrpinf has been emphasized' For -this reason it seems the sixth step of the scale has been achieved in major-as ii is in prelude
No. 1-_
may play a significant
that the converse of this is also true; rhythmic grouping it is likely that it will move on up. But the presence of these other factors does not
one of the values of rhythmic
role in shaping melodic experience. Indeed, detract from the importance of rhythm in shaping subsequent melodic motion.
of melody' Hoy-
analysis lies in the fact that it can increase our understanding Finally, in both Preludes the latent as well as the manifest rhythms have
ever,sincethisproblemisperipheraltothecentralpurposeofthisbook'the melodic and rhythmic consequences later on. In prelude No. 1 the latent
end_
pointwillbeillustratedonlybriefly.Example46presentstheopeningmeasures accented temporal organization breaks through and becomes the manifest
ways the two are quite
of Chopin's Preludes Op. Z,+ No. i and No' 4' In many organization as the piece rises to its climax (measures 17-20).In prelude
No. 4
Agltato Largo the legato melody and accompaniment, together with the temporal proximity
within measures, create a rather marked ratent trochaic gro.rpi.rg irrrityi.rg tt
possibility of upward melodic movement. The implications of the tut..ri gror;i.rg"
sivo are in fact realized progressively: first, in measure 9, then in measure 12, and
a)
,lf p --*.t.no bt it
IEEE'EEEE
.1 the climax of the Prelude (measures 16-1S).
7ffi
'1 tv - llv - |
RECAPITULATION AND ILLUSTRAT-ION
| - v rL-=-----\l-J Perhaps the best way to summarize the materials presented in this chapter
EXAMPLE 46 is
to examine particular examples from the literature of music which clearly illus-
Both contain marked temporal trate the various rhythmic structures discussed. The examples will be presented
similar. Both begin on the fifth degree of the scale.
measure is a non-chord tone'
differentiation. In both, the short, final note of the according to groupings, and within each grouping the simplest, most obvious
akindofchanging-.'o...Butinrhythmthesetwomelodiesareverydifferent. illustrations will appear first. The discussion will be confined to essentials.
Where
of pitch, the melody
In spite of marked temporal differentiation and proximity only the melody is given, the reader should consult the original score to discover
rhythm. Therest at the be-
of the first prelude does ,rot hurr" an end-accented the ways in which the particular grouping is supported by such factors as har-
from another' creating a
ginning of each measure separates the groups -o.te -one mony' texture, orchestration, and so forth. Often other instances of the rhythmic
of the things which give this
trochaic grouping within each measu.e. l,.td patrern being discussed are cited. The reader is urged to study and analyze
these.
that a temporal organization
Prelude its agitated, unstable character is precisely
Rhythms on Lower Architectanic Leuels 39
3S Ttu RhYthnic Stnrcture of Music
The theme of Beethoven's Bagatelle Op. 119 No. 10 (Example 48) illustrates
IN DUPLE METER the important role which harmony often plays in the articulation of rhythm.
TROCHEE
33 No' 3 Without the dissonances in measures 1, 3, 4, 5,7 , and 8 which make us perceive
of the fourth movement of Haydn's String Quartet Op'
The theme It does not seem the accent as resolving to the weak beat, this theme might have been perceived
second leveis (Example 47a)'
is trochaic on both Jt" nrrt""d as end-accented-or at least as ambiguous.
ltJ'1ij"Ut"u11t this motion is absorbed
necessary to analyze iit '"up'i*u'y which
general ,r""fr"i" *o,ion of ine o,t.i i.".ft. The accompaniment' Allegramente
into the the trochaic
comes on the beat, provides
a slighl.";;:. that'helps-t:-:"tot"t third level' =+_'- '+ -L-i=-
of the
level as iell as the iambic grouping
organization of the ,"1."a slight tension
hu'u. b".r, too statlc, is given a
The theme, which might otherwise it is worth observing that when
instability oi,f,. six-four nur*onf.-end in
bv the
(measureTz) ur,.r'l''lCrund Pause," the tonic chord is
the tune comes back
t- v t t- v lt- v tr- v
root Posrtron' ' eto, -*
a. Presto EXAMPLE 48

---EI -=E=-- The sixth variation of Brahms's Variations on a Theme b2 Haydn, Op. 56a (Ex-
d- l.a1 ,-vr ample 49), demonstrates the importance of stress in the articulation of beginning-
p
accented groupings. For without the stresses, whether "composed" as in the first
ln i i measure or indicated by the mark " ) " in the following measures, a temporal
-t
__-4--------+--- :---1----)- organization such as this would tend to be heard and performed as end-accented
(iambic). The trochaic organization is supported and assured by Brahms's
1. t- vtt- v t t-Vlt-Vl
orchestration.
b. ?.. Vivace

v i:--llJ r- vl
1.c--Y-.r t- v t L=!! r- v I 1l-!! r- t
p w rr- v '

EXAMPLE 49
EXAMPLE 47

In the trochaic groupings discussed thus far, the temporal organization has
Thisthemefurnishesaninterestinginstanceoftheimportanceofseemingly been such that, were it not for harmonic progression, orchestration, or stress, the
The sixteenth-notes i" change the rhythm of the pri-
insignificant details' ""t
essential m.toai" mJtion' That
is' the eighth- weak beat of the group might easily have become an anacrusis to the next accent.
((open"
mary level; nor do ,t*V "ii.. afte changing its Such groups are in the sense that they trave an "on-going" quality ena-
throughout the theme without
notes G and E.oua ijrr. continued figures? one is bling them to link easily with subsequent rhythms. However, where the accented
structrrr.. whuiihen is the function'oi tr,. sixteenth-note part of the trochee is divided so that it is faster than, and moves to, the weak part
basic is only a plausible
create variety. But such an answer
tempted to answer ,t ",-irr.y crucial is the of the group, the trochee is stable and "closed"; the weak part of the group does
or u"rrlrrirrg variety. what is
platitude. There -. "o,r.rit.ss ways In this not tend to become an upbeat (see pp. 30-31).
of ,ru.i.,f ,nfp_., musical experience'
way in which the particular form of The opening measures of the third movement of Handel's Concerto Grosso
the particular ;;;; *hich the ng"l. i. elaborated at the beginning No. B in C Minor (Example 504) is a clear example of such a closed trochee. This
case the grouping on the second
measure 2 andin the following *.u.,.r..r"determines is so for both the subprimary and the primary levels, since the accented beat on
andthirdrhythmiclevels'Forinstance'hadthethemebeenwrittenasinEx- the primary level is faster than the weak beat. As with most echo effects the
ample4Tbrthe""o"alevelwouldt'ut""ot"i'tedoftwoiambsandanamphi- second level too is trochaic.
probably have been anapestic'
brach; and the tf itJ f""tf *ould
n Ttw Rh2thmic Structure of Music Rlrythms on Lower Architectonic Leuels 4l
Althoughintheclosedtrocheetheaccentandtheweakbeatareequalin Minuet of Mozart's String Quartet in G Major (K. 3g7). Even though a rest
inverted trochee and
g.o.rp is similar to that of the follows the second beat in this case (Example 516), the group is inverted, because
duration, the effect ;m
should perhaps be classed i"itt it. Example 500, from the third movement of the weak second beat is, so to speak, maintained in the mind of the listener. How-
ever, since its literal duration is normal, it does not tend to receive the slight stress
Haydn,sStringq.'.,t..op.54No.3,isanillustrationofaninvertedtrochee
on the subPrimarY level. which is generally felt to accompany the weak unit of the inverted trochee.
b. Allegretto since the inverted (short-long) effect of this kind of pattern is emphasized
a. Andante Allegro
when the accent is divided (Example 51a), this procedure is, as onl would
expect, quite common. What is less common is the division of the second part of
i.
1.
r ,!t t- ,4t
t r- 6rt-,4tr '4t the inverted trochee into smaller note values. But this is precisely what occurs in
the second movement of Brahms's Sonata in A Major for Violin and piano
2. (Example 51c). Though the actualization of the third beat of the measure would
EXAMPLE 50

For further study: Beethoven,


piano Trioop. 1No.3, iv,g-20.;serenade op. 25, vi'
4, 7-4i variations on a Theme b1 Handel, i,7-4;
seem necessarily
) ,fTn,,
to produce a dactyl,
I JTtr, rather than an inverted
23_30;Brahms, Rhapsody op. irl No. trochee, this is not the case. For the final weak beats are so strongly
Chopin, Etude Op. 25 No. i, t-+; Nocturne
Op' 37 No' 1' 4l-44; Polonaise Op' 71 t-vt
Major, v, 1-4; Haydn' Symphony unified that they are perceived as constituting a single unit. That Brahms thinks
No. 1, 38--47; Harrdei corr"oto G.orro No.5 inD of the group in this way is shown by the rhythm of the accompaniment. It is
No.94inGMajor,ii,1-16;Mozart,ClarinetQuintet(K'541)'iv'1-B;ShingQuartet
Musicaux op' 94, v, 1-B; s*ing interesting to note that this inverted, somewhat forced grouping is allowed to
inD Minor (K.421), iii,40-47; schubert, Miments Allemande"' 1-B'3 assume a more natural pattern when a second melody is presented at measure 31.
[uartet Op"29,ii, 1-B; S"ht*u"t', Carnaual" 'Valse -ll
The long-short,,4 trochee is much less stable, for the short weak beat
1,,
TROCHEE IN TRIPLE METER has a strong tendency to become an anacrusis to the following accent. This
Aswehaveseen,durationaldifferencesnecessarilyoccurwhenatwo.unit tendency can be counteracted in two ways: (1) by placing stress upon the accent,
The two basic patterns possible as in Beethoven's ouerture to Egmont (Allegro, measures 22g-3s; Example 52aj
sroup. such as . t.o"h.., arises in triple meter.
'^*. ^j and (2) by explicitly connecting the accent to the weak beat with a series of
J urra ) ) . Of the two the second, the inverted trochee, is by far shorter notes as in Example 52b ftorn the second movement of Mozart's Horn
beat, which is long and consequentlv felt to
be some-
;;. #. *;;;rffi*eak to the following accent' Quintet (K.407), measures 12-14. The first of these patterns is muchless stable
what stressed, does not tend to become an anacrusis than the second. Indeed the strong tendency for the weak beat to become an
b. Allegro c. Vivace anacrusis is realized at the end of the passage. whether Example 526 should be
a.
analyzed as having a subgroup as noted in the first measure is open to question.

( on the whole, the simpler analysis as a singre trochee seems preferable.


'Prw tr- v I
r-Vt ,-(tr-4r '-17t a. Allegro
b. Andante
'tf:
EXAMPLE 51

Example 51c, from the sarabande of Bach's


French Suite No' 4, is a typical t,{ ! tf p
l-vr
al-- -,
l- v lt- v r
instanceofthispatternasitoccursinBaroquemusic..Asomewhatdifferent
rheasures of the
of the inverted trochee can be found in the first
exemplification
EXAMPLB 52

3 The following scheme wiII be used in referring to parts


of musical works: The title of the work, Just as the weak part of an inverted rochee was subdivided in Example 51c,
us number' opus number' ktt:
together with whateve. "ifttt iat"tificutio" 1s"tn -tt-t;l :::il so the long, accented part of a trochee is divided in the first two measures of the
of works such as movements, variations, or separate preces
necessary, will be gi\,.,' fi,.t'- Pa.ts presto movement of Schubert's wanderer-Fantasie (Example
,',,*..als: i, ii, etc. Measure numbers 53a). Because the
included within one title wiII be designated uy s,,,utt,o-u,,
first two beats of these measures form a single pattern and because the third and
will be given in arabic numerals: 1-8'
Rh2thms on Lower Arcltitectonic Leuels 4J
42 Ttu Rhltthmic Structure of Music
comes at the end, and hence it is the weak part of the trochee which is divided.
fourthmeasuresareclearlytrochaic,indicatingthegroupingintended.b'.1.
aJ trochies rather than as dactyls' Both the accompaniment (measures 9-1 1) and later melodic patterns make it
composer' tfr.r" ,rr.u,,,'"s have been analyzed
to be. That this is the proper clear that the basic grouping is trochaic.
which they might otherwise have been ihought
analysis is also rh"*;;;;;rir.., *t i"n is pla"ced on rhe first eighth'-note and by For further study: Bach, English suite No. 4, Sarabande; Sonara in c Minor for
unaccompanied cello, sarabande; Beethoven, symphony No. 7, iii, 14_16 and 30-59;
the phrasing. The second pattern-,J' J)i,-is perhaps the most common way
chopin, Mazurka op. 41 No. 3, 7-4; wa]tz op. 64 No. 1, 7a-74; Handel, concerto
meter. observe, incidentally' that GrossoNo.2, iii, 1-6; Haydn, stringQuartetop.55 No.3, 1-4; Mozart, Divertimento
of achieving a trochaic organization in triple ii (trio), 1-6; string euartet in D Major (K. 575), iii,34-37;
in D Major (K. 205),
it is essentiilly the same as that of Example 520' Schumann, S2mpltonic Etudes, v, 7-4.
Thereadershouldalsostudy*"u,.,..,t62_63and168_30intheScherzoof
of the same temporal
..Archduke,, Trio, *h.,. the trochaic grouping
Beethoven,s
by the use of orna- DACTYL IN TRIPLE METER
;rr*r"* , !-Tn.f , is enforced and emphasized both
figure' Although triple meter easily gives rise to the dactyl, unless melodic and tem-
mentation and by a clearly trochaic accompaniment poral organization, and orchestration and stress are carefully handled, the group-
fifth Prelude from Bach's Tuelue
Example 53b, the opening measures of tlh" ing tends to be unstable-that is, the final beat tends to split away from the first
First, it emphasizes the importance
Little preludes, i, ir,t.r.*ti.rg fo", ,..r.rul reasons. beats and become an anacrusis to the following accent. For instance, the opening
b.
a. Presto measures of Purcell's "Golden Sonata" for Violin and Clavier (Example SS) u."
unmistakable dactyls. But, for melodic and harmonic reasons and perhaps be-
cause of a fundamental tendency toward end-accentuation, the final beat tf the
i.
vl I third measure becomes an upbeat.
Allegro
EXAMPLB 53

the time signature' f,'


of metric organization in rhythmic strrrcture. without instead of 3 X 3' The l- v v I l- u u t

the grouping might;;;; i"tt'p"tta as being 2 X &'


former grouping, involving exact repetition,
woull in a sense.be simpler' But, in EXAMPLE 55

Second, ir might seem,.since all


view of the time signature-, it would be wrong' A patent and stable dactyl organization can be assured in a number of differ-
the notes are equal u;J "it are part of the
tonic triad, that the organization is ent ways. One of the most common is to subdivide either the first or the second
so' Howevet'
ambiguous and should be thougit of as being :h: lTlll:lt-:ll1: elements of the group: ) n ) As is the case with the closed
*ut.iiut, both within and between measures makes such an interpretatron rnap- ", ) n ).
trochee, the motion thus created on the subprimary level tends to tie the group
The most straightforward mode
propriate. The effect must be clear and direct' together. The "Davidsbiindler" March (Example 56a) from schumann's
of organization-and this is what seems called
for-as shown in the analysis' is carnaual, and the opening measures
the accompaniment' of chopin's Mazurka op. 56 No. 1 (Ex-
that of a trochee with a divided accent. Furthermore, that Bach intends
ample 56b) ate illustrations of this type of organization. Observe that in both
to indicate
emphasizing the first beat of each measure,
seems
Non
b. Allegro non troppo
the groups to be beginning-accented'
54) also employs a pattern
The third of Bacil's sli rntu Preludes (Example
case the simple triadic organization -#tr-t_ I L--
composed of six equal notes. But in this qf -'
a)
tr ?bp? bft rt*
-1#-

vl UI
r-
// u wrr-v v,
r- 9vtr- uvrr - Vvtr- vu,

EXAMPLE 54 EXAMPLE 56
Rlrythms on Lower Arclitectontc Leuels 45
44 Tfu Rhltthmic Structure of Music
on the point of metric coincidence-at the beginning of each group. This effect
casesthebeginning.accentedorganizationissupportedandenforcedbystressor is supported by the accompaniment figure, which is a closed trochee.
byornamentation.Intheschumann.*.-pl..t^hesforzandimakethisobvious. orchestration, too, can be used to prevent the final beat of a dactyl from be-
uoin or the richer sonority on the first
In the chopin example stress is a result coming an anacrusis. For instance, the third beat, F-sharp, of the first measure
beatofthemeasur.u,'dtheornamentation:thebrokendiadandthemordent of the scherzo of Beethoven's Symphony No. 2 (Example 59a) might have been
emphasizethisbeat.ThisagainSupportsthehypothesisthatthereisacloserela-specifical- thought of as an upbeat to the following measure (Bxample 596) were it not for
tionship between o"tuln"tttltio"
u"d rhythmic'orSanizatlol^1nd' more
the fact that Beethoven separates it in both range and tone color from the follow-
ly,seemstoindicatethatinSomecasesstressandornamentationperformsimilar ing accent. observe, by the way, that Beethoven uses dynamic change to create
*Ti?:""ing_accented articulation such a trochaic organization on the second level.
group can also be assured by special Allegro
a, b.
aSonefindsinthe..Alla-danzatedesca,,(Example5T)ofBeethoven,sString
Quartet,Op'130'Wereitnotfortt'ee*pha'ispiacedup::thesecondbeatof + -=l!'- -:_- #
-.|.+ --+.
VIn.
themeasurebythe.,o".,'aoandthesixteenth-noterestattheendofthemeas-
t.rrd.n"y to hear the final beat of the ww t' p f p Fr r a)

ure, there would u.1., ut-ost unavoidabl. measure, where the


hrns.
is particularly clear in the first
measure u, un urru.ri,J*. rni, -+-----.t---r- #
motiondownwardthroughthetriad-uk.,,th.Gagoalofmotion.Notethatthe so to r-wVlt-VVr ,- u vl t- v v tetc,
emphasizing the second beat corresponds'
crescendo in the n"i"tti*t"e by the temporal
I J.
speak, to the subdivision of ttre Uttt
i" E*'ample 56a and' that
is like that of Example 56b' The "1-":::::t EXAMPLE 59
organizatio., of ,nt^ifti'a measure
thetuneisaproductofthisnegationofthetendencytowardend-accentuatron. For further study: Bach, Sonata No. 6 for violin and clavier, iii (Allegro), 1-2
and,
Allegro assai
74-17; well-tempered clauier, vol. I, prelude xx, 1-4; Beethoven, string-euartet op.
95' iii, 1-4 and 9-72; Brahms, symphony No. 2, iii,7-.4, 10620, ana tie-is; chopin,
Mazurka op. 50 No. 3, 45-52; waltz op. rg, 5_12; Handel, concerto G.or.o wo. o,
p p v,7-2 and' 5-6; Mozart, symphony No.39 (K.543), iii,9-r2; Schubert, Impromptu
'p>
:'- O v l- O vl
t. I
I
Op, 142 No. 4, 1-16.
1.

EXAMPLE 57
DACTYL IN DUPLE METER
enforce the impression of dactYlic
group-
HarmonY and meter can also help to motion Dactylic grouping is not common in duple meter because, as we have seen, if
9 (Example 58a), the melodic
ing. In Schumann 's S2mPhonic Etudes, No' in the second measure the grouping is to arise, one beat of the meter must be divided:
of the lower voices and the dominant-toni'c
progressron @) ) Jl ,
the dominant harmon y at the end of the @ n ) , or (c).l)J .D . Because of the Principle of Metric Equivalence (see pp.
help to create clear dactYls' However'
rhythmic Pivot (see PP 62tr.), an uPbeat
third measure makes the final beat into a Abends" (ExamPle
22-23), patterns a and D tend to be perceived as trochees with a divided
weak
linking the last two measures. ln "Des t-
AS well as an afterbeat,
a hemio le rhythm-2 X + against 3 X r% beat,d !j, o. a divided accent, a-ta a .
58b) from Schumann's Fantasiestilcke
ted by placing a metric stress l-vll-vt
ts the groups from becoming end-accen Of the three patterns, a is tn. tilEsily perceived as dactylic. Because the
-prevelr
Presto Possible Sehr innig zu sPielen pattern is open and "on-going," either one or both of thaeishth-notes tend
b to
a. I I
-_-+---f--- become upbears, forming groupings such as
) nlJ - .1 hll
lv-tt-utlv-ut
.i;. B.r,.,r.r,
-l- when the pattern remains within th.
P
P -euruiffi i, ;.*-i"lly #rceived as
trochaic rather than dactylic. The long initial note, acting as a standard of
r4lJr- v':!t l*rJJ
measurement, tends to make the final beat seem divided. only where the
EXAMPLE 58 last
Rhythms on Lower Architectonic Leuels 47
46 Ttu Rh4thmic Structure of Music
re- pardy melodic and partly harmonic. Melodically the final beat of the measure
forming part of a new harmony or
note of the group has some independence' occur' But even in Example 610 is closer to the following accent than to the half-note which pre-
se,nse :f dactylic organization
solving an appoggiatura' will any cedes it. Consequently, it tends to group with the accent. Moreover, the half-note
insuchcasestheq.,o.io,,isindoubt.Thus-.u,.',olland12ofthesecond in Example 67b is a note of harmonic resolution-a tone to which the preceding
(Example 60o) might, as indicated in the
movemenr of grulr-Ji, Vlofi' Concero tones have moved. As a result it is perceived as the goal and end of a rhythm.
analyses,beinterpret.daseith..dactylicortrochaic.Tempoisclearlyanim- In Example 61c, however, the half-note is a dissonance and the final note of the
of the divided beat' the greater the
portant factor; the more rapid the speed measure is its resolution. Therefore this final note is heard as the goal which
i..ra"rr"y to apprehend it as a unitary subgroup' b. (74) c.

a j b. Allegro sPiritoso
-Tlr
--)--#
-trl --l---# a) t .FT p moho espreuiao t)
T- r T - p (50)
.tf
I

FGI-- p
V I +, T]]TTJIJJEJJJ] *rp'tcsxc
),---A-
.,lET---?-- -rl
.V--+
-w
m fffiwn--f-+f ffi
.z)*-*1-
,
---# T
-
tv-vl ,t ,-J.--t
a,
a. b.
il:l4rt4J, BXAMPLE 61
-u. ( L=ia r-vl
v vr
lr- r

completes the group. Though theoretically possible, the pattern never gives rise
EXAMPLE 60
to an end-accented grouping in this piece (for an instance of a similar temporal
AlthoughthetempoofthefinalmovementofHaydn's..London,'Symphony organization which gives rise to an end-accented grouping, see Example 70b,
inDMajor(E*u*pt.6oa;i,q,,it"fast,itsmelodic-rhythmicpatternSeemsmore p.52).
it employs' establishes the smaller
dactylic' In the fi"t;i;;, pitt""t.a'-which
Example 62, from the first movement of schumann's string
euartet op. 41
note-value-thequarter-noteinthiscase-astheinitialstandardofmeasure- No. 1, presents no analytical problems. It is included to illustrate the fact that
a trochaic grouping can extend over several measures.
ment.And'second,theskipfromthesecondquarter-noteemphasizesthattone
andgivesitsomeindependence'Blteveninthiscasethegroupscanalsobe Allegro
(36)
analyzedastrochees*ithudividedaccent.Itshouldbenotedthatthemost
fact that they are beginning-
characteristicfeature ,iu",r, patterns (a and b)-the
chosen'
accented-is not affected by the analysis --l-
as in pattern c' will an un-
Only when the secondary accent is suppressed'
ind even then the melodic, harmonic, i4
equivocal dactyl be found in duple -.t... end-
such that the pattern does not become
and orchest.ur orgurri"ution must be
or middte-accented: J Jv I w
o' l-) wtlv J I J,' no' instance' in the
J ]I ) ) I
-llu -l/l EXAMPLE 62
-tv
l, piano Qrrint.i i.t f' Vfinor the figure tl J J o'
first movement of fruri.t For further study: Bach, suite No. 3 for orchestra, iv, 13-j16; Beethoven, Leonore
When it is introduced at oaerture No.3,13ff.,70-71, and316 ff.; string Quartet op. 1s No. 4, i, s-7; Franck,
n ) b..orne, the basis of two different groupings'
J symphony in D Minor, i,729-32 (ambiguous?); Mozart, serenade in D Major (K.
239), i, 6-9 and 57-54; Schubert, Wanderer-Fantasie, AIlegro, 772-79 .
measure50(Examp|e6!a),thefinalbeatofthemeasuremovesacrossthebar
line,forminganamphibrach.At-"^,.,..74(Example6lb)thisgroupingbe-
NORMAL IAMBS IN DUPLE AND TRIPLE METER
comesthebasisforaratherlongpassagewhichcontinuestomeasureg0.Atthis
to a clearly dactylic rhythm (Bxample
point essentiuffy tn. 'u,,'e figt"t"gi"t' 'i" on lower architectonic levels the normal iamb (short-long) is a very common
ut?;. these groupings are grouping in both duple and triple meter. When clear temporal differentiation is
reasons for the difference between the last two of
Rlrytlans on Lower Architectonic Leuels 49
48 Ttu Rhythmic Structure of Music
present'thereisgenerallynoproblemofrecognitionandanalysis.sincesuch accented rhythm is created by the phrasing, stress, and orchestration which
typical instances from mark the beginning of the upbeat groups.
rhythms do not require dir".rrriorr, we will cite onty a few
theliterature:
= | )_I )- -#t----{-F--
Bach, Brandenburg Concerto No' 3, t' J.,tl
Iu
-l[-J '
aJ
'tf
qf
rf
sf TT
-sf l-F "f
' .tf
etc,

Bach, French suite No. 2, Gigue: J],1 't-r t-'tr I


it;-nv t
r.rj jl.l ,\J.J
Bart6k, Sonata for Two Pianos and PercussiP" i1;[2,q'' I i' 1-t-,r w -r rv - r
- lv-|lw -llv -llv -t _r-

No. 5, i,
1J))l)lt)J)l) lv-rt v-l
EXAMPLE 64
Beethoven, SymphonY ,,'+!,'
I v -l I v -l
Example 65, from the fourth movement of Mozart's Eine kleine Nachtmusik, il-
Brahms, symphony No. 4, i: J l1 t IJ l/ lustrates the important role which ornaments can play in articulating rhythm.
| -l -l
The repeated notes in measures 33 and 34 would create trochaic groups, as they
J
Chopin, Nocturne, Op. 15 No. 3, 7 tr.t ,J lc-, rv
rr )l)- | do in measure 32, except for the turn (acting almost like a sforzando) and the
phrasing which force the weak beat to move toward and group with the accent
:: J that follows.
J |
Debussy, Nocturnes Jor Orchestra, "F6tes," 724 ff.:l'iUJ rrlbJv -"'' w't -''
t ' -- Nlegro @2)
N
d

Faur6, piano euart.,;.";r,', df#i.., rv - vt t- wt *J vl


1!T11ffi
t3- '"=3
vv
t-lY+JIw -llv -ilv -

Handel, Concerto Grosso No' 4, iii: ,, , EXAMPLE 65

iii: l-j l1 t For further study: Beethoven, String Quartet Op. 18 No. 3, ii, 32-34; Symphony
Mozart, Strins Quartet in G Major (K' 387)' f+lJ-l No.9, i,92-101; Brahms, Symphony No. 1, iv,95-96; Chopin, Polonaise Op.71 No. 1,
'-lt'J
4-5; Haydn, Piano Sonata No. 33 in C Major, i,20-40; String Quartet Op. 71 No. 1, iv,
schumann, symphonic Erudes,i\o' tu: )ffi a:11J a= 12015; Mozart, Symphony in B-flat Major (K. 319), iv, 114-30.
-=) lv -J lg:J
create a clearly iambic
Where the temporal or melodic organmo'fi$'ot
are often used
grouping, phrasing, instrumentation, stress' and ornamentation INVERTED IAMBS
of Example 63 (HuI91'
to articulat. g.orrpin;r. Fo, i"*tuttce, the third measure The inverted iamb is not uncommon in music literature. This rhythm is, for
PianoSonataNo.3TinDMajor,i),comingaSitdoesaftertwomeasureswhich instance, the one used in the opening melody of the third movement of Berlioz'
areclearlytrochaic,wouldhardlybethou-ghtofasiambicwereitnotforthe Synphonie Jantastique (Example 66a). It is also the lhythm of the theme of the
phrasing (see also Example 81, p' 65)' Chaconne of Bach's Partita in D Minor for unaccompanied Violin (Example
5'iv'22-25) a clear end-
Similarly, in Example 64 (Beethoven, Symphony No' 66b).It should be noted that the inverted iamb always requires an anacrusis
a.
Adagio b.
Allegro con brio /i
,* ,* ,lv
a+

r-L-t'Jlw -llw -tt-Eill


r- ,v l-l
? rv -t tv -ltw - | r 7 v r
f t- vtl- vt t- vt t- vl t-\4t- vt t-wr lljj l-vl
lv -t
EXAMPLE 63 EXAMPLE 66
Rlgttluns on Lower Architectonic Leuels Sl
50 The Rhltthmic Structure of Music

longer than the following accent-and the length


of the accent includes any rests does not carry the burden of articulation alone. It is supported by a change in
instrumentation as well as by the phrasing.
whichmayfollowit. Patternssuchas : l1,t lJ *,J llt!-l-4t u"
For further study: Beethorren, StrirYg Quartet Op. 18 No. 4, ii, 2028,64-65, and.129-
'
not really inverted iambs. In other words, to be inverted
an iamb must show a
30; Symphony No. 7,ii,49-57; Brahms, String Quintet Op. BB, ii,26-30; Symf,hony
disproportionbetweenupbeatandaccent,sothattheaccentSeemstobecut No.2, iii, 17-19; Chopin, Etude Op. 10 No. 71,7-4; Haydn, ,,London', Symphony in
short. D Major, i,45-52;Mozart, Symphony No. 41 (K. 551), iii,61-62 and 64-65; Schubert,
Symphony No. 8, i,63-68 and 85-93.
Adistinctionmustbemadebetweenaninvertediamb,whichinitsclassic
and an extended
form is a long, undivided upbeat moving to a shorter accent'
anacrusis. In the latter, the relationship between
the note-values of the upbeat ANAPEST IN TRIPLE METER
notes precede longer ones' Bx-
and those of the accent is normal; that is, shorter Like dactyls, anapests arise easily in triple meter. However, where all the
1 (measure 41)' is an extended'
ample 6'7a, fromChopin's Nocturne Op' 48 No' members of the group are equal in duration, care must be taken if the grouping
the anacrusis, taken as a
sirnple anacrusis. It is like an inverted iamb in that is to be clear. Thus the rhythmic organization of the upper voice in the beginning
*hole, is longer than the accent to which it moves' of the Prelude from Bach's English suite in G Minor (Example 68a) might have
Allegro molto been ambiguous if taken alone-particularly when played on a harpsichord,
Lento b.
a. which lacks the ability to create special stress. It is defined and specified by the
anapestic groupings which enter in the lower voices. Clarity of grouping may

f rL/-l
b.

-:---a-
a) IJ I'L+J \i_,. I \_-'
EXAMPLE 67
1ll I tTT'l ffir I rrn F
----t4
Example6Tb,fromthefirstmovement(measures5g_60)ofMozart,sSerenade -|re
But it is complex' That is, the
for Winis (K. 361), is also an extended anacrusis. lvv -tl!l_!l_=J
several different levels' The differ-
anacrusis is made up of rhythmic groupings on
encebetweentherhythmicstructuresofthesetwoexamplesistheresultof EXAMPLE 68
elements of the chopin
temporal and melodic differences. Because all the
also be achieved through orchestration, as it is in the third movement (particu-
..tu.r,rrirareessentiallyalike,groupingsexistonlyonthelowestlevel'Onecan-
is an upbeat to the second and larly measures 17 and 18) of Bach's Brandenburg Concerto No. 1 and in the first
not, for instance, say that the first triplet group movement of Haydn's String Quartet Op.74 No. 3 (measures IZtr).
thirdgroups'Furthermore,sincethepassageisa..unison,,,neithertexturenor The anapest is unmistakable where the upbeats are divided so that shorter
level'
harmony serves to create groupings above the subprimary note-values lead to a longer one. This is the case in the Polonaise from Handel's
TheMozartexamplei'q"i,.different.Itillustratesbothharmonicand Concerto Grosso No. 3 in E Minor (Example 686). observe that the anapest
eighth-notes of the upbeat
melodic differentiatiorr. R, a .es.,lt the first three grouping is supported by the accompaniment figure, except in measure 3. It is
groupareperceivedasananacrusistothelastfournotesofthegroup.The
rhythm less necessary in that measure because the return to tonic harmony on the accent
anacrustic character of these last four notes
is intensified both by the
clearly marks the end of a group.
oftheaccompanimentandbythefactthatthefirstandthirdnotesofthe
be not- Harmony often plays an important role in shaping rhythm, even on low levels.
the stress should
group are appoggiaturas' Finally, the importance of A particularly clear example of this is furnished by the Minuet of Mozart's
given rise to the rhythm
ed. without the forte-piano, the pattern might have symphony in A Major (K. 134), where the same temporal relationships are
banal' Of course' the stress beginning-accented in the opening measures (Example 69a) and, end.-accented
, which would have been dull and after measure 13 (Example 69b). In the first case, the harmonic-melodic motion
Rlrytltms on Lower Architectonic Leuels 53
52 The Rh'ltthmic Structure of Music
dominant harmony of the if the secondary accent of the measure containing the upbeat is suppressed by a
is contained within the measure. In the second, the
the bar line to the first beat of the syncopation. What is perhaps the classic example of this grouping is to be found
second and third beats moves the rhythm across
in rhe fourth movement of Beethoven's String Quartet Op. 131 (Example 70b).
followingmeasure.Notethattheplacementofthefigureinrelationtothebar
way around' Observe how orchestration-the alternation of the violins playing the rhythm-
line is changed because of the harmony' not the other
accompaniment and phrasing are employed to articulate this grouping.
Minuetto
b (13b The reason why this pattern is considered an anapest rather than a variety of
{r
a
-# - Y?o-= inverted iamb is that the suppression of the secondary accent prevents the ana-
A-'--'7. + -# ?
H crustic group from having a focal, organizing point-from forming an independ-

f r+^ I a)
il p 3
f;'f_}l.l
ent upbeat group. The figure , such as one finds in Example
I

)r:hel nu* 660,


r v -l
I?T
r--e--frt F--+ =-# --+-F--<J-
L---# entering as it does on a beat, is more properly considered an iamb than an ana-
ur,- w vt Ly----:l pest. However, once again it should be observed that the rhythms are closely
EXAMPLE 69 related in that both are clearly end-accented.
,/ For further study: Bach, Suite No. 2 in B Minor, Rondeau, 7-2 and 32-35; Suite No.
For further study: Bach, Brandenburg Concerto
No' 4' ii' t-12 Beethoven' String 3 in D Major, Gavotte, 1-10; Brahms, Horn Trio, i, 1-6; Haydn, String Quartet Op.
Op. 18 No. 4, ii, 1't4--41 ; Brahms, Piano Trio Op'
10f i'73-90: Handel' Con- 71 No. 1, i,72-75.
Quartet 1_4and18-21 ;
certoGrossoNo. 1,iii, 1_4and !}_2|;ConcertoGrossoNo.6,Musette, in B-flat
Mozart, Symphony in G Major if' tll;, iv, 7-4 and 104-10;-symphony
Simphony No' 39 in B-flat Major (K' 543)' i'
1-8; Schu- AMPHIBRACH IN DUPLE AND TRIPLE METER
Major (K. 319), i, 108-14;
mann, Fantasiesti)cke, "Grillen," 4-6' Amphibrach groupings are common in both duple and triple meter. The
theme of the Finale of Haydn's Salomon Symphony in C Major (Example 7l)
ANAPEST IN DUPLE METER is typical of the amphibrach rhythm in duple meter.
found in duple meter' If the Presto assai
Anapests, again like dactyls, are not commonly
then there
group is created by subdividing one of the
three-unit ""it'',J J I f ,' p lu - vllv - vllv - vttv - vl
values to group together,,] J,lJ , for-i"g
is a strong tendency for the smaller
#-' BXAMPLE 71

aniambwithadividedupbeat'onlyifthemelodic.harmonicorganrzatlon Like so many of Haydn's minuets, Example 72a from Symphony No. 102 in
grouping be apprehended as
emphasizes each of the last two beats, will the B-flat Major, is an amphibrach rhythm. In order to tie the weak afterbeat of the
anapestic. The second movement of Schubert's
Stiing Quartet "Death and the group to the preceding accent, there is, as has been noted, a tendency to stress
one seems
Maiden" provides as unambiguous an example of this grouping as the accent. In this example the stess is supplied by the turn and is intensified
likely to find (ExamPle 704)' by the other instruments of the orchestra, which play only on the first beat of the
can arise in duple meter
As noted earlier, a more Paten t anapestic grouping first two measures.
cantabile
b Andante, ma non troPPo e molto Example 72b, frorn Chopin's Mazurka Op. 50 No. 2, is a somewhat more
Andante con moto
I l I t --F complex illustration of the amphibrach rhythm in triple meter. For in this case
-H-1- =# a' Arl"gto
a 6?t (
pptrl
tr ( r"r - 7
) .tr -h
b. Allegretto
) )) I

l-F-F- -F-#
v
-l--
Pizz. i, v
t l-ltw -utrw avtv - r
p
tv-
r\l_3>
vltl- t-ttfv- \-/l
t-l+-::-:Jlwv -llv -l -l
EXAMPLE 72
EXAMPLE 70
Rlgtthms on Lower Arcltitectonic Leaels 55
54 Ttu Rhythmic Strutture of Muszc
weak afterbeat to the otganization. And one should not attempt to im,pose precision and clarity upon
the accent is subdivided. This subdivision serves to tie the
to stress the accent such an organization. Passages involving ambiguity Irequently serve to create
accent. Notice that in this case, too' an ornament is used suspense. That is, because they present a melodic-harmonic-temporal organiza-
slightly. In other cases-for instance, Example Zg
(p.26)-stress is Placed on
tion lacking clear shape, such passages create uncertainty and a feeling ofexpecta-
the upbea t in order to make its lunction clear' tion that they will be followed by a return to the stability and certainty of clear,
in the sense
In the preceding examPles the temPoral arrangemen t was normal understandable patterns. For these reasons ambiguous groupings are particularly
weak beats. But this
that the accent was as long as or longer than either of the common in transitional passages and development sections.
longer than the accent
need not be the case. In ExamPle 31 the anacrusis is much Such an ambiguous organization is presented in the first movement of Bach's
the grouP its Peculiar
to which it moves, and it is this disproportion which gives Brandenburg Concerto No. 5 just before the final statement of the main theme.
No. 1 (ExamPIe
flavor. The theme of the second Bourr6e of Bach's Chamber Suite In this case (ExampleT4), extreme uniformity is created by the melody, which
73a) is also a partially inverted amphi brach, the weak
final beat of the grouP
the rhYthm Indeed the moves chromatically; the harmony, which consists of parallel diminished-seventh
being longer than the accent. This Pattern tends to close chords (themselves harmonically ambiguous); and a temporal organization
the organization to be a
closed effect is so marked that one is temPted to consider which is essentially undifferentiated. The ambiguity of the passage is resolved
bY an anacruslsr as rn
compound one, made uP of an inverted trochee preceded when the dominant-seventh chord (V) ir reached.
a. b. Allegro molto moderato
(200)

'pp
4. rw -.it
r-(t
a) - eoor -- y= -trF
b. lv -l EXAMPLE 73 gH
analysisD.Againtheuseofornamentsisofinterest.Thegracenotesinmeasures2
"ffisb.Fgr-ffigr-ffi
the weak half-note, F, to the preceding
are important because they serve to link- ?
to the following
c; without trr.- tn" r *igrrt well be thought of as belonging
r"itljr"o* string Quartet in G
73b, from the first movement of schubert,s
a)

Major(measures64-68),isaninstanceofafullyinvertedamphibrach-thatis,
the accent is the short.ri ,ro,. of the group. Schubert
takes,considerable care to
g,, Fs F:, + * --i-t r-
is placed over.the A, shortening-it
make his intention clear. In measure 65 a iot 1o
il
that it will group with the following notes'.tl'u"a t o"
stress is placed
-lh:.G
order to emphasize its anacrustic function.
th. next measures the division is EXAMPLE 74
voices'
made clear iy the motion of the accompanying Uniformity is not always as extreme as this. For instance, the structural organ-
Forfurtherstudy:Bach,chambersuiteNo.2inBMinor,i,20-24;chambersuite ization of the bridge passage in the fourth movement of Mozart's String Quartet
No.4inDMajor,Bourr6eI,l-8;Well-temperedClauier,,]o'.-I,j'.'udexii,1-4;Bee- in C Major (K. 465), is much less uniform than that of the Bach example given
thoven, Piano Trio Op. 1 No' 3, iii, 1-6; String
q"a'tJOp' ]9It'3' iii' 16-20; Sym-
b2 Paganini' Var' iii; Haydn'
Piano above. The metric structure is made clear by the general parallelism between
phony No. 1, ii, 1-4; Brahms, Variations o' oihi*'
measures and parts of measures. The harmonic and melodic changes also create
sonataNo.35inDMajor,Finale,l-4;stringq"urt"top.71No.1,iv,34-39;
Serenade in D Major (K'
Divertimento-i"-"s-ilut Mujo' tr' izoll iul t-+;
a sense of emphatic articulation. Nevertheless, as indicated in the analysis of
11 Mozart,
s+ tr.; Schumann, Fantasiesiilcke' "Ende
vom Lied"' 25-32' Example 75, the groups on the primary level (measures 36-38) overlap one an-
" 2391,iii,
other because the function of the weak beats is unclear-that is, they function
both as afterbeats and as upbeats. The second level is also functionally ambigu-
RHYTHMIC AMBIGUITY
ous. The two F's in measures 39 and 40 belong together because they belong to
Rhythmicgroupsarenotalwaysaspreciselyarticulatedasthosediscussedin the same harmony and are the same pitch and because they constitute the goal
theprecedi.rgpug.r.Oftentheyareambiguous'Thereisnodecisivepatternof
Rhytlans on Lower Architectonic Leaels 57
56 Tfu Rfuithmic Structure of Music
tions as part of the upbeat of an amphibrach group. Because the voices are per-
ofthepreviousmeasures.Fromthispointofviewtheyformatrochaicgroup. ceived as sepa-rate entities, these functional differences create no ambiguity.
However, because tt utt so sepa'aied in range and because they occur in an The analysis and interpretation of music depend upon a knowledge of and
"y 40) is also heard as moving
unstable harmonic ,.ilittg, the second F (measure sensitivity to the style of the work being studied as well as upon an understand-
to an upbeat' The symbol
to the following E. I.r thiJsense the low F is an upbeat ing of the objective relationships presented in the composer's score. For instance,
goal of the preceding motion,
indicates that measure 41 is initially felt to be the the F in measure 40 of Example 75 might be made into a decisive upbeat to the
a further goal-to be an upbeat'
but is subsequently perceived as leading toward following measure by somewhat shortening the high F in the previous measure
Allegro molto and by stressing the low F in measure 40. Similarly, the primary level in measures
(40)
*- -+--
-+-- 37-38 might be articulated as an unequivocal iamb. But both these interpreta-
I r+l ())) tions would be unfortunate; for they would weaken the on-going, kinetic motion
l))-) v
,tt
a)

f: LJ.IJ
+ tfr1 J + +4 +i which is an important characteristic of this modulatory bridge passage.
The opening measures of "Nuages" (Example 77), the first of Debussy's
#
ff Wa W
.ffi T-f- # Nocturnes Jor Orchestra, might be thought of and interpreted as being decisively
l-_-ll-J r- v I trochaic-particularly since this is the organization of the second level. However,
1.
t v I ,vl
such an interpretation would be in poor taste. No effort should be made to
3 create patent quarter-note groups. The quarter-notes should be played as
EXAMPLE 75 evenly and objectively as possible.
Moderato
one in Example 75 as being ambiguous
is forced to perceive the weak beats
because, within the single melodic strand of
a homophonic texture' the same
where several voices
tonal pattern performs"two functions at once. However,
move in a contrapuntal texture, weak beats having
different functions may occur w r-,9 Er#J l-+J
simultaneously without creating any ambiguity.
In measuresl8-21 of the fourth
EXAMPLE
Grosso No' t i" G Major (Example 76)' weak
77
movement of Handel's Concerio
as upbeats in another' Thus in Ex-
beats function as afterbeats in one voice and In this case definitive groupings are out of place more because of the character
is the-end of an amphi-
ample 76, trre *..orrJ q,'u"t'-ttote in the upper line of the movement than because of the style or the function of the particular pas-
the second quarter-note func-
brach group 1u' uft.rn.'ut), while, in the line Lilow, sage. That is, the stress which would have to be placed on the secondary accents
Allegro and the slight shortening of the length of the weak beats, which would be re-
r=------l quired in order to create unambiguous groups, would tend to destroy the feeling
+- of placid, effortless calm which the composer evidently wishes to communicate.
For further study: Beethoven, StringuQ,ru.t.t Op. 130, i,45-46; Symphony No. 3,
lv-l i, 276-83; Brahms, Variations on a Theme b2 Handel, Vars. ii, iii, and vi; Haydn,
Salomon Symphony in C Major, iv, 5-€; Mozart, Symphony No. 41 in C Major (K.
551), ii, 58-59.

EXERCISES
a.
--1---------#,
)J In this chapter, as well as in the ones which follow, what might be called an "experi-
mental technique" has at times been used both to determine the proper grouping of a
b. passage and to discover which musical elements are crucial in the formation of such
e.ljl- -J[l -J t--J l-v --Jl--.J groupings. Generally speaking, this method proceeds by isolating and varying one or
b.sz-J
more of the factors responsible for group formation, exaggerating such factors, and test-
EXAMPLE 76
Rlytbns on Lower Architectonic Leuels 59
58 Ttu Rh2thmic Stntcture of Music
B. For each of the phrases write an accompaniment which supports the desired group-
ingtheresultingpatternsubjectivelytoseewhetheritagreeswithwhatseemedtobethe ing.
grouping and progression oithe original passage'
At times different groupings are com-
possibilities of interpretation' V. Write a sixteen-measure melody whose middle part involves the use of rhythmic
i.r"a i"" -a". ,oloirrt up the different and the appropriate grouping .is
Because rhythms are generally subtle and elusive ambiguity.
not hesitate to employ this method in
often difficult to determine, the student should VI. Using one of the folk tunes given in Exercise I, write a set of three or four variations
doing the exercises given below' for piano.
or sung. This is a rma'rf through-
The music in the exercises of this book is to be played
out. There can be zo analysis without hearing'
I
A.Followingthemethodsandproceduresusedinthischapter,writeaseriesofvariants
on each of these tunes:

2.

3.

4.

the variants written and discuss how


B. Analyze the rhythm of each tune and.each of
the musical materials create the grouping'
IT
A,Analyzetherhythmofthepassagescited..forfurtherstudy,'inthefinalsectionof
thischapterandine."h"usestudythewayinwhichtheelementsofmusiccombine
to create the particular grouping'
B.Severalofthesepassagesmakeuseofspecialstress.Ineachcasewhatistheeffectof
this stress upon the rhythmic grouping?
are cited. How do these
c. often several .*u*p1", of th" I.*e basic rhythmic grouping.
effect? Account for these differ-
passages differ from one another in characier
and
of the passages in question'
ences in terms of the particular organization
"Ah, vous dirai-je' mamam'" In what way does
III. Study Mozart's Variationson
of music in
Mozart vary the of rri, tune? How does he articulate the elements
rhyil*
clear? What problems of interpretation do you find?
order to *ut. tnlr.
"hanges
How would You solve them? WhY?
IV
A.Writetensimplephrasesexemplifyingthefiverhythmicgroupings.Useavarietyof
meters.Wherepossibleemploystress,rhythmicinversion,andnon.congruence'
More Compkx RlAthmic Structures 6l
THEO RETIC AL CO NSI D ERATIO NS
Before beginning the analysis of rhythms on higher levels it might be well to
MORE. COMPLE,X make some general remarks concerning the formation and characteristics of
such larger groupings. As might be expected, the basic principles of rhythmic
RHYTHMIC formation and grouping already discussed are the same for all levels. Thus on
higher as well as on lower levels the rhythmic organization is determined by such
factors as separation and proximity, similarity and difference of stimulation,
STRT]CTI.]RE,S stress and non-stress, and the like.
However, the very fact of greater length makes some of the elements of music
more important than others in the formation of higher rhythmic levels. For in-
stance, instrumentation plays a more important role in the articulation of higher
The concepts and procedures thus far presented have, on the whole, been clear rhythmic levels than of lower ones. Likewise, harmonic tension and release be-
and uncomplicated. In chapter i distinctions were established between the come vital factors in the unification or separation of larger groups. On the other
various aspects of temporal oiganization; meter was distinguished from rhythm, hand, the importance of stress is necessarily minimized on higher levels. It is
accent from stress, u.rd *o forth. In the second chapter two folk tunes were
sub-
of music act to articulate almost impossible to feel the organizing force of stress over a long period of time;
jected to "variation" in order to show how the elements
stress will not by itself tie together or organize groupings for units larger than
ih. buri" rhythmic groupings on lower architectonic levels, and further examples one or two measures.
from music literature iliustiating the occurrence of these groupings were intro- Two things follow from this. In the first place, the type of non-congruence
duced. Now, as more complex examples are presented, not only does analysis discussed in chapter ii is impossible on higher levels. On higher levels non-
also be-
become more complicated but the tasks of selection and organization
congruence would seem to arise between independent coexisting rhythms rather
come difficult and uncertain.
hence its own than within a single rhythmic part. And, second, temporal or durational dif-
Because every rhythm is unique, having its own organization and ferentiation is even more important in the articulation of groupings on higher
particular anaiytic-problems, no selection of examples can possibly cover the
One can at best levels than it was on lower ones. As the lengths of rhythmic groups increase, the
ihythmic permutations and analytic problems which may arise. ability of stress to act as an effective organizing force diminishes and the role of
illustrate what seem to be fairly common rhythmic types and typical analytic durational differences in determining grouping necessarily becomes more
problems. Moreover, the examples have been chosen in order to present a given important.
or procedure as clearly and concisely as possible. No attempt has been Since, as we have seen, durational differences tend to give rise to end-accented
"orr".p,
mad,e to cover the literature, historically or otherwise. In general, the examples rhythms. it is not surprising to find that iambic and anapestic groupings are the
have been selected from the more recent music literature, from the eighteenth most common ones on higher levels. This also explains why the higher the
century to the present, because it was felt that the readers' familiarity with this architectonic level, the greater the tendency for the groupings to be end-accented.
repertory would enable them to follow the analyses and discussions with greater There are two other reasons for this tendency toward end-accentuation. Be-
sensitivity and understanding. ginning groups, simply because they are beginnings, seem to be leading or mov-
The task of organization also becomes more difficult once one leaves the "hot- ing toward a conclusion and therefore expectation is directed toward and em-
music.
house,, variety of example behind and ventures forth into the world of real phasizes (accents) the completing groups or units. An antecedent appears to be
Here, variables do not operate singly. Nor is it generally to
possible classify the
directed toward the consequent which is its goal. And this goal is stable, focal,
rhythm of a given .*"-pi. under a single simple category. Not only do groupings and accented in comparison with the motion which precedes it. Second, this
rury frorn o-ne architectonic level to another, but particularly on lower levels antecedent-consequent organization is so common in the literature with which
as possible
churrges of grouping are the rule rather than the exception. Insofar we are dealing and the pattern is so well understood by the practiced listener
the eiample, girre., itr this chapter have been grouped together according
to the
that he tends if possible to organize groups in this way even though they have
organizatior, of th. highest architectonic level. And generally speaking, the
dis-
not been completed.
cuision proceeds from simpler to more complex organizations.

ffi
More Complex Rlythmic Structures 63
62 The R@tthmic Structure of Mustc
combination. It is a rhythmic piaof. Because the unification of this final group pro-
Thisobservationcallsattentiontoapointofsomemoment.Aswelistento duces temporal differentiation among the three units of the phrase, the whole
time as we evaluate
music, we anticipate the future course of events at the same
what a grouping is wh-ile phrase is heard as an anapset. That is, the final group is heard as the goal or
those which have passed. Thus, though we may guess
accent toward which the first groups move.
it is in progress' we know what the grouping was only when it isgrasped complete' On
almost
can be Sind BIit - ze, slnd Don-ner in Wol
Iower architectonic levels, where the groups are short and Vivace ver- schwun- den
great importance' However,
instantaneously, this fact, though operative, is not of
groups' A gt"-t"
it becomes of great i*porturr"Jin the analysis of larger_ rhythmic 1. tv - ,v tl-_+_jJ
it.is in process-while v
subgroup may seem to be one kind of organization while 2.
meaning, its relationships
we are in the act of hearing it-but may change its EXAMPLE 78
group (of which it is
to what has preceded it and what follows it, once the
whole
seem accented while we it
a part) has been completed. For instance, a group may However, should be noted that there may also be a tendency to hear this
and comprehended, we
are hearing it; but when all its relationships are known phrase as a large iamb with a trochaic subgroup in the first part, thus:
*uy .o-.-,o realize that it was really unaccented. such changes will be or schema-
event
tv - l/Jtv -vttv -vl |l]+J
to a group t-vt
tized by placing the sign indicating the function attributed
understanding of its
when it first appear oio* the sign indicating our subsequent
" but later it is It should be remembered that while the difference between these two organiza-
function. Thus if a group or beat appears at first to be accented,
understood to have i..o ,r.ru""ented, the symbol used
is E/ . conversely, if a tions is real, the most important differences between groups are (1) whether they
the symbol used are beginning-, end-, or middle-accented and (2) the particular proportion of the
group which is first felt to be weak is later felt to be accented,
io.,ld be v . (For a complete listing of the symbols used in this book'
see group.
Whether initial groups such as these amphibrachs are perceived as two dis-
p.20a.)
crete, independent impulses leading to an accent or are heard as related to each
other in a trochaic group depends both upon their similarity and upon their
THE COHERENCE OF HIGHER-LEVEL RHTTHMS proximity. The more similar and the more clearly segregated they are, the more
the units
In order for temporal differentiation to arise on higher levels, one of they will tend to be heard as two separate upbeats. Exact repetition does not
must be longer than
or groups in the organization, most frequently the final one, create shape or pattern. To put the matter the other way around, in order for
indivisible impulse' Such
the others. That is, its parts must coheie into a single, the initial units to group together they must create a pattern; and pattern de-
groupcohesion*'yu,i,.inthreedifferentways:(1)throughthepresenceofa pends upon there being change between the elements involved.
ifrytnrrri" pivot, (2) through fusion, and (3) through pyramiding' Reconsidering the organization of Example 78, we find that the pattern seems
to be quite clearly an anapest. The two initial units are identical with respect to
PIVOTED GROUPS temporal organization, are similar with respect to melodic shape, and belong to
Wherethegroupisboundtogetherbyarhythmicpivottone,alltheelements the same harmony.
missing-but one of
of the group are heard-r.,ol-'. of the beats is suppressed or As indicated in the analysis of the example, our understanding of the first group
thebeats,usuallyaweakone,belongsequallytotwoofthesubgroupsofthe changes as the phrase unfolds. Because it is the beginning of a part, because of its
'E"u-pl.
larger unit. For instance, i., 78 from Bach's Passion According to St' patent melodic-rhythmic structure, and because it is generally performed forte,
set off from each other
Matthew,r the first two groups are amphibrachs, clearly this group is at first understood as accented. However, as the phrase proceeds,
divided. As indi-
by group repetition. Hi*.rr.., the final measures cannot be we realize that the initial groups are both upbeats to the longer final unit. And
beat in measure 4 belongs the first group is in retrospect understood as having been weak.
cated in the analysis under the example, the second
bothtotheprecedingamphibrachandtotheensuinganapest-amphibrach Though pivoting generally takes place on the primary rhythmic level, it may
is urged to look-upthe also occur on higher levels. The theme of the last movement of Schubert's String
1 In the case oi each of the examples analyzed or referred to, the reader will repay study' The
original source. t ut"t. purt, not given may throw light
upon the analyses and Quartet in A Minor (Example 79), furnishes an instance of this. Because the
given for this example ii, for instance, supiortei by the organization of the bass part after third measure is a repetition of the second, it tends at first to be perceived as a
analysis
the tenors enter.
More Complex Rh2thmic Structures 65
64 The Rhythmic Structure of Music
For further study: Beethoven, String Trio op. 9 No. 1, i,76-79; Handel, concerto
second weak unit of a dactyl. However, the very fact of repetition creates expecta- Grosso No. 7in B-flat Major, i, 7-4; Mozart, String Quartet in C Major (K. 465), iii,
tion of change-of motion toward a goal-and therefore this
unit also groups
64-71; Schubert, Piano Trio Op, 99, i, 59-62; Schumann, Daaidsbijndler, ii, 1-8.
with what follows.
Allegro Moderato PYR.AMIDAL GROUPS
The rhythm of the theme of the last movement of Haydn,s ,,Surprise,' Sym-
1 w phony (Example 81) has a much more patently iambic shape. In this case the
2 initial groups, though both amphibrachs, are very different melodically and
BXAMPLE 79 temporally, are in close proximity to each other, and are held together by a
might argue strong chord progression (I-V-I). Hence these two units tend to form a trochaic
Rhythmic pivots may also occur on accented beats. Indeed one group on the second rhythmic level and constitute a single, unified anacrusis on
that amphibrachs are essentially accent-pivoted groyps: ' However' the third rhythmic level.
is because organization
amphibrachs are not common onhigher levels. Partly this -" Allegro dl molto
in the Iiterature of recent Western (-
in extended units of three occurs infrequently (- v)

music. Partly it is because the mind, grouping in the simplest way possible, is
able on higher levels to perceive weak afterbeats as part of
a larger accented '1. tv - ul tv -l
,rrrit, tt.rr,"oE*]. (See the discussion of Bxample 96, pp' 88 tr')
l-vr

2; French Suite No' 5 3.


For further study: Bach, Brandenburg Concerto No' 2, i' 1 and
in G Major, Gavotte, 1-5; Beethou.", Sttittg Quartet Op' 130' ii' 1-16; Brahms' EXAMPLE 81

Symphony No. 1, iii, 1-5; Haydn, "surprise" Symphony, iii'


1-16; Schubert' String
ii' But though, if a choice must be made, this example should be analyzed as an
qrrura., in D Minor, i, 67-66; Tschaikovsky, Symphony No' 5' 9-72'
iamb rather than an anapest, it should be noted that it is an iamb with a more or
less divisible upbeat and hence is very different from the kinds of iambs to be en-
FUSED GROUPS countered later in this chapter (see Example 88). In other words, there is an
The independence and individuality of the weak beats in an
over-all anapestic important difference between an iamb in which the anacrusis is indivisible and
grouping ur..u.r, clearer in the opening theme of Mendelssohn's "Italian" sym- one in which it is made up of a compound group, as it is in this case.
alike and their separa- In this example the unity of the final impulse is a product neither of the func-
ifr""y (E*ample 80). Here the initial groups are exactly by the harmonic
tion is marked. The anapestic grouping is iurther emphasized tional ambiguity of one of the beats (rhythmic pivot), nor of the suppression of
group'
change which takes place at the beginning of the final one of the beats (fusion). All the beats are present and their functions are clear.
The cohesion of the final group is rather the result of its strong structure and
definitive organization.
This organization is peculiar in that the parts of the final group are to each
lj- --yJ lv --9 other as the parts of the whole phrase are to each other. That is, just as the whole
phrase consists of an iamb with a trochaic subgroup on the weak beat, so the
EXAMPLE 80
accented part of the phrase itself consists of an iamb with a trochaic subgroup on
is not the result of
observe that in this example the cohesion of the final group the weak beat. From this point of view the whole phrase may be described as
group is suppressed' That is' in pyramidal in structure.
a pivot tone. Instead, one of tf,. parts of the
is tied to the second half so In such pyramidal structures the separable rhythmic impulses making up the
measure 4 the final beat of the first half of the measure
Thus what
that the rhythmic impulse is carried over into the following measure' final group must of necessity be shorter than the corresponding units of the phrase
is potentially a two-unit group isJused into a single indivisible
unit' In this par- as a whole. For instance, in Example 81 the smallest rhythmic pattern in the first
lowest architectonic level'
ticular case, fusion does not aller the group type on the
The final unit is also an amphibrach. But this amphibrach is very
different in
two measures is the amphibrach group which is four eighth-notes long: ))
lj
But in the second two measures the smallest rhythmic pattern-comparable to
feeling from the ones which precede it-it feels pulled out or
stretched'
66 The Rhltthmic Strutture of Music More Complex Rh2thmic Structures 67

)1.1. Haydn, like most composers) was very careful to indicate in his notation
the earlier amphibrach-is the iamb which lasts for only two eighth-notes rU -t precisely what the rhythmic organization was to be. For instance, had he written
Thus in the second part of the tune two rhythmic patterns occur in the same his theme as in Example 82, the grouping of the rhythm would have been very
amount of time previously taken by only one. different. The amphibrach grouping would have been continuous and, save for
Because *or. hupp.ns in the same amount of time, the pace of the second half a slight tension arising from non-congruence between the melodic and rhythmic
of this tune is fast.r. Our judgments of speed are not absolute. They depend organizations, the theme would have been rather banal and dull.
upon what takes place in a given segment of chronological time-on how the
,.g-..rt is filled. just as one feels that an hour moves slowly as we wait for an
appointment or that a play moves slowly or quickly, depending upon the num--
blr of events which fill the minutes or hours, so too the psychological tempo of rV -U,
a musical passage depends upon the number of identifiable events or changes EXAMPLE 82

which take place in a given segment of time. The absolute rapidity of the stimuli Other elements are also used to insure the desired rhythmic organization. For
does not determine paie. Notes may follow one another with great chronological instance, the organization of the tune into two parts-its iambic structure-is
rapidity-as they do in a tremolo or in many codas-and yet the music may supported by the fact that each half has a different type of accompaniment. In
move slowly or not at all. the second half Haydn not only omits any accompaniment figure which might
The accretion of impulses in the final measures of this theme has still another conflict with or weaken the melodic figure but is careful to reinforce the rhytf,m
consequence: it creates a "stretto effect" something like a syncopation. Because of the melody by having the cellos and basses play the same rhythm in parallel
the amphibrachs are at first the sole basis for temporal organization on the low- tenths (Example 83).
est architectonic level, the meter is felt in quarter-notes and the second quarter- Allegro di molto
note in each measure is weak. However, in the second half of the tune the quarter-
notes are subdivided, creating iambs, so that the second quarter-note, which was --t-f--=ll-J-:
.U /
a 1 ?1
formerly weak, now receives an accent. Since the earlier organization tends to
a)
,p
ar/
v/ v11 1

be continued in the mind and motor responses of the listener, there now seems
to be an accent where there should be a weak beat. This conflict between earlier # -+ -f1--l--l----Y-
and later organizations might be schematized thus: ---,+-L
EXAMPLE 83

Measures I and 2, '-"


El)^nl
l- vl'v l-
.E.tr vl
We noted earlier that the more similar two groups are, the greater the tend-
Measures 3 and 4, ) l.ll )lr: )
rv l-tlv -tlv I
-
vr
ency to perceive them as discrete and independent units. This point is strikingly
rllustrated by the two versions of the consequent phrase of Haydn,s theme. In its
On this architectonic level the "syncopation" is resolved, since the final unit first version the consequent phrase has the same rhythmic organization as the
of the second part of the phrase is an amphibrach. But what has happened is antecedent. As in the antecedent phrase, the amphibrachs on level 1 are different
that the conflict has shifted to the second level. The initial organization between from one another and form a trochaic group on the second architectonic level
measures (level 2), which is strong-weak, conflicts with the later organization of (Example B4c).
level 2, which is weak-stong. This disparity between organizations might be sche- a. b,
matized thus

Measures I and 2,
n Lv__-ltv - vtlgJtv-ilv - vl uz__+J____r
Measures 3 and 4, ) jJ? ) t-l
.taa l.l

L----------=---) EXAMPLE 84

These quasi-syncopations are what produce the stresses indicated in the analysis However, when Haydn wants to bring this theme to a definite conclusion, he
given in Example 81. rewrites the consequent phrase (Example B4D). Now the initial amphibrachs are
68 The Rfuithmic Structure of Music More Complex Rlyttlmic Structures 69

in construction and shape that they do not form a trochaic group' dred years derives its basic metric patterns from dance music, which tends to be
so similar
anacruses duple, triple meters are not common on higher metric levels. But they do exist,
They remain independent and discrete rhythmic patterns which act as
rather than an as Example 86 from Schumann's Papillons No. 6 shows.
to the final group. Thus the whole phrase becomes an anapest
iamb. The second half of the phrase is changed even more radically. In
place of In the beginning the lowest rhythmic level is iambic and the second level is
trochaic. Though there is relatively no temporal separation between the trochaic
the pyramidal construction of the earlier version the final group is unified
by
a pivot tone. ...................-
J-:)z?)t'=
iU ,h.r" changes have one purpose' They create a sense of-conclusiveness and
finality not pres;t in the eariier version of the consequent phrase' First, on the v ll- v I l- vl

lowert archiiectonic level the theme now ends on an accented beat rather than
on a weak one. Second, because of more marked temporal differentiation within
^ :-:54-l-'.
the units of the group, the anapest is more strongly end-accented than is
the
earlier iambic organization. Third, the finality of the leading-tone to tonic
progression is used for the first iime to end the theme. Last, the quasi-syncopa-
iio.rl dir".rrsed above, is absent from this version of the theme. The resulting
,iv ,{v *v

direct.ress and simplicity of the phrase make it a fitting conclusion for the
whole
a)
section.
For further study: Bach, French Suite No' 5 in G Major, Bourr6e' 1-4; Beethoven'
Violin Concerto, iii, 1-B; Dvoi6k, String Quartet OP' 96, i, 3-6; Handel' Concerto
;> i), f,;:\- tifi
lrrlrrrr
E-sD: XFATC t C iD f-i
Grosso No. 9 in F Major, Gigue, 7--4; Mozatt, String Quartet in D Minor
(K' 421), iv,
1-4; Schumann, SYmPhonY No. 4, ii, 2-70.

EXAMPLE 85
GROUPINGS ON HIGHER LEVELS
ANAPEST
O=r.z1

When rhythms are built in proportions such as 2-2-4 ot 3-3-6, there is, as
we have seen, a tendency for the first two units to form a subgroup of their own'
As a result the rhythm is iambic rather than anapestic (see Examples 69 and 81)'
Only if they are very similar, clearly structured, and separated from each other _#
in time or musical space, will the initial units be perceived as coequal upbeats
iL-+ffi
(see Examples 78, 80, 84b). tw -trv) tv -tv lv -tlv) tv -t
The beglnning of the first Bourr6e of Bach's English Suite No. 1 in A Major
(Example 85) provides another illustration of a pyramided anapest' In this case EXAMPLE 86
the integrity of the two weak parts of the group is primarily the result of their
groups, they are perceived as independent and discrete because they are exactly
melodic-harmonic similarity, their patent melodic structure, and their separa-
alike. The final group is a pivoted amphibrach. This becomes obvious if, realiz-
tion in range. Without the imitative entrance of the lower voice, the sequential
ing that the meter is actually f, rather than f, one cuts ,h.g5 values in half so
motion of the upper line might well have been apprehended as a trochaic subgroup
in an over-all iamb' that the meter isg. Now the finat group looks like this: J )))l) .lt.N.
anapestic grouping arises in triple
Just as on lower rhythmic levels the clearest
.n.t.., so on higher levels anapests are least equivocal rvhere the weak beats and This kind of reduction is an important analytical device. Though extended
the strong beat are of the same length-that is, where the superior metric levels rhythmic groupings are felt as we listen to a piece of music, it is some-
times difficult to determine what the organization of a particular rhythm is.
are triple: l-7-I,2-2-2, etc. Because much of the music of the past three hun-
More Complex Rlrythmic Structures 71
70 The Rhltthmic Structure of Music
by simply cutting time values' as in Like most antecedent-consequent organizations, this melody is also iambic in
Melodic and rhythmic reduction-either its over-all rhythm. As in the third statement of the Haydn theme discussed above
this case, or by abstracting the most
important melodic.harmonic-temporal rela.
of extended patterns' This meth- (Example 84b), the consequent phrase is an anapest rather than an iamb. How-
tionships-often reveals tile rhythmic structure ever, in this case the anapest is created not by changing the melodic-temporal
odhasforalongtimebeenemployedbythefollowersofSchenkerintheanalysis
And' as will become apparent in structure of the first two parts of the pattern, but by inserting two extra measures;
of larger harmonic u"l-ti"tut^organizations'2 rhythmic organ- measures 65 and 66 might have been omitted without changing the fundamental
the course of this m"r.,]r""" also"be. useful in the analysis of the
organization of the period. The result would have been a respectable, but dull,
ization of phrases, periods, and sectlons' consequent phrase-and one which would have been much less conclusive. For
with this example' (1) The
Two other points should be noted in connection the consequent which Schubert wrote seems conclusive, not only because it pro-
pivotnoteisonanaccentedbeatratherthanonaweakone,aSinExarriplesTB vides the harmonic-melodic resolution of the antecedent, but also because ana-
and84b.Suchpivotingonanaccentgenerallygivesrisetoanamphibrachwhen
equal duration' However' if the pest groups tend to be more clearly end-accented than iambic ones and because
the upbeat a.rd afte'b'e at are of approximately the stretching of the phrase creates a melodic and metric tension whose resolution
anacrusisisequalinlengthtotheaccentandtheafterbeatcombined,theeffect coincides with the end of the melody.
(2) The low A does not become part
will generally be iamli""("t Example BB)' Indeed the conclusiveness of the melody is such that, in order to maintain
oftherhythmicfigure.ItissimplytoberegardedasaweakpulsewhichServes motion, Schubert begins the second statement of the theme on the same beat
low A is markedly separated in
to articulate the meter. That is, because iite which ends the first. Such overlapping or dovetailed phrases should not be con-
range and ambiguous in its relaiionship
to the melodic voices, it is perceived as
sidered as being pivoted. For though the same pulse constitutes both an end and
partofacontinuousmetricbackground,notaSpartofthemelodic-rhythmic a beginning, these are apprehended as functions of different patterns and can be
pattern. kept distinct in the mind of the listener.
Forfurtherstudy:Bach,FrenchSuiteNo'5inGMajor'Gavotte'1-5;Beethoven' A distinction needs to be made between structures such as those illustrated by
71t-22; Chopin' Fantasie-Impromptu' Op' 66'
5-B;
String Quartet Op. ti, iii',27-36 and String op', 76 Bxamples 81 and 87, in which the theme itself is iambic, and those structures in
Major , iii,7-2; H3y9"., Quartet
Handel, concerto c-rro No. 7 in B-flat Piano Op' which a melody (not necessarily itself iambic) is preceded by an extended ana-
No" 3'\v' l-.4; Schubert' Quintet
No. 5, iv,7-6;Schoenberg, string Quarttt crusis, making the over-all structure end-accented.
i--tz;rr" puJ'ugt' cited onpp'65' 66'and67'
irJ, iii, ul,o
Furthermore, the character of an anacrustic group, and consequently of the
accented group to which it moves, depends not only upon the fact that it is an
IAMB
upbeat but also upon its own peculiar structure-the tendencies, ambiguities,
Partlybecauseofitsirregularities,thesecondthemeofthefirstmovementof and incompleteness of its inner organization-and the way in which it moves into
(Example 87) is a particularly interesting
Schubert,s piano rrir i" nlnat Major the accented group which follows. For instance, the second period of the Haydn
(65)
Allegro Moderato
Minuet discussed below (pp. 83-87) is an anacrusis which functions as a discrete
and separate group. Because it becomes a closed shape when the G is reached,
rv,-vilv.-lig+i]-
this anacrusis does not move to any particular note in the following group, but
tv -vllv - vl
r-qU4-, t- \J I to the group as a whole. In this sense it is an anacrusis on oniy one level.
2.t:L) In the Scherzo of Beethoven's Symphony No. 3 (Bxample 88) the relationship
3.
4. between the upbeat group and the accent is different. The upbeat group is not
EXAMPLE 87
self-contained. It has no shape of its own but moves directly to the theme itself.
on the primary level (1) the first two And the force and drive of the theme is largely the result of the instability and
example of a higher-level iambic rhythm. urgency of the motion of the anacrusis.
groupsareamphibrachswhichforharmonicandmelodicreasonscombineto
the second levet (2)' This trochee-is in turn
a weak Since the organization of the theme proper (measures 7-14) is relatively simple
form a trochaic g-;t;t is iambic on and should be clear from the analysis given in the example, most of our attention
follows' so that the first phrase
upbeat to the piuo,tig'o"y which will be devoted to the analysis and discussion of the upbeat group (measures 1-6).
level 3. The only probematical point in the analysis of the theme is that of the status of
(Vienna: Universal Edition'
2Heinrich Schenker, Der Jreic Satl, ed" and rev' Oswald Jonas
the F in measure 10. It might at first be thought that, as the longest note thus far,
1es6).
72 The Rh2thmic Structure of Music More Complex Rlytltmic Structures 7J

it is an accent. However, a melodic and rhythmic reduction of the structure of the unformity of the accompaniment, which in the first four measures consists
stressed weak beat only of a repeated E-flat, provides no basis for grouping. rndeed, it tends to weaken
the theme seems to indicate that the F should be analyzed as a
(part of a closed trochee), rather than as an accent' whatever groupings might arise. The repeated E-flat also emphasizes the non_
on the lowest level the organization of the upbeat group is ambiguous. For harmonic status of the c and thereby points up the non-congruence present in
pattern
while the meter is triple, the melodic organization of the changing note the melodic-rhythmic-metric organi zation.
does not
is most easily perceirred'as being duple. As aresult, pattern repetition The strained, striving intensity of these measures is also a product of their
of meter and
coincide witl metric .."rr...rr".. Indeed it is this non-congruence tendency to move-a tendency which is frustrated. First, the initial melodic mo-
melody within the two-measure units which establishes the rhythmic
organiza-
tion from the accent-from B-flat to c-creates expectation of an ascending mo-
arising from non-congruence in meas-
tion oi the second level. For the tensions tion, which is delayed for three measures. And second, the very fact of ..p.iitio1
ure 2 are resolved in the following measure, making it seem an accented focal
of the changing-notes makes the listener expect change.
point. Thus though the initial upbiat, B-flat, tends to perpetuate itself, the subse-
Thus the repetition of the first two measures is important both because it in-
Allegro Vivace (5)
hibits the "on-going" tendency of the pattern and because it enables the am-
biguity and non-congruence of the organization to be fully effective. Without
such repetition the cumulative drive toward a clearly accented goal is, as can be
w i. r:-.v'E:-ll.J seen from Example 89c, greatly weakened.
1.
2. when the pent-up energy of these groups breaks through at the end of the
3a. L fourth measure, it seems momentarily as if the first four measures will, in retro-
3b. spect, be heard as anacrustic to measure 5. However, as measures 5 and 6 move
EXAMPLE 88
upward and become chromatic, it becomes apparent that the true organizing
il

a. b. accent has not yet been reached. For since these measures are uniform in both
melodic and temporal otganization, they, too, are ambiguous rhythmically. In
fact, were it not for the harmonic and melodic articulation present in the ac-
EXAMPLE 89
companiment in measures 5 and 6, the chromatic scale might have continued on
beyond the B-flat.
quent groups on the subprimary level (i) are decisively neither anapests
nor am- The articulation of the accompaniment in these final measures leaves no
phibrachs' doubt that the B-flat is tlte accent. But Beethoven, to make his point doubly clear,
first f^,,-
The pattern of the G-.+ four maecrrrFc qr
measures seems strained and unLnatural. This is in marks this focal point with the entrance of the oboe, which plays the remainder
pafi a product of non-congruence. In part it arises because the changing-note of the theme in octaves with the first violins.
pu,,.r1 i, such an obvious one that the listener strives to keep the functions of the The function of the upbeat part of the theme of this scherzo must be
is a
individuat tones within the pattern constant. That is, when the C, which distinguished from anacrustic organization which is purely introductory.
non-chord tone, comes in place of a chord tone in measures 2 and 4, it seems
out
All introductions are, in a sense, anacrustic in character. Lacking strongly
of place and forced. organized, substantial shapes, they tend to be apprehended as moving
The tremendous importance of melodic-metric non-congruence can easily be toward more patently structured patterns. To put the matter in another way:
seen if the first four measures of this theme are rewritten so
that the motives fit
expectation, directing attention toward coming events, gives such passages a
with the meter (Bxample 89a and,&). Now these measures, though much the
same
They are flat and quality of anacrusis. This is the case even with simple, relatively stable introduc-
in melodic direction, u.. rro longer either ambiguous or tense.
weakened tions-such as one finds in some Schubert songs or chopin Waltzes-which
dull. And, in eliminating their ambiguity and tension, we have also
serve mainly to establish the melodic, harmonic, or metric framework for later
their anacrustic character. They are too stable' events. Where introductions are built of obviously weak melodic-rhythmic
of
The accompaniment of the anacrustic group supports both the ambiguity materials, shifting harmonies, and so forth, as in the opening measures of the
non-chord tone' For
the groupings and the strain produced by the "misplaced"
74 The Rlythmic Structure of Music More Complex Rlrytlmic Strurtures Zi
the sense of anacrusis is note motion) the rhythm is obviously trochaic. The first four
second movement of Berlioz' srymphonie Jantastique,s measures might be
considered dactylic on the primary level (1). But because the
often intense. third beat of each
measure is a repetition of the second and because the rhythm
Thoughtheyhaveweakmelodicpatternsanduncertainrhythmic-metric of the ,niotu, J J .
Scherzo are not in this that
organization, the opening measures ol. the Beethoven is of an inverted trochee, it seems preferabre to anaryze an. *"ur.r?l,
sense introductory. They"are directly tinked
to the main melodic event' They trochaic. In the second harf of the passage, the rhythm of the melody
treats them as such in is even
constitute an integral pu,t of the theme, and Beethoven more patently trochaic. The repear.i ,ro,. separates the second
and third beats
Iater parts of the movement' from the first, and the phrasing serves to ernphasize this separation.
of iambic organiza- Frowever,
since further examples of anacrusis and other examples the viola now plays an amphibrach rhythnl across the bar
and the marked
(see Bxamples 66,69,80, 95, 100, latent organization thus created makes the over-arl rhythmic effect
tion are discussed in oih., places in this book more
organizations are
103) and since instan"., oi iambic (antecedent-consequent) dactylic than would otherwise be the case.
for further study'
common in the literature, only a few works will be cited On the second level, the repetition of the first and third
measures creates un-
1-4; chopin, valse Brillante equivocal trochees. But the second half of the passage i,
For further study: Bach, Toccata in G Minor for clavier,
Op. 34 No. 3, 9-3i; lvoY5k, Symphony No' 5 in E Minor'
iv' 7-25; Haydn' Symphony Though the slight change in line at measure 7 (where a -o." ambiguous.
in D Major (K' 385)' iv' 38-52; G would have followed
in D Major, "The Clock, " i, zi-3i; Mozart, Symphony had the pattern been regular) creates an accent on the higher
level, the se_
Schumann, String Quartet Op' 41 No' 1, ii, 1-6' quential motion (measures 5-B), tending toward uniformity,
makes measure 6
function as a pivot between measures 5 and 7. It welds
the second half of the
TROCHEE theme into a single unit.
Because so much of Western music has been based
upon duple patterns derived The analysis of the first half of the theme on rever 3 is more
on higher probrematicar.
from the dance, marching, and the like, trochees are not uncommon Though they have been anaryzed as being trochaic, these four
measures might
by exact repetition-
architectonic levels. Fo, iir.n two-unit groups are created also be considered the two upbeats of a larger anapest
group, thus:r v v _r. 1r,
either case, however, the organization of the fourtl lev-el --^
Allegro is .'a-u".*r.a]'
since these eight measures are repeated exactly in the next
eight measures, the
organization of the fifth level is again trochaic.
z -7;T;T r For further study: Bach, French Suite No. 5 in G
a) ,t l) -. -. -. -. P.tsts ?. rJ.Fr.
-l
P.!
a

turne op' 9 No. 2, 1-B; Hander, concerto Grosso No. s


Major, Gigue, 1_2; Chopin, Noc_
i,'n trujt., ii, 1-1g; Haydn,
0 l--l I - string Quartet op.20 No' 5, iii, 7-16;Mozart, piano
euintet in E"-Rat Major (K.4s2),
I

W ffi I'r I iii, 1-16; Ravel, Le Tombeau de couperin, prerude, 1-14;lchum


unn, Kindrrrrenen, iv.

1. 2 g)
, DACTYL
3.
4. feeling of a dactyric rhythmic grouping is most marked
__The in tripre meter.
5. However, since triple meter is not .oi.-on on high.,
EXAMPLE 90 architectonic revers, ex_
tended dactyls are not frequently encountered in music
literature. Furthermore,
phrases or periods even when the higher metric organization is in threes,
whether the groups be pitches on the lowest level or complete the tendency toward end_
accentuation makes groupings such as rw v | 21d r ,wr
on higher levels-the risulting rhythms are as a rule trochaic' - - -, more common than
have already dactyl groupings.
since several example, of"high.r-level trochaic organizations
47, 53, and 77), only one If a dactyl is to be unequivocal, the organization must be such that
been presented in other conne.iiorr, (see Examples the three
elements of the rhythm maintain their individuality.
illustration of this grouping will be analyzed here' OLhenvise subgroups will
the eight-
The Scherzo of gorodin;s St'i"g Quartet in D Major begins with be formed, making two-unit rhythms,
U ) or'J ,J J,, or
the middle
level (that of eighth- ele-
measure melody given in Example 9o. on the subprimary
3 For an analysis of this passage, see Meyer, Emotion and Meaning in Music, pp' 180-82'
ment of the group wil act u, u pi'ootff aryffithe third erement of
More Complex Rfuith,mic Structures 77
76 The Rhythmic Structure of Music
of the second' so that Another, less common, way in which dactyric groupings can occur is by .,inter-
the group will be a repetition, either exact or modified, ruption." This mode of articulation is employed in the Scherzo of Beethoven's
An instance of such an
their parallel furrctisos(us weuk beats) will be clear' symphony No. 9. In the discussion which follows we shall proceed from an
(measutes 42-66) of Mozart's
organization is to be found in the first movement analysis of the main theme of the movement to a more detailed examination of
StringQuintetinCMinor(K.a06;Examplegl)'Thefirstphraseisdactylic its structure on lower architectonic levels, and from this to a consideration of the
Allegro (45) dactylic organization which appears later in the movement.
The main theme of the movement consists of a four-measure phrase (Example
v ll- v I
93) in which the weak beats of the initial beginning-accented group also functiron
l:g-lr-
1. l:lJ as upbeats in the final end-accented group. That is, they function as pivots
be_
2.
3.
tween two accents. Notice that while the juxtaposition of pivoted weak beats
(measures 10-11) within the larger group makes for cohesion, the juxtaposition
Presto
r- utt- v rL--J r- v rt:#-U3#l-- t
(15)

EXAMPLE 91 t
on the second level. The second phrase begins as though
it were going to be EXAMPLE 92
a result of the harmonic
du"tyti" on the third level, but turns into an anapest as
more rapid motion and Molto vivac€
and melodic emphasis or in" cadential measures. The highest
(15)
iambic on the ffiE=
end-accentuation of the second, phrase make the grouping
-i# i:--.F
# # zE-=---f
-#
level: 3+3 3+ vl3 Z+2+2 2+2+2 a
3 . [=-!ll r- tev-l 19w-l PP
4 vl pp )h) i Aha
+--'+ -
F
5
is a metrrc -s-1__ r--7,"-
Note that the change from 2 X 3 measures to 3Y.2
measures
change very like that of the hemiole rhythm'
in- EXAMPLE
If a three-unit rhythm is to arise in duple meter, there must bewetemporal
93

inequalitv tends' as have seen'


) ) , ) )'), o, ) ) ); and such of strong beats between the larger groups (measures 12-13) makes for separation.
"q;;lt;j
to proi,rJ.;"d_;;;;.a gro"p*. only if the weak afterbeats
of the group are
whenever an end-accented group is followed by a beginning-accented one or
equivalent, or so separut.l tfrut they retain their
individuality, will the dactyl a beginning-accented group is followed by an end- or middle-accented one
or can give rise to a com-
impression be assured. (If the afterbeats are proximate (provided the weak beats do not act as pivots), the rhythmic flow is disturbed.
the whole pattern
pori,. pattern, they will tend to form a trochaic subgroup and such "reversals of rhythm," as we shall call them, generally play a significant
are often imita-
will be perceived as trochaic.) such equivalent afterbeats, which aS ..echoes.,, part in articulating musical structure. The progression from an end-accented
usuall ji referred to
tions of the final part of the accented group, are group to a beginning-accented one (as in Example BB, level 2, measures 7-14,
groups (see Example 50a, p' 40), are
And while single echoes, creating troJhaic and Example 93) creates a more marked disjunction-a stronger reversal-than
fairly common, double echoes are not' does the opposite procedure.a For in such cases the disturbance of the normal
IntheFinale(measuresg-16)ofHaydn's"Military"symphonyinGMajor movement (accent-weak-accent) of the rhythms is made patent because the
as separate ele-
(Example 92), the weak beats of the dactyl retain their integrity accents-the nuclei of the groups-are placed side by side. where a beginning-
ments of a group because (1) they are repetitions of
the end- of the accented part accented group is followed by an end-accented one (as in Bxample sl, level
in from the accent i)
;;;; rrry,fr*; (z; ,n.y are separated in range and time both relatively closed
the normal order, accent-weak-accent, is retained and the reversal is less
an6 from each other; and (3j as cadential figures, they
create
rFor an even more striking instance of reversal see Meyer, op. eit., p. 113.
shapes.
78 Ttu Rfuithmic Structure of Muic More Complex Rtyttmic Stntctures 79
marked. To put the matter in another way, because there can be more than one measure of the theme is so constructed that it is
unmistakabry beginning-ac_
weak beat to a group the juxtaposition of weak beats gives comparatively Iittle cented. Because the.eighth-note berongs both to the pr.".iirrg
dotted quarter_
feeling of disturban""-*. can move easily from group to group. However, since note and the folrowing quarter-not., it serves to bina
the measure together,
thereis only one accent to a group, the juxtaposition of accents makes us acutely making it indivisibre. It is a single impurse. consequentiy,
;;. final A is not able
aware of the disjunction of the groups-of the reversal of the rhythm' Finally, it to separate itself out. rt cannot become an anacrusis to
measure 10.
should be pointed out that such reversals are not common on the lowest archi- The orchestration also supports the desired beginning-u""..r,.a
organization.
tectonic level, for there they would make the phrases seem disjointed and broken The oboe, playing the first note of each *easuie witt
the viorins, marks the
up. Reversals are used rather to articulate the separation of larger groups- beginnings of the groups and at the same time necessarily
places a slight stress
phrases, periods, etc' upon the accent. And, as we have seen, the stressing
- or u., u*..rt tends to tie the
Of course the separation of groups is a product of other aspects of the organiza- weak beats to the accent.
tion as well. The most important of these, in Example 93, is the obvious repeti- Beethoven establishes and emphasizes the indivisibility
and independence of
tion of the pattern both between the groups taken as whole units and between the octave motive, which begins the theme, in the eight-measure
introduction.
parts of the groups. Not only does the theme begin again in measure 13, but the That is, since it stands alone, folowed by a rest, therels
no question of the third
last measure of the first statement of the theme begins the counterpoint to the beat becoming an upbeat. It is one pattern.
second statement. The introduction performs other functions as well.
In the it creates
first prace,
expectation-a feeling that a more substantial and palpable
I\folto vivace (5) (10) ) ) merodic_rhythmic
I pattern must be forthcoming. It establishes no
&.
tr (r a
meloiic shape; it merely moves
through the triad with the groups so separated temporaily
that they remain dis_
crete units. Rhythmicaily the primary lever is clear,
1. l-vl
(\-/ \-,fl but'since all the units are
| (v)t |
- alike, no rhythm arises on the second architectonic
2. - iever; that is, the rhythm is
b.
3- incomplete.
Second, the introduction serves as a kind of ,.potential,,
.___,4_ or .,pre-state,, for the
rest of the movement. For instance, it establishes
the basic eight-measure metric
t-wilv -wtlv -vtlv -vllv length which the main theme exhibits. And the beginning-a"""r,,.d
organization
EXAMPLE 94 of the first two measures (on level 2) is also latenl in
tlr. introduction because
compared to the forte octave motive, the rests are
weak. Most important of all,
Before considering the interruption of this theme which creates the dactyl for our purposes, the_ introduction sets up the interrupted
overrapping of voices
organization on higher levels, let us analyze the main theme in more detail (Ex- (measures 5-6) which plays such a vitai rore
ample 94), for it provides an excellent illustration of the care taken by a composer
in subsequent rhythmic deverop_
ments' That is, the relationship of the motive to
to articulate grouPing. the foilowing rest, established.
in measures 1-4, is broken in measure 6. The motive
Beethoven wants the groups on the lowest architectonic level to be beginning- enters too soon_where the
rest should have been. Not only does this create
accented. To achieve this organization he must counteract the tendency for an a sense of interruption_a kind
of double downbeat-but it makes the final organization
end-accented organization to arise. The melodic-rhythmic structure of this pattern triple rather than duple.
of the motive_rest
theme does not create unequivocal groupings. Particularly in measures 10-12 the
This has significant consequences at measure 776, where
rhythm might be interpreted as being middle- or end-accented. In fact this after onry three
measures the theme is interrupted by its own
grouping is potentially so strong that all one needs to do in order to make this repetition (Example g5). This inter-
ruption effects a striking change in the grouping-in
.nytnt" upp.ut is to change the organization of the first measure so that the final the way the passage is
heard. In the theme proper (Example 93) the dominarrt
A is a clear anacrusis to the D in the following measure (Example 94b). Once organization is one of a
this takes place, the anacrustic organization continues with ease. more or less independent, accented first measure
which is differentiated from
This calls attention to one of the most important ways in which Beethoven the following measures merodicaly and rhythmicaily;
urrd ti. second measure,
counteracts the tendency toward an anacrustic organization' That is, the first though necessarily grouped here with the motive measure,
is onry weakly so.
B0 Tfu Rfuithmic Structure of Music More Complex Rlrythmic Structures gl
Therefore, as the theme unfolds, the second measure is heard as part of the final tively clear instance of this grouping, such as is found in the opening measures of
three-measure anaPest. the Finale of Brahms's String Quartet in A Minor (Example 96).
In Example 95, however, this organization is reversed. The grouping is now The theme is in four parts. The first three are similar. The fourth, a concluding
clearly dactylic. And the whole feeling of the passage-the tendency of the second phrase built upon the final motive of the preceding part, is different. on the pril
and third measures-is changed. The initial measure seems to receive even more mary level, the rhythm of the first three phrases depends upon metri" orgu.ri"u_
stress than before and is linked to the following measures. Indeed, Beethoven tion as well as upon the melodic-harmonic structure. As indicated in Example
makes this grouping clear by having the second bassoon enter on the third beat 97a, the melody consists of three dupie units, while the accompaniment is uuitt
of the measure so that this beat becomes an anacrusis to the second measure' of two triple units' This metric crossing prevents genuine accents from arising
Similarly, in the next three-measure unit the second clarinet links the motive on the subprimary level. As noted in the analyses of Examples 96 and 97b, thl
measure to what follows by playing the last beat as an anacrusis' And whereas Allegro non assai
in the original version of the theme there was a tendency to hear or make a
crescendo on measures 2 and 3, presaging the coming of an accent, no such mo-
(180)
t+t
).11 !'
I
l\
-aLt l .J)

Ritmo di tre battute


z-$D--
a)

f
lf I rr tt
I I T

i.l
H I
_4-*l--
a)
-bc- .lkl=
-t(l' Lrilr r
+
ftrrl jj jj j )
,P
,(' --) - --J-t.-----{- 1.
2
---.#- --t--l----*tr-- 3

EXAMPLE
t *##
10)

:TT-T _T
95 #
+

tion toward a goal is now present and there is no feeling of crescendo' The second
a) rll
r I I
l- I
t-
I
# r+
r
and third measures are now heard as hanging from the initial measure' almost 41 i i ) I
i j j ) -D-l P
like slightly varied echoes. + 't tF ___l_-__t+_ -# I rur
For further study: Beethoven, Piano Trio Op. 97, iii, 760-67 String Quartet Op' lwl
727, iii,27-32; Brahms, String Quartet in A Minor, iii, 1-7; Dvoi6k, String Quartet 3. 2. v
Op. 51, iv, 40-49 and 58-63; Mozart, Divertimento in B-flat Major (K' 287)' v, 1-B;
String Quartet in D Major (K. 499), iv, 1-8; Stravinsky, Suite No. 2 for Small Or- EXAMPLE 96
chestra, iv,5-72.
potential accents of the upper line are felt as stressed weak beats. A further
AMPHIBRACH metric irregularity results from the fact that each two-measure hemiole is fol-
lowed by a measure of normal f meter. consequently, the metric pattern
Although on lower architectonic levels it is one of the more frequently en- ceived as consisting of a unit of f, followed by orre of
is per-
countered and patently articulated groupings, on higher levels the amphibrach f,-or, if reduced as in Ex_
ample 97b, as + + 8.
is one of the least common and most equivocal rhythmic patterns. Not only do
As the reduction in Example 976 shows, the melodic-harmonic organization
most of the difficulties discussed in connection with the formation of higher-level
is congruent with that created by the metric crossing. The melody
dactyls obtain here, but some of these are intensified. In the amphibrach the through
the A minor triad in the first part of the phrase, changing its-orr.,
accent must function as a pivot between two weak elements' As a result, ,the direction and
organization at the end of the pattern. Similarly, the harmonic motion is
tendency for subgroup, to urir.-for the organization to become ,'r4- ,orl9-,, articu-
rather than t-s€ -, even greater than in the case of the dactyl. lated in the third measure (see Example 96) by a progression from the
tonic to
Rather than embark
-is upon a lengthy discussion of the theoretical conditions the dominant.
necessary for the formation of higherJevel amphibrachs, let us turn to a rela- Had there been no metric crossing-had the accompaniment also been duple,
82 Ttu Rhythmic Structure of Music More Complex RlAtlmic Structures gJ

as in Example 97c-the rhythmic structure of the primary levei would have been progression creates an over-all motion toward the dominant and. because the
very dif- melodic structures of the first two phrases are different enough to form a true
much the same. However, the character of the theme would have been
level would have subgroup. But whichever interpretation of the relationship between the first two
ferent. The regular occurrence of accents on the subprimary
made the theme seem prosaic and pallid, rather than intense and vital'
Without phrases is favored, the most characteristic feature of the rhythm of the whole
metric crossing, the metric organization of higher levels would also have been theme-that it is middle-accented-is the same in both analyses.
different. It would probably have been duple or quadruple, as in Example
97c' For further study: Beethoven, symphony No. 3, iii, 175-82; Brahms, Intermezzo op.
76 No. 3, 7-5; chopin, Polonaise op. 40 No. 1, 7-2 and, 3-4; Mozart, symphony in
instead of triple as it is in the theme proper
a. b. + D Major (K. 504), ii,7-4.

---<L-
l-Jru - I a ANALTSrc OF A WHOLE SECTION
1
The trio of the Minuet of Haydn's "salomon', symphony in c Major consists
>ti-pFr- ;+--+-4
@
.l- of three eight-measure periods. The third of these is a repetition of the first. The
=tE--ae-l-
\t ,} parts of the trio might be symbolized thus:
1.r: v (v)t - v(w)tl- -v
1. I- 16 v ll:A:ll:BA:ll.
e,
The rhythm of the first period is trochaic on both the first and second levels
4
(Example 9B)' Since the lowest common denominator of motion is the measure,
--+--------v- -l---t---* the primary level is between measures.
a) Allegretto (5)
>

-----.r- + -{---------t-
i. etc.
ru -l lg-=-llv - ltl/---:J 1.
,
3.
EXAMPLE 97
EXAMPLE 98

The metric crossing is also important because, together with the harmonic- The subprimary level, within measures, is dactylic. Frowever, because the
melodic organizationl it articulates the structure of the whole theme and so initial upbeat organization tends to be perpetuated and because repeated notes
emphasizes and closes out each phrase that the final phrase
is perceived as a
tend to separate themselves from their fellows, particularly where the harmony
stable and
separate unit. Furthermore, although the final phrase is harmonically remains static, there is a tendency for the final beat or beats of the dactylic group
mltrically regular, it does not constitute the accented portion of the theme' It is
to split off and become upbeats.
an unaccented feminine ending. The resolution of the metric conflict and the
Haydn counteracts this tendency in several ways. First, by dividing the second
arrival of stable, dominant harmony, which the final phrase merely repeats,
beat in measures 3, 5, 6, and 7, he creates an internal grouping which welds the
to
are reached and established at the end of the third phrase. Also contributing last beat to the firsr one. The importance of the eighih-trtt., i' this grouping
the feminine effect of this Iast phrase is the fact that it reiterates in echo-Iike
can be seen if the tune is sung without them-for instance, with repeated F;s ii
fashion the melodic fragment which ended the preceding phrase' measures 3 and 5. When this is done, end-accentuation is all but inevitable.
The decisive articulation of groups on the primary level has yet another con- second, Haydn emphasizes the beginning-accented organization he wants by
sequence. It makes Possr ble an alternative analysis of the third
level For the
placing a stress on the accents. And, finally, the dactylil grouping is reinforcej
second group is so closed out and separated from the first that the entire theme
by the grace notes which not only support the stresses already indicated in the
might be analyzed as to a trochee, notation but also articulate the organization by separating the groups from one
-13,
Er ,e
another. Notice too that the stresses and grace notes of the first phrur" are so
it is because the harmonic placed that they also support the trochaic grouping of the second rhythmic level.
If the analysis given in Example 96 seems preferable,

I
84 The Rhythmic Structure of Music More Complex Rlryttmic Structures Bi
important in shap- amphibrach grouping (indicated by the inverted brackets in
As units become more extensive, grouping, though no less the example) is ar_
pronounced' One most as strong. The organi zation is now such that the initial
ing musical experience, often does btto*t less palpable' less upbeat is able to
more perpetuate itself, affecting later groupings. Furthermore,
wiy i' which the grouping of such extensive units can be studied and made melodic motion, associated with a sense of ,t.irrirrg, tends to
the general rising
its essentials'
paient and distinct i* uy r".a,r.ing the rhythmic-melodic motion to make later notes the
and rhythmic proportions accented focal points rather than earlier ones. Lastly,
In any such abstraction and condensation the melodic the final notes of each
is correct, it should measure tend to become upbeats, both because they
and relationships must be kept intact. And if the reduction act as neighbor notes to the
not do so, it is prob- first beat of the next measure and because of the motivic parallelism
reflect the musical sense and intent of the passage. If it does between
one must feel the the upbeat and downbeat groups (see upper brackets in
ably a distortion. Thus this technique is an art, not a science' example).
be correctly abstracted' rndeed, in measure 11, when this parallel organization
motion and sense of the original befor. the passage can breaks through, an
the highest level' end-accented grouping seems about to become dJminant.
Though the grouping of tli, period is quite apparent even on A strong finar accent
of rhythmic reduc- is expected. And this feeling of motion toward an
its very simplicity -ukJ, it welf suited to illustrate the process end-accented goal is heightened
natural unit for reduction is the both by the faster pace in measure 11, where things happen much
tion. Because of the clear motion by measures the more rapidry
iambic grouping on level 3 than earlier, and by the crear inrent of the melod]c linl^to
dotted half-note. And if the period is thus reduced, the reach a G. Had the
obvious tonic-dominant expected G arrived in measure 12, the organization of
becomes readily apparent (Example 99). Aside from the the phrase on the primary
level would have been different; the second group
l_r worrli then have been end_
d.- J
b. accented.
)-- Owing in large measure to the general anacrustic character
of this phrase our
opinion concerning the function of its groups on the primary
1.t-rAt-at l-vl l-vl and secondary
2.E-.t t L-=---gJ E-U-l- ll-l levels changes as the music unfords. For instanc.,
9 is first perceived as
accented, but is in retrospect considered to have been -.urrrr"
anacrustic to measure 10.
EXAMPLE 99
As the passage progresses, its intent becomes unequivocal-everything
points to
a culmination on G. However, this goar is not reached immediately.
Allegretto
(10) (15) Instead the
melody falls back to the D, which is weak and unaccented
on the primary revel.
our sense of the rhythmic organization is revised once again. Ir, ,.trorp""t
both
t. l-Jrv -r,. ,l,t r" l6tf ,=i':-\1+ the primary and secondary levels now appear to have been
trochaic.
g vr The second phrase of this period is an extension by repetition
1. ' of the final end-
, accented grouping (on the lowest level) of the first phrase,
EXAMPLE 1OO
with the F-naturar
changed to F-sharp. This phrase is unmistakably iambic
or, ,h. subprimary level.
notice that the Ffowever, because all the groups on this level are the
dominant-tonic chord progression which shapes this grouping' differentiation between units-there is no basis for grouping
same-because there is no
This is in part
beginning of the ,..oni phrase-the F-receives a strong accent. on higher levels.
rn short, the rhythmic organization on both revers r and'z is incomplete.
striking in re-
u i.od,r.i of the repetition across the measure and is particularly
feeling is so strong The apprehension of a beat or group of beats as accented or
duction D of this example. Indeed, the iambic, end-accented pends upon some differentiation between groups
unaccented de-
itself out and
in this abstraction that the first half-note F almost seems to separate on the level in question. where
back and look at all units are alike, there is no basis for thinklng of th.- as accented
become an anacrusis to the second phrase. And if we now turn or unaccented.
relationship is not simply an since, as defined in chapter i, a rhythm is-,,the grouping of
the original period (Example 9B), we see that this
accented beats in relation to an accented one,' (p. oi,
one or more un_
second half of the
acciderit arising from the pio.e* of reduction. By building the slch a seri.es of undif_
points up this ana- ferentiated units, lacking either an accent or a weak beat,
theme on the ,u*e fig.,.e which ends the first half, Haydn rh2thm.
will be an incomplete
crustic relationship in the theme itself'
In the analysis given in Example
on the subprimary level the first phrase of the second period (Bxample 100) -bolized 100 the incomprete rhythm has been sym-
organization of the by weak-beat signs.And since the unit is a repetitiol of o.r. which was
is rhythmically somewhat ambiguorl*. Altho.tgh the dactylic
weak in the first place (measures 10-11), this seems entirely appropriate.
individual measures is still the dominant one, it is only slightly so'
The latent But
Muic More Complex Rh2th,,mic Stnrctures 87
86 The Rh4thmic Structure of
symbolized as A-B-A; it is A-BA. The final two periods constitute a single
evenhadthefirstmemberoftheseri'esbeenStrong,thefollowingunitswould
the initial unit, in retrospect' For a impulse.
have been designated as weak. And so would
l-r a
seriesofequivalentunitsleadsthelistenertoexpectthearrivalofafocalpoint, o'- (5) (10) g g (15)
apointofrhythmicorientation.Suchapointwillbemarkedforconsciousness-
willbeaccented.A"dul*o'tanychangewillcreateaccentuationinsucha
6 t- v I t- u I v t:--gJ
situation. 4.4 _L-J
3-r v r rv-l
Thetensionsandexpectationscreatedbymeasuresll-l5areaproductof rela- L
as of the more strictly temPoral
harmonic and melodic organization as wett EXAMPLE 101
tionships'Thatis,rhythmicincompletenessissupported'and-emphasizedbothby
a'iecisive cadence on G-and by
harmonic incompleteness-the need to reach EXERCISES
delay in the arrival of the
melodic incomplete,,",, i,, the form of the four-measure
expectedG.Notice,too,thattheaccentwhichoccursinmeasure16whenthese I
becomes in retrospect an anacrusis
to the repetition of the
tensions are resolved A. Find two themes which are iambic and two which are anapestic on the highest
first period. architectonic levels. Find one which is trochaic and one which is dactylic on the
interrelated purposes' First' as we
The second phrase of this period serves two second or third level.
haveseen,bycreatingrhythmicincompletenessand.bydelayingtheresolutionof B. Analyze each of these themes on all architectonic levels.
9, it creates expectation and
the melodi"-hu.*ori'" *otio' begun ir, *.urrrr" C. Write a brief essay discussing: (1) how the higher levels are created in each case-
whether by pivoting, fusion, or pyramiding (try to use instances of each of these); (2)
therebyshapesourrhythmicexperienceofthelasttwoperiodsasatotalunit. the problems encountered in the analysis of each theme.
Second'itcontinuesthephraselengthsestablishedbyhighermetriclevelsinthe
even if the expected G had D. How would your interpretation of the rhythmic organization affect the performance
initial period. 1.o prrrifr. *atter soirewhat differently, of each of these themes? Where alternative interpretations seem possible, discuss how
arrivedinmeasurel2-whichitcouldeasilyhave'done.hadthefinalFinmeas.
would have had to be prolonged' Ilor
each alternative would affect performance.
ure 11 been written as F-sharp-the phrase
would be irregular and.the feel- II
without measures l2-l6the metric organization
ingofbalanceandstabilitycreatedbythispassagewouldhavebeendisturbed. A. Analyze the Minuet (without trio) of Mozart's 'Jupiter" Symphony and Chopin's
Prelude Op. 28 No. 1. Employ the technique of rhythmic-melodic reduction where it
(ThereadercantestthisforhimselfbyimaginingtheGalarrivinginmeasure12.
for four more measures')
He will then feel the necessity or "ontirrrrirr"g thJperiod
seems useful.
be desirable in a work B. Write a brief essay about each work, discussing such matters as the articulation of
While such me*ic disturbances and irregiarities -might is more forceful and higher rhythmic levels, rhythmic inversion, rhythmic ambiguity, the influence of
character
whose proportions -. turg.. and whoseleneral temporal organization on melodic structure, and so forth.
i" a wo"'tt such as this' in which stability and
dynamic, they are out of feeping C. Choose two different recorded versions of each of these pieces. To what extent do
delicacy are desired. they differ rhythmically? How does each interpretation fit with the one you have
Becausethesecondhalfofthisperiodfailstogiverisetoagrouping^onthe made?
primarylevel,no,t,y.t'*i"organizationiscreatedonlevels2or3either.Group-
on lower levels' As a III
ing can arise on these levels ottty ir there is differentiation A. Write
an independent, cohesive rhyth- five phrases or periods such that the highest architectonic levels exemplify each
result, the period as a whole does not constitute of the five rhythmic groups.
not related to each other directly'
mic unit. Unlike the first period, its parts are B. Write three melodies, one using a pivot, one using fusion, and one using pyramiding
TheyarerelatedtoeachotherbecausetheybothfunctionaSanacrusestothe to unite the highest architectonic level.
followingperiod'Thus,asindicatedinExamplel0l,whichisareductionofthe C. Using the Haydn trio analyzed in this chapter as a model, write a trio of your own
indivisible, cohesive' anapestic group'
whole trio, the final two periods are one invention which is similar on the highest level but has a different melody and rhyth-
Theseobservationsmakeitclearthat,eventhoughthefirstperiodofthis mic organization on lower levels.
t th"t-pu't form' The form cannot be
piece ends on the tonic, this is not
Rhytlan and Meter 89
the earlier triple meter continues in the minds and motor responses of the
audience as well as of the performer, making the music seem somewhat strained
a hemiole-the new meter is more like g (3 x *) than like the previous
RHYTHM -like
organization of X (2 X *).In this connection the reader should consult Example
s6 (p. 81).
AND When we come to the interpretation and analysis of earlier music, the bar
line is even less reliable as a clue to metric organization. Before the seventeenth
century the bar line did not have the metric significance it later acquired. And
ME,TE.R earlier still, during the Middle Ages and the Renaissance, there were no bar
lines at all. Hence the placement of the bar line in most modern editions has been
determined by the editor of the work, not by the composer. often such editors,
one'- On the one hand' the barring the music in the light of nineteenth- and twentieth-century attitudes, have
The interaction of rhythm and meter is a complex done violence to musical stylistic sense.
temporal relationships, melodic
objective organizatio; ;i; piece of music-the The use of the bar line by modern composers has not been uniform. some
forth-creates accents and weak
and harmonic structure, dynamics' and so composers use bar lines in the traditional way, to mark the beginning of metric
relationships' And these accents and un-
beats (unaccents) u,'J atn"t' their units. But in much of this music the metric crossing of voices is such a common
accents,whentheyo""'.r.*ittsomeregularity,wouldseemtospecifythemeter' meter' And as occurrence that while the bar line will indicate the meter of one voice, it will
In this sense the elements which produce rhythm 95'.changes
also
_produce not do so for another. Indeed, composers have written the meters of different
91' and' in melodic' har-
we saw in conneqti;;i; E*u*plt' 17' voices with different time signatures (as Bartdk does in his String euartet No. 3,
in metric changes'
monic, and temporal reiationships may result measures 370-80), made the beams of series of eighth- or sixteenth-notes run
Ontheotherhand,metercanapparentlybeindepenflntofrhythm'notonly across bar lines, and used many other notational devices to indicate something
any dlfinitive rhythmic organiza-
in the sense that it can exist in the absence of about the metric structure. Other composers seem currently to be using the bar
orgartizaiion can conflict with and work
tion, but also in the sense that rhythmic line to mark off the limits of melodic, harmonic, or rhythmic groups rather than
against an establisheJ meter. Thus,
for instance, beats which might become
to designate the beginning of metric units. fn general the bar line has become
accents(potentialu""",'t,;orwhichactuallyareaccentedmaybeatoddswith less and less indicative of metric organization. And it must not be considered
Conversely' beats which for
the accentul ,"fr.*. esiubli'nea in the meter' conclusive evidence of the metric scheme.
naturally be weak may be forced
melodic, hur*oni", o-' oit"' "u'ons would
because of the meter to become accents.
while such conflicts of natural rhythmic
NON-CONGRUENCE
groupswithmetricstructureconstitutedisturbanceswhichtendtomodify
in a change of meter' Rather they may
grouping, they neeJ tot """t*'urily result LATENT METER AND RHYTHMIC STRUCTURE
beats or forced accentuation'
iroarr"J.ither stressed weak Sometimes the particular organization of a theme or passage makes two dif-
ferent interpretations of the metric structure possible. One cannot be certain at
first whether, for instance, the meter is duple or triple, though once a decision is
METER AND THE BAR LINE made (perhaps on the basis of subsequent developments), the resulting metric
rAwordofcautionSeemsinorderhere.Eveninthemusicoftheseventeenth
lines do not always ac-
structuring will be neither vague nor ambiguous.
[ -ul,,".i'in] i".i..nth centuries time signatures and bar Such an equivocal melodic-rhythmic organization is presented by the opening
them
I :,::::; ;"il;';;;al metric organizalion. At times composers have used -
theme of the second movement of Mozart's 'Jupiter" symphony. The theme
to interpret
I :ffifi.,;;j;-rs a co've'i-ence-relying upon the performer might have been written in duple meter, as shown in Example 102. This looks
For instance, though the last move-
and communicate the true metric structure. convincing. The opening beat functions well as an upbeat, the stressed offbeats
piu.ro Concerto in A Minor is written in f, time throughout'
ment of Schumaoni, come in the right place, and so on. And although the meter becomes manifestly
is so strongly duple on the primary level
the melody which enters at measure 80 triple in the following measures (see Example 103), the theme could conceivably
the metric structure. While
that the time signature no longer really ,"p..,.,,t*

88
g0 The Rh2thmic Structure of Music Rlythm and Meter 97

have been written in duple meter throughout.


Certainly a latent duple meter is accents are forced to act as weak beats. They constitute an urgent anacrusis and
very strong in these first measures'
their potential is realized in the heavily accented D, in which is released the
(Example 103), definitely in cumulative potential of the earlier tones. Coming, as it does, on the first really
However, the melody is, as Mozart makes clear
triple, not in duple -.,... Nor should the theme be thought of or played as satisfactory downbeat thus far, the D would have been accented even had it
for this' From a historical been marked "piano" (cf. Example I37, measure 280).
metrically ambiguous or vague. There are two reasons
with its typically heavy second All this is lost if the theme is thought of and performed in a dominantly duple
foi", of view, th"e ,nftnm is"that of a sarabande, is normally precise, it would meter. For the A in measure 1 is then an accented goal-a point of stability-
beat (see Example sito, p 40). since this rhythm
Trom the point of view of
be stylistically wrong ,o pttio'* it ambiguously'
rather than a tone imbued with the necessity for motion. So too are the B-flat in
articulated. For if measure 3 and the C in measure 6. And the importance of the D is so minimized
internal structure, too, the triple meter shiuld be decisively
or dominate the manifest that it almost constitutes an anticlimax.
the latent duple orgurrirutio., i, p.r*itted to obscure
(5) 3 Measures 5 and 6 have not yet been discussed. Melodically they perform two
Andante Cantabile
functions. At first they act to complete and fill in most of the structural gaps
created in the earlier measures. And at the end they make manifest an intervallic
p p
p
EXAI\4PLE 102

(5)
Andante Cantabile aJb
IHffi --_-__-u

qng q!_r T4w


4- c.
al ffi
ts
r^l ))ta EXAMPLE 104
I ,Tl--7- I

motion present, but hidden, in the earlier measures. That is, the unheard interval
r-,J'-v t-t J r- '6' r-t t4 of a fourth from F (measure 1) to B-flat (measure 2) in the first group and from
v- J G to C in the second group is continued and made patent to the ear in the skip
from A to D (measures 6-7).
EXAMPLE 103
Metrically measures 5 and 6 are less decisive in feeling than the earlier meas-
ures. They constitute a continuous and progressive anacrusis to the accented D-
ri

that is, each group seems to be an upbeat to the next, as the accented part of a
triplemeter,themeaningandcharacter,notonlyofthethemeitselfbutofthe
weakened.
i

are considerably beat becomes weak. This effect occurs partly because each group is faster than
whole movement u, J.rr."loping out of the theme,
As is often the case, ttre rfrlitfrmic organization
of this passage can be better the one which follows. In order to insure this feeling of progressive anacruses,
The theme establishes three care must be taken in performance not to stress the C in measure 6 but rather
understood if its melodic structure is made clear'
to make the two eighth-notes move smoothly, as an anacrusis, into the two
separable,thoughinterrelated,strandsofmelodicmotion.Thesearelabeled
104' The most important of these quarter-notes, which are themselves anacrustic to the D.
a, b, and.c in the analysis given in Example
clearly strand a, which In short, measures 5 and 6 are a proximate anacrusis to measures 7 through
strands-the one that ieaas the ear most forcefully-is
10-a kind of parenthetical upbeat-while measures 1 through 4 are the real,
-o,,.,upstepwisefromAtoB.flattoCtoD.Mozartemphasizestheimportance though remote, anacrusis to the second part of the theme, which begins on the
ofthislinewith","pp'..i"gaccompaniment,labeleda,,whichmovesintenths
with it. But as indicated in Example 104, as well as in
the rhythmic analysis accented D. This is not mere speculation. Mozart makes the anacrustic character
are not accents. They are of the first four measures clear in the coda (measures 92tr.), where the par-
given in Example ro:,,t.r. vitai melodic tones
stressed weak beats whlch seek to become accents'
And the sense of restrained enthetical anacrusis is omitted and the real anacrusis, the basic melodic strand
the melody communicates is the result of the
fact that potential A-B-flat-C, moves directly to the D.
tension which
Rlrytlm and Meter 93
92 Ttu Rh4thmic Strntture of Music
self uses almost-the same melody in duple meter in the final movement of his
duple meter, placing con-
It should be clear from this analysis how the latent Symphony No. 5 (measures 319 ff.).
siderablestressuponweakbeats,shapesthesenseandcharacterofthetheme. The strained striving of these first measures is rather the result of the duple
Butaswasobservedearlier,thelatentdupleorganizationofthethemealsohas accompaniment being so placed that potentially weak beats are forced to become
importantconsequenceslaterinthemovement.underitsinfluence'sotospeak' accents. Thus if the theme were heard in duple meter, it would naturally fall
duplemetercontinuallybreaksthrough.thebasictriplemeterofthemovement. into three end-accented groups. We would want to accent the note which follows
23-25)' in the
It is manifest, for "*u*pft, in the bridge passage-(measures the skip. This normal group is in fact established by the opening iamb, which
(measures 32-37), in the development sec-
second half of the subordinaie theme seems quite relaxed. And had the accompaniment coincided with the iambic
tion(measures5t_59),andintherecapitulation(measures6T_T0andmeasures groups, as in Example 1066, the result would have been very ordinary. On the
g0_g6). Each of these passages is an lnstance of a clear duple
meter written other hand, considered in triple meter, without the conflicting accompaniment,
half of the subordinate theme the tune would easily fall into freely moving amphibrachs, as indicated by the
within a $ bar scheme.^To illustrate, the second
is given in ExamPle 105' brackets above the example. It is of some moment to realize that the tune is
potentially amphibrach, because this enables one to understand the tremendous
Antlante Cantabile
feeling of freedom and release which measure 3 creates, where the suppressed
Ag
amphibrach breaks through.
Wrltten The analysis given below Example 106a indicates (and Beethoven corroborates
this with his rests and phrasing) that the meter of the accompaniment is to be
taken seriously; it must dominate the natural meter of the melodic line. The
As it
sounds second and third groups are to be performed as trochees on the primary level.
Notice how important it is to hear and perform the first group as an iamb. For
\-/ I \-/ -l
I -l

only then do the power of the rhythmic reversal from iamb to trochee and the
EXAMPLE 105
strained accentuation make themselves forcefully felt. If the first group is heard
as a trochee or if later groups are allowed to relax into a swinging triple meter,
Allegro Vivace-
a. t" -l b. the whole effect is weakened.
The instability of this uncomfortable conflict between the natural accentua-
tion of the melody and the metric accents of the accompaniment (harmony is
tr
1.
constant) makes the trochees act as stressed weak beats on the second archi-
2.
tectonic level. And the whole dactylic group (level 2), stiving for stability, acts
3. as an anacrusis to the second half of the phrase.
EXAIvIPLE 106
The function of the conflict between meter and melody thus becomes clear. It
creates the strident, striving character of the theme and at the same time welds
FORCED ACCENTUATION AND RHYTHMIC
STRUCTURE the whole phrase into a single dynamic impulse toward a goal Without the con-

In the Mozart just discussed' tones which might have been (one is
theme
flict, the resulting amphibrachs would have been pleasantly dancelike at best,
are constrained to act as weak and the character of the impulse on the third architectonic level would have
tempted to say "should iave been") accented
No' 4- (Example l06a)' the re- been much less forceful and cohesive.
beats. In the Scherzo of Beethoven's Symphony Although not always as striking as in Examples 103 and 106a, instances of non-
verse of this takes place-that is, a tone
*ii"tt should have been a weak beat is
congruence between meter and melody are quite common in music literature.
forced into being accented' For instance, the rhythmic organization of the opening of the Scherzo of the
Itmightatfirstglance.seemthatthetensionofthefirsttwomeasuresarrses "Eroica" Symphony (Example 88) also depends, as we have seen, upon the
conflicts with the duple meter of the ac-
because the triple meter of the melody presence of a changing-note figure which tends to be duple in a metric scheme
companiment.Butthisisnotthebasisfortension.Themelodycouldwithout which is definitely triple.
anydifficultyhaveu"",'r,"u,aasduple(Bxample106,).Indeed,Beethovenhim-
Rlgtthm and Meter 95
94 Tlu Rhltth,mic Structure of Music
to maintain the four-note melodic pattern and change its point of accentuation
(and per-
STABLE METER AND SHIFTING
CROUPS than to keep changing the structure of each successive melodic group
not invariably produce stressed weak beats and haps its rhylnm as well), which would be the case if meter determined melodic
But non-congruence does
It may, utli*ir, cause a rhy^thmic organization organization.
forcefully directed anacruses.
whichwouldotherwisehavebeenregularandstabletofl,uctuate.Non-con. It *u* observed earlier that the melodic sequence is such that several different
gruence produces ttti'-L'i oi irregularity
i" u p*'uge in the first movement of patterns, each of which is regular, might arise. But it must be added that one se-
701a). The excerpt occurs twenty-srx qrr"rr". may create a more forcefully structured pattern, more patent to the ear,
Bruckner,s Symprrony li.. n ln"'.*pf.
Eulenburg miniature score.' t'ha1 urroth.r. In this otherwise uniform pattern there is one obvious point of
measures after letter 7 in the
Thetonal-temporalorganizationofthispassagedoes.notcreateanunequivocal
articulation-the skip of a third-which serves to differentiate and separate the
that the
-rJp.."er,nough the *otirric"r.petition makes it clear groups. As a result, the clearest and most natural way to group these tones is that
rhythmic_melodic that any one of the notes
sequence is so of the descending scale.
melodic grouplng 's i" fo''"', the ""ifo'* Thus while metric structure determines the placement of the accent within
couldconstitutethebeginningofaregularandcontinuouspattern(seeExample
organization is empha-
inhere"i""*Uig"ity of the iorrut-t.*poral groups, melodic structure orders the length and organization of the groups.
t[lb).The
I{o*.u.r, because the melodic repetition in fours is not congruent with the
Z6gernd
metric recurrence in threes, the position of the accent within the melodic pattern
ltr
-J--r T-t--T-t I tttttt': keeps changing. The result, as can be seen from the analysis given in Example
w ??r
I
'

tr -.- w
T
r_\
-l
-T.98
-,

a. PP :| (271
I I I I

ffi allmiihllch mehr rurd mohr


gebunden
----E-
r-Ul l-vl
t
l-VVl r- vl
lvv-ltv-vl
l=l/.j l- v v
v
I

EXAMPLE 108

3.
4
1
2. 108, is a continual shifting of both the structure and proportions of the rhythmic
groups.
b. - As the reader listens to this passage, he will observe that the irregularity of the
EXAMPLE 107
rhythm can be maintained (in the last four groups) despite the fact that it is,
as Bruckner indicates, to be played "more and more legato." This is possible for
sizedbythecounterpointinthelower,strings,whichplaythesamepattern point' Notice' two reasons. First, because the same pitches are repeated, we are more aware
against that of tn. nli "iolins'
that i" btgi"ii"g ?t^: different of their changing function in the rhythm. Take the tone E, for instance' At first
triplets do in fact
incidentally, that in the actuai
passage iB'"ut"pfL 107.r]-:1.
it is a weak afterbeat in a dactyl group (see * in Example 108), which itself acts
placeaccentsso'thateachofthepattern,.i,,E*u*p|e|0Tbisbroughtout.That as an upbeat to the next measure; then the E becomes an anacrusis, and finally
*u'k' the beginning of pattern 1'
(;;"ntingin tf"ttt it acts as an accent. Or looked at the other way around: on the lowest architec-
is, the first metnc
"t;; *"rts thJ beginning of pattern 4, and so
forth' Because
the second metric .""""i differentia- tonic level each accent in the final measures occurs on a different pitch-first F,
of the metodic
uuorr. uil b."uir. there is no temporal then C-sharp, then D, and lastly E. In earlier measures) however, tones tended to
"*b;i;;^;;J
tiontodefineanunequivocalpattern.ofaccentsandweakbeats'atriplemeter maintain their functions because the melodic groups themselves moved in pitch.
is able to impose i,t;il;p;; the weakty duple melodic organization' Thus in measures 27 and 28 (see Example 107) the F is the only accented note'
^*eter able to impose its accentual scheme upon the
Flowever, though ihe is Second, group differentiation is more marked in measures 29 and 30 than in
That is' the
maintains its identity as a pattern'
melody, the melodic"s-;p ,,ilr within the pattern' earlier ones because the first note (F) of later groups does not belong to the same
of the accents and weak beats
meterdetermines d ;i;";nt mind tends harmony as the final note (C-sharp) of the preceding group. Up to these last
not make tiJro"'-"ote unit i";;;;t;-note one' Since the four groups, the final note of one scale and the fi.rst note of the next could be
but does for the mind
simplest *"V pt*iUfe' it is easier
to perceive u., o,guttilutio" i" the
gO Rfuitlm and Meter 97
Ttu Rtythmic Strutture of Music
making the transition be- mine the existing meter, making the metric organization seem uncertain. The
understood as belonging to the same harmony, thus stressed offbeats in Example 109, for instance, create a sense of metric insecurity.
tween groups smooth and less marked' Where there is no melodic, temporal, or other differentiation at all, offbeat
dactylic rhythm,
This ease of transition from group to group makes the latent stresses tend, when they are regular, to determine the meter and, when they
two measures and in the
which pure meter would imposl, qrrit. titottg in the first are irregular, to destroy all sense of meter (see Example 1 1 1).
And insofar as
similar measures which precede ihi. "*."rpt in the movement'
passage' they would' as
such dactylic groups influence our perception of this SHIFTING METER AND STABLE GROUPS
noted .urii.r, iend to make melodic structure seem
irregular'
There is then a danger of oversimplification. Actually, until the final measures In general, the more uniform and unorganized (not disorganized) a passage
both- these is, the more susceptible it is to outside influences-to melodic or temporal pat-
of this passage *" urJ aware-though ttot perhaps consciously-of
and both must be ac- terns in other parts of the musical texture, and to stress. Indeed in the absence of
*od., of org-uni"ution. Both play a pirt in o,rr e*petience
any objective basis for metric organization, the mind will tend to impose its own
counted for in our analYsis'
meter is not simply a meter upon the series of undifferentiated pulses.
This discussion points to an important fact, namely, that
matter of regularly recurring dynamic intensification. It
is a set of proportional Whether such subjective groupings are determined by the nature of human
beats within which mental processes or whether they arise out of the listener's stylistic habits and
relationship*, u' orl.rirrg f;*t*ork of accents and weak
out of which rhythm
rhythmic grouping takes-place. It constitutes the matrix Allegsetto
Scherzando
arises.
(Example 108) would
Thus the rhythm and hence the meaning of the passage ::-ffi
E=E
with a stress on every
have been very different had Bruckner written it in fours V _E ----
PP -E
sempre st.au'nlo
third beat (ExamPle 109). 1r .:1 .:::\ Collit --
lr !-Jl-Jlv -llu,-ll.I/Elv -l ii. rV -llV -llV
l. | - v i. u= v.t
r-rtt- J' l-wll- Y I r- Jrr- . t t- tt 1.r-r2rt-V:
I
1. \t
r- V I r- t2 t r-Vll-vl
2.
EXAMPLE 109 EXAMPLE 110

determine meter, or dispositions is a question which requires further study. Probably both play a part.
Which comes first, the chicken or the egg? Does rhythm
upon the.point from which the But whatever the reason, it appears that in our culture, at least for the past few
is it the other way around? The answer depends
organization to be centuries, such a series of pulses tends to be mentally organized into a duple
process is viewed. For the composer, the rhythmic-melodic
the meter indicated meter. Any fair sampling of the metric organizations used in Western music will
projected determines the meter chosen. For ihe performer,
in of itself determine, the possi- show a clear preponderance of duple over triple organization. For while meter
by the composer limits, though it does not and
well as for the Iistener is frequently triple on lower metric levels, it is seldom so on higher ones, as an
Uiti,i", of grouping. For both composer and performer-as which
and weak beats examination of the examples in this book will show.
establishes a structured lontinuum of accents
-meter
acts as a basis for rhythmic and melodic expectation;
that is, it becomes a norm Thus when the ear is presented with a series of pulses such as one finds at the
and felt' beginning of the second movement of Beethoven's Symphony No. 8, it tends to
in the light of which both the regular and irregular are apprehended
does not organize the beats into two ftrur-pulse units (Example 110). And when the
It has from time to time been emphasized that stress on a weak beatrecurring melody enters, this subjective organization is confirmed. (Of course the con-
reason' even regularly
make the beat into an accent and that, for this
And as long as melodic ductor with prior knowledge of Beethoven's notation and of the subsequent
stresses will not change a given metric organization.
such that strongly shaped melodic and rhythmic organization may project this grouping in performance.
articulation, temporallifferentiation, and so forth are But such an objective articulation does not seem desirable here. The series should
shapes are ambiguous
patterns are created, this rule holds. But when melodic be played uniformly.)
weak beats may under-
and temporal differentiation is absent, strongly stressed
Rhythm and Meter 99
98 T-he Rhlthmic Structure af Music
the meter- of duple meter. They are felt as regular offbeats, and had they continued
It might at first appear that the rhythm of the melody is against sense

that is, as indicated Uy tft. brackets over the example'


that the rhythm consists in measure 5, the meter would not have been weakened one whit.
The metric organization begins to seem insecure with the stress on the first
ofananapestwiththedottedeighth-note,F,aStheaccent.Thisisplausiblebe- if
to be short-short{ong' Flowever' beat of measure 6. This happens for two reasons. (1) Because special stress seems
cause the temporal organization"would seem
carefully and are re- appropriate to and has thus far occurred only on weak beats, the stressed accent
the melodic-temporal relationships are considered more
F is not a goal' There has feels misplaced-wrong. It is as though the accent were not really an accent but
duced to their essenti;ls, it is clear that the eighth-note
it. (This analysis is supported a weak beat. (2) The number of pulses from accent to accent is no longer regular
been no melodic or harmonic movement toward
temporal distribution and normal for a duple meter. That is, instead of stresses being separated by two
by the fact that ut *"u,u,. 9 [see score], where the same
is imbued with a clearly directed melodic motion from B.flat up to E.flat, or four pulses which would fit the meter, the stress on the first beat of measure 6
E-flat-from becom- occurs after three pulses. And because the recurrence of stress continues to be
Beethoven is careful to prevent the goal of the motion-the
ins an accent by introducing sy"copation') The rhythm
is in fact a trochee with irregular-coming after three pulses, then four, and finally five (see brackets
mind of above Example 111)-our sense of metrie organization is almost completely
;^ffiil;;;fr1, -J ;. inl *"u;.ctive organization imposed bv the
organization by mak- destroyed.
the listener on the opening measure serves to support this
ing the meter begin on the first pulse of the measure'
Althoughthepartialphraseinthecelloscorrespondswiththeopeningmeter, STNCOPATIONS, SUSPENSIONS, AND TIES
like that
when it stops in the middle of measure 4 the wind accompaniment DEFINITIONS
of the meter in relation
which began the movement establishes a new placement
and that of the The passage from The Rite oJ Spring raises an important question of definition
to the bar lines' Both the meter of the pul,e accompaniment
and terminology. Such stressed weak beats are often referred to as "syncopa-
themenowbeginonthesecondhalfofthemeasure,ignoringthebarlines.The
tions." But there is a vast difference between the rhythmic and metric organiza-
original,..correct,,placementofthemeterisresumed,afteranotherpartial
phrase, in measure 8-
tion of Example 1tr 1 and the organization which results if the same chords are
played with the following temporal relationships:
(5)
THE INFLUENCE OF UPON METER
,STRE,S,S 'i ITn [Tnl JUJJJ-J-I] I trl-J Fn I F1? Iil,,l l, il
The Rite of-
The beginning of the "Dances of the Adolescents" from stravinsky's In this version the sense of meter is nol weakened. Rather, by suppressing the
of stress upon a series of
spring provides an interesting instance of the influence downbeats, the ties create strong anacruses to measures 5, 6, and 9, and make the
pulses (Example 111). first beats of those measures strongly accented. Since the rhythmic-metric organ-
(13) )= s6

q
Horns./
fr
1

v--- >>-- -
T

-|.---{-i---t-
V'Faqq
1
lttt
v7 7q7
r g

VTrt
't11
r
+.
1

w-='
'1 '1

-
ization and the effect of these two versions are so very different, it would seem
that only confusion can result from calling both stressed weak beats and ties
t'syncopations."
il vJ---- Let us take another instance. Both the Haraard Dictionary of Musicr and Groue's
(5)
f r-r'Tl t--tq
)a)) -
l-TT: -=
.)-lllri).) ) j-;-]: ^-rr: Dictionary of Music and Musicians2 cite the opening measures of the third movement

>
ffi -l- of Brahms's Symphony No. 4 as an example of syncopation. However, though
the phrasing creates stressed weak beats which modify the groupings on the
lowest level, making them end-accented (Example ltZa) rather than beginning-
EXAMPLE 111
accented (Example 1l2b), this is very different from the effect and the rhythmic
organization created by genuine syncopation (Example ll2c).
Attheoutset,themetricorganization,whetherimposedbythernindo-ftheis How then shall syncopation be defined? As employed in this book, the term
listener or by the articulation 6f ,fr" orchestra, is clearly
in duple meter' Nor
3. Instead, these I Willi Apel, op. c;t., p.726.
this impresrior, *euk.rred by the stressed offbeats in measure
stresses, coming u* Jy.ru*i" intensifications most frequently do,
confirm our ' (3d ed.; New York: Macmillan Co., 1936), V,242.
100 Tlu Rhltthmic Structure of Music Rhythm and Meter 101

dissonant. For not only are such tones similar to harmonically activated sus-
"syncopation" refers to a tone which enters where there is no pulse on the pri-
pensions in their general configuration and placement within the metric scheme,
mary metric level (the level on which beats are counted and felt) and where the
following beat on the primary metric level is either absent (a rest) or suppressed but because they are tied from a weak pulse they are imbued with a strong need
(tied). It follows from this that whether there is syncopation or not depends to move and hence feel like and act like those suspensions which actually become
dissonant.
upon how the beat or pulse continuum is felt and hence upon the tempo of the
Suspensions must be distinguished from ties. A "tie," as the term will be used
piece as well as upon the performer's articulation of the meter. If the tempo is too
here, denotes a tone which enters on an accent rather than on a weak beat.
slow or if the performer overarticulates lower metric levels, the effect of synco-
This means that the word "suspension" will be used in two senses. First, it will
pated notes may be weakened. Or if the tempo is too fast, what should be a
designate a tied note which enters on a weak beat. As is the case in Example 113a,
higher metric level is felt to be the primary metric level, and notes not intended
this note need not become a dissonance. Second, it denotes a tied note which
to be syncopated become so.
actually becomes a dissonance. Thus in Example 114, excerpts from the same
Allegro glocoso b.
aria in Messiah are used to illustrate: (a) a suspension which does not become dis-
sonant, (D) a suspension which does become dissonant, (t) u simple tie, and (d) 4
w wt
tie which becomes a suspension (dissonance) in the second sense of the term.
!=gJl- w I t-Jtt -
tJ -trri - ttu vl
ll---J t,- v ll- a. (14) b
EXAMPLE 112

Andante b. Allegro
shall be (58)
.l.l

#-
Andante d
c.

c. (?0) d. (20)
1
EXAMPLE 113
*Lt#F
Thus the B in the opening motive from the aria "Ev'ry Valley" from Handel's a
rough pla-ces plaln shallbe ------ed
Messiah (Example 113a) is zol syncopated because at the andante tempo it comes
on a pulse, albeit a weak one. However, the variants which follow are syn.co'
pated. In the first of these the time is cut and the tempo faster and because we
count the measure in twos rather than in fours, the B does not come on a beat- EXAMPLE 114

even a weak one. In variants c and c'both the G-sharp and the B are syncopated, Thus the difference between a tie, a suspension, and a syncopation lies in their
The importance of the suppression of the downbeat B in the second measure of placement in relation to metric pulses. A tie comes on a strong beat within the
the variants is obvious. As the reader can test for himself, if the B is sounded or meter. In a compound meter it enters at the beginning of either half of the
heard as a separate impulse, there is no effect of syncopation. If we now turn measure: in f, time, it may begin on either the first beat or the third; in g time
back to Example 111, we must revise our analysis somewhat. While the strings on either the first pulse or the fourth; and so forth. A suspension begins on a weak
playing continuous pulses are not syncopated, the horns, which play only off- pulse within the metric scheme. A syncopation begins where there is no beat on
beats, are syncopated (see also Beethoven, Grosse Fuge, Examples 156-58' pp. the primary metric level.
164-66).
If the note B in Example ll3a is not a syncopation, what is it? Unfortunately STRESS AND SYNCOPATION.
we have no unambiguous term for this. However, rather than introduce a new
term we shall broaden the use of the term "suspension" to include tones which One of the things which all musicians will have noticed is that there is a
marked tendency to perceive or perform syncopated notes as emphasized even
enter on a weak pulse and are held over, even if they do not become literally
702 Tlu Rhythmic Structure of Music Rhythm and Meter 703

though such emphasis is not marked by he composer.s Perhaps this is intuitively beginning of measure 2 is suppressed and the rhythm is fused together by the
done to compensate for the fact that there is no pulse at that point-to give the syncopation across the bar. The suppression of this accent makes all the beats,
note a "beat status" which it does not really have. In any case, because emphasis beginning with the B-flat in measure 1, anacrustic to the last G of the group and
and syncopation seem to belong together in this way, it is possible to make a its afterbeat, D. However, the first G is also accented, and one may ask why the
suspension feel like a syncopation by adding stress to it. Thus if the B in Example whole group is not a trochee or even perhaps a dactyl. In order to understand
113a or the E in Example ll4a ate stressed, they will feel like syncopations. the structure and behavior of this group, it is necessary to analyze the melodic
These observations call attention to a function of stress not yet considered, and harmonic structure of the phrase as well as its temporal organization.
namely, that it may be used not to change grouping but to modify the character Whenever an accent is suppressed, as it is in this case, the mind, searching for
of a rhythmic group. It is this function of stress which Schumann exploits in the the focal stability of an accent with reference to which it can group weak beats,
Scherzo of his Symphony No. 4 (Example 115). places particularly stress on the subsequent downbeat. Furthermore, the stronger
The inverted amphibrach and inverted trochees in measures 8-14 are not the potential of the unrealized accent-the stronger it would have been had it
syncopations. No pulse is missing or suppressed. Since the tempo is fast, the pri- not been suppressed-the more effective the syncopation and the more forceful
mary-metric level moves in dotted half-notes, and hence the weak part of the the impulse toward the next accent.
Allegretto
trochee is followed by a pulse. The function of the sfess is to make this part of (5)

the group feel like a syncopation even though it is not one.


(10) (15)
Lebhaft (5) tv -l lv -l
2.
3, :z
l. tv -vilv -vl r-J slmllar tw - ,itt- rirr- (rr- ,7rr- 6tr-,Jtu4!-l EXAMPLB 116
\-/ r- (rz)t L -
v rr v I
1.r
2.
-
I
:
----Y:Jl - Allegro molto
3.
4.
BXAMPLE 115
1.tv -|lU -l lU
-!rU
2.I v | rvr
Actually in this case the fact that the stress comes on an inverted group gives 3.
-
it such power that the rhythmic-metric scheme is almost upset; the stress hovers 4.

on the brink of becoming an accent. As a consequence of this, the G-sharp in EXAMPLB 117

measure 15 seems strained. If the metric placement were changed so that the
In this case the suppressed beat is crucial because had it been presented it
half-notes became accents and the eighth-notes became upbeats, making the
would have organized not only the primary architectonic level but the second
rhythm iambic, then the G-sharp would itself become a syncopation. And though
and third levels as well. If the melodic-rhythmic organization had been allowed
this does not take place, it comes so close to happening that we feel the tension
to follow its initial impulse, it would have continued upward through the triad
anyhow.
to an accented high D, which would be a natural point for the completion of the
upward thrust begun in the first measure. This is shown by the motion of the
SYNCOPATION AND MELODIC-RHYTHMIC STRUCTURE oboes and clarinets, which move through the G minor triad in just this way and
Although the role of syncopation is sometimes primarily that of characterizing by the motion of all the other instruments.to D at the beginning of measure 2.
a theme or a passage, more often it performs a crucial function in shaping the Had the melody reached an accented high D, the rhythm of the initial two-
whole pattern of musical progress. And this is precisely what it does in the first measure unit would have been iambic and that of the phrase might have been
periodtf the Minuet of Mozart's Symphony in G Minor (K. 550; Example 116). trochaic. The missing D is therefore the tone which would have been accented
The initial group is three measures long. The primary metric level is that of on all levels. This can perhaps be more clearly seen if the Minuet from Schubert's
the measure. On the lowest rhythmic level the accent which should arrive at the Symphony No. 5 in B-flat is considered along with the Mozart Minuet. For the
s SeeH. E. Weaver, "syncopation: A Study in Musical Rhythms," Journal oJ Gencral PsYcholog, Schubert Minuet begins exactly like the Mozaft one but continues in a much
xx (1939), 409-29. more normal and regular way (Example 117).
104 Ttu Rhlttmic Structure of Music Rfuithm and Meter 105
its goal on the high D
Here the melodic motion begun in measure 1 reaches quence. It is as though the pent-up energy of the two initial accents in measures 3
immediately, and the second iamb is accented. This accent
in turn contributes and 6 had been released. However, although the impulse created by this new
to the organization of the second and third architectonic levels. syncopation still places a stress upon the accent which follows it, the force carried
ThuswhatissuppressedintheMozartisnotsimplyalowerarchitectonicac. over is no longer concentrated on a single accent but is distributed-even dissi-
for the organization of
cent but the beat which might have been the focal point pated-over three similarly placed accents. And as the passage unfolds, the
the total structure. This has several consequences.
(1) It- places an unusually equality of the parts of the sequential series makes even the group ending on F
interruption
i.urry stress upon the weak beat, B-flat; the performer feeLs the seem weak in retrospect. In other words, though each anapest in the series is
strongly and compensates for it by emphasizing the B-flat. (2) It makes the fol- clearly shaped and differentiated, the groups themselves are so equal that none
that the
io*irig'u"".nt on G so powerful and the whole shape so end-accented is able to serve as the focal accent around which the units themselves can be
(3) It shapes the
motion of this impulse is brought to an unequivocat tr-at-t. grouped. What is needed is a clearly organized accented group to which this
for the denial of the expected D makes
f"ogr.* of the subsequent melodic motion, whole theme is a search for a D which sequence can be related.
the need for it even greater. In a sense the The necessity for such a group and the importance of its particular organiza-
is not forthcoming until the
comes on a strongly'accented beat-and this tone tion can perhaps best be illustrated by writing several alternative endings for this
final note of the melody (see Example 1 18) ' theme. Pafi a of Example 119 is unsatisfactory for several reasons. First, by
(5)
Allegretto a. b.
-C-

f
3

(10)

1. tv u -l
2. EXAMPLE 119
2a
3.
simply continuing the series, it ends on a very weak group-though the listener
EXAMPLE 118
will probably do his best to make the ending seem strong. Second, the melody
one should
Lastly, in discussing the anacrustic character of this group, -not now ends on the tonic. This melodic step has already received much emphasis
neglect the importar"". of the harmony in articulating
the rhythrn' The weight and now constitutes a feeble anticlimax. But, even more important, the end is
by the fact
urri ,"rrd.n"y Lf tn B-flat toward the G are considerably increased weak harmonically. For this phrase, having moved away from the tonic har-
chord' mony to a tonal area around D, now simply collapses back to where it started
that it is treated as part of a Fi progression leading to the tonic
way as the first.
The second phrase (^.urrrrl'+-oi i, constructed in the same from without any real tonal departure. Third, as we have seen, the first part of
Minuets is ap-
Here another difference between the schubert and the Mozart the theme sets up the tone D as an important goal. And part a oI the example
in the relation of antecedent to con- leaves this motion unfulfilled. Finally, and perhaps most important of all, the
pu".rrr. Because Schubert's two phrases are
not the with the final phrase is now regular relative to the preceding ones: the structure of the
i"q,..rrt, they establi*fr u "o*pt"ied rhythmic unit. This is case
rhythmically'
Mozart Minuet. Here the phr.r", are harmonically as well as theme as a whole is 3-3-6. After the strained, compact structures of the two
heard as upbeats earlier groups such flaccid regularity is a vapid anticlimax.
parallel. As a result, once the second group is begun, both are
,o *o*" expected longer group which will constitute an accent' It is this regularity of phrase structure which constitutes the major flaw in
like the fi'rst one, parts D and c of this example. Only by stretching the length-by weakening the
Though melodicalfi th"e new phrase (Example 118) begins
it is clearly set off harmonically lrom what has gone before. For it moves which away intermediate accents for the sake of the final one-can the tension of the final
E-flat triad,
from the earlier,orri" hur*oni urtd is harmonized with an
groups make them an adequate culmination of the earlier ones. This is ac-
complished after the octave transfer by extending the downward motion of the
functions as the Neapolitan second in D minor'
The end of the syncopated group now becomes the basis
for a descending se- scale passage still further (see Example 118, measures 11-12), and just at this
106 TtM Rlrytlanic Structure of lulusic
Rlrythm and Meter 707

measures 3-1 1 from the third movement of Bart<ik's Piano Concerto No.3. As
point the melodic motion becomes more uniform, moving chromatically rather
is clear from Example 122, Bartrjk intended no syncopation, The upper voice is
ihan diatonically down from the B-flat. The melodic ambiguity thus created is
barred in $ meter, and the rhythm of the upper part is trochaic on the lowest
reflected in the constant dovetailing and pivoting of rhythmic groups' Thus
the
from architectonic ievel.
return to clarity in measure 13 is indeed impressive. And the accent, arising
Both the trochees comprising the two-measure group are "abnormal." The
a return to melodic, harmonic, and rhythmic certainty after the suspense and
of the
doubts of the preceding measures, is strong enough to carry the burden
conclusion'
first (
S I is inverted and the second ( ;|'l ;,-U..u,rr. of temporal differ-
previous intensity a.rd to bring the whole theme to a satisfactory entiation, "wants" to become end-accented. There is a tendency, that is, for the
weak final eighth-note to become an upbeat to the following measure. To prevent
METRIC CROSSING this from happening, the preceding accent must be somewhat stressed.
As we noted earlier, the meter of a piece of music-the way it is barred- Allegro vivace
indicates and limits, though it does not necessarily specify, the possibilities
of
rhythmic organization. Of course when a composition is in a familiar style whose I

musical ,ror*, have become part of our habit responses, barring does not
seem etc.
(>)
group-
of crucial importance. Like punctuation in literature, it is felt to facilitate I

ing but not to determine it. Stylistic experience would in most cases enable
musicians to reconstruct the rhythmic-metric organization, even if bar lines
were
) t) ) t)
1 1 1 1
a
missing.
But if the norms of a style are in doubt or if the temporal-melodic organization EXAMPLE
is such that several groupings seem equally possible, then the absence of a
121

specified meter may creat. pioblems of interpretation whose solution


will play
for instance, Allegro vivace (b) (10)
an important role in determining rhythmic organization. suppose,
order to do so it
that we were asked to perform the melody in Example 720a'In
l.t- . vt ,2
would be necessary to decide how it is to be grouped-where the accents and t! u, tl
weak beats come. we would have to decide what the meter is.
t t

-v-J
t-vl
t-
(>),
vt
ut l-Wl

(>)
b
-i---+---- -----t---*t-
-----#
a, ---*-
,t A
I
'! I
rV -VllV -\/l 3. v
EXAMPLE 120 EXAMPLE 122*

One possible solution would be to interpret the rhythrn as a series of amphi- Our understanding of the rhythm of the upper voice depends in part upon the
brachs,ln which case the meter would be in f time, as shown in Example
1200'
counterpoint of the lower voice, which also influences our apprehension of the
However, since this melody is accornpanied by a counterpoint in other instru-
whole texture. The lower voice is in triple meter (3 X B). This is made very clear
ments, we know that this is not the correct placement for the meter' As
indicated by the repetition of the pattern. The grouping is beginning-accented. This is a
in Example l2l, therhythmic group begins on an accent, not on an upbeat. As product not only of the instrumental stress placed on the first beat of the group
it stands here, it upp.um that thi melody is syncopated within the bar against the' by the whole orchestra but also of the dynamic stress on the second beat. The
regular meter of th. accompaniment. As a result, the syncopated beats act
as
latter stress prevents the final quarter-note of the group from becoming an up-
,rpi"u,, to the following u"".rrt. Notice that this produces a polyrhythm:the beat. observe that by writing the eighth-note beams across the bar lines,
not
melody is end-accented and the accompaniment is beginning-accented-but =O it
J J, Bartdk makes clear that this is not a syncopation. Had he written
a polymeter. Both Parts are in *.
'Fo.t,rrrut.ly
in this case the comPoser put in bar lines to tell us what rhythmic * Copyright 7947 by Boosey
of & Hawkes, Ltd. Reprinted by permission.
organization he intended. The excerpt is a somewhat simplified version
708 Tlw Rh2thmic Structure of Music
Rlythm and Meter l0g
the lower part might have been thought of as syncopated and the exploratory investigation is of value. In the first place,
J this music involves
UJ, rhythmic-metric proced ures rather different from
those we have discussed thus
stresses would have had a different meaning. far: ones in which meter is more than the matrix
out of which rhythm arises. For
Because our minds tend to organize the musical texture in the simplest way, as Gombosi and Goldthwaite have shown,
metric relationships both within and
simultaneous musical events will, if possible, be apprehended as having one between voices are a fundamental shaping
force in this music. Indeed in the
basic mode of articulation. It is for this reason that the beginning-accented work to be analyzed metric alterna tion and
metric crossing are a basic facet of
grouping of the lower part is able to impose its organization upon the upper part, compositional technique. In the second place,
as will become very apparent, a
so that the upper group also ends after the second measure. Thus the upper part discussion of this music of necessity emphasizes
the ultimate dependence of in-
becomes trochaic on the second level. terpretation and performance, as well as analysis,
upon sty listic understanding.
The articulation into two-measure units is also a result of the crossing of the (1) (2) (8) (4')
upper meter, which is 2 X g, with the lower meter, which is 3 X S. This cre- (5) (6) (?)
ates what Curt Sachs has called a metric "dissonance"o (D) at the beginning Cantus

of measure 2 which is resolved to a "consonance" (C) on the first beat of measure


cDC l(.2*z)*zJla * Bl Lz* + Is * s I (2 +2)+zffz +2+zl [a + a ]
,,1, itl. This crossing groups the smaller units, the trochees of the up-
d ,r c.T
per voice, and separates larger ones, by determining the primary metric level. [a r31 [g + 3 ] Is +3 I L2 +2+t) [S+g ] [s+s ]
That is, it forces us to feel the basic meter moving from one metric consonance O

to another in two-measure units. --r--


Tgnor
Not only does the lower part give rise to this two-measure unit, but it also
articulates the structure of the phrase as a whole. Because the groups in the
1. 6 6 12+ + l 6 ?.0 Lz*z+z)
upper part are more or less similar in length and structure, they do not create (8) B. (e)
(10) (13) (L4')
unambiguous groups on higher architectonic levels. However, the final groups
of the lower part are linked by a rhythmic pivot (see analysis under Example
122). As a result the whole period, including the upper part, is apprehended as 3+
[s+ s][B + lo
a well-structured anapest.
The problems posed in the preceding discussion of the theme of the last move-
ment of Bartdk's Piano Concerto No. 3 were fictitious, first, in the sense that the [8* 3 ] [3*3 Jt3+a ][s* s] [g* 3I [3 + B]
composer had in fact written bar lines specifying the metric organization and, 6

second, in the sense that we know from stylistic experience-hearing and ex-
periencing this kind of music-how this passage should sound.
+
But these problems are not fictitious when we turn to music of the Middle +
+ +
Ages and the Renaissance. Composers then used no bar lines, and we are not at
all sure what the norms of the style really are-how this music was performed and EXAMPLE 123
how it sounded.
The work chosen to these matters is the Kyrie from Dufay,s
Since analysis of the rhythmic-metric organization of late medieval alnd salyt! Jaylr (Example'rustrate Missa
Renaissance music has only recently begun,s what follows must be considered
lz3). The text is that given in Arnord schering,s Ge-
schichte der Musik in Beispieten.. schering,s
tentative rather than definitive. However, in spite of its provisional nature, an not-e-values, however, have been
halved in order to make the metric problems
I Rh2thm antl Tempo (New York: W. W. Norton 7953), p. 41. more patent as weil as to make the
, notation conform more closely to present_day practice.
6 See Otto Gombosi,
"Machaut's Messe Notre-Dame," Musical Qyarterly, XXXVI (1950), 204-24; Let us look first at the tenor, since this voice, taken
see also his reviews in the Journal oJ the American Musicological Society, IY (1951), 139-47; YI from prainsong, generalry
served as the point of departure for the composer.
(1953), 240-43; VII (1954), 221-28. And W. Scott Goldthwaite, "Rhythmic Patterns and Formal It is divided into two large
Symmetry in the 15th Century Chanson" (Ph.D. diss., Harvard University, 1955). 6 (Leipzig: Breitlopf und H6rtel, 7931), pp.
33_34.
Muic Rhythm and Meter 111
710 The Rhltthmic Structure of
(indi- this music was performed and how these temporal relationships are to be articu-
sections (labeled A and B), each of which is in turn divided into two parts
lated. Consequently, style cannot be taken for granted. Indeed, the problem of
catedbylongbracketsbelow).Thesecondpartofeachsectionhasthesame style is precisely the one which must be solved.
temporal arrangement as the first part' Our choices between the possible alternatives depend first of all upon internal
Sections A and B differ f.om each other not only
melodically but also metrical-
meter on the evidence. For instance, where a clear structural parallelism exists between
ly. Section A, consisting of two four-measure phrases' is in duple equivocal organizations and unequivocal ones, the latter may serve to determine
phrases, is triple on the
highest level; Section Blconsisting of two three-measure
to lower levels, the metric the organization of the former. And these choices will seem the more plausible
highest level. Observe that as one moves from higher
o. is duple (2 X i) on if it can be shown that they yield a convincing and self-consistent picture of the
organization changes. Thus in the first section ih" t.t procedures of the work as a whole, making sense not only of the relationships
(3 X +); and the lowest level'
the highest level, in the next lower level it is triple
is The two within sections but also of those between sections. Such choices must, however,
againiuple (2x*). In Section B the order triple-duple-triple. also be supported by external evidence. The analysis must, that is, not only be
metric hierarchies look like this:
self-consistent; it must also be consistent with what we know about the style from
Section A Section B other studies of works of this period and style. Finally, if the analysis is correct,
(z x s/+) (s x o/a) it should be possible-although beyond the scope of this book-to relate the gen-
eral aesthetic tendencies it exhibits to the aesthetic ideals and practices of other
1

,JJJ,,JJJ, (a x z/a) J-l' (z x s/a)


arts of the early Renaissance.
ti ,4tr8 (z x t/a) ,J)),,))),ut".
- - (a x r/a) Let us begin by considering the first two measures. In the first measure the
"". cantus part is unmistakably in triple meter. The second measure is apparently in
find that although the
Turning now to the cantus and the countertenor, we duple meter; the sixteenth-note D is clearly an upbeat to the D which follows it,
duple (two-ineasure
high;r; *lrri" Ievel (labeled 1, above) in Section A is clearly and the second D is accented on the lowest level.
level ii as well, is less The countertenor is in duple meter in measures 1 and 2. Thus it creates a
units), the organization of level i, and consequently of
be thought of as being
certain. For instance, the cantus in measure 3 might metric dissonance (@T?r*') in the first measure which is resolved to a metric
either duple''(J-t tl)J , or triple' J7 n J '; similarly'
measure 7 might be in- consonance (31) i" the second measure. Since we know from the studies of
3+a ) (z*z+ 2)- Goldthwaite and others that such metric crossing is a common procedure in this
terpreted as a syncopated triple meter' - o" u' a compound (duple) style of music, the analysis given seems convincing.
I,II), The meter of the cantus in measure 3 is more doubtful. Two considerations
meter. ,)J,,J ),. Measures 3 and 5 in the countertenor are even more open
make it seem probable that it should be interpreted in triple meter. In the first
[r-;:;;)i't."pr.tutions: each coutd be either,rint.,,l tF^,or duple' place, the melodic motion is such that the E sounds like an escape-note
," ditr
(6chapp6e) moving to the A. Without this melodic inflection the grouping might
J_)JI,
(3 +31
have been duple, as it is in measure 13. Second, as we shall see, this interpreta-
matter' They will tion fits with the alternation of duple and triple meter which runs through the
The choices made between these alternatives are no minor
part but will also determine entire piece. That is, the pattern of the cantus in the first eight measures runs like
affect not only the rhythmic groupings of the cantus
the interrelationships between the voices and ultimately
the structure and mean- this:3-2 3-2 3-3-2-2.
ing of the Kyrie as a whole. The meter of the countertenor is also open to question in measure 3. But if we
r . , a familiar one with consider its relationship to the tenor, a melodic parallelism seems clear. The
On what basis can such decisions be made? Were the style
part of our parts are moving in thirds. Hence it seems plausible to regard the A as the ac-
a living tradition of performance-one whose norms had become temporal
had these
habit responses-therl would be no problem. For instance, cented note of the subgroup and to consider the C an escape-note. This analysis
would almost cer-
relationships occurred in a piece by Haydn or Mozart'.th-ey has the further advantage of being consistent with the one given for a similar type
tainly all be understood as being triple in meter (2 + 2 f
2), since patterns such
of melodic motion in the cantus. If such an interpreta tion is correct, then the
style. But in the case of
u, ,J' .hJ_J are simply not part of the Viennese classical meter of the countertenor would be duple-/JdJ This is all the more con-
we are not sure how (a + 3
fifteenth-century music the tradition of performance is lost.
)
.'-t
l

112 The Rlytthmic Structure of Music Rh2thm and Meter 113

vincing because measure 3 is now similar to measure 1, being a metric dissonance just given indicates that the organization of the second half of measure 8 is in
which is resolved to a consonance in measure 4' doubt.
Since only one voice moves in measure 4, the measure is metrically
consonant' This leads us to a consideration of the metric organization of the passage be-
is somewhat problematical' For ginning with the final tones of measure 8 and running through the beginning of
However, the triple meter of the countertenor
this is the only measure in the whole countertenor part which is not in duple measure 11. In these measures the meter of the cantus is particularly ambiguous
meter (3 + 3). The explanation of the change would seem to be that a
simple and might be interpreted in various ways. Of the many groupings possible for
duple meter would at this point have produced so great a letdown of tension that this passage an examination of the pros and cons of four of the more plausible
the two halves of the larger section (measure 1-B) would fall apart, becoming ones will, it is hoped, bring out some of the problems and considerations involved
almost disconnected, sepirate entities. In short, the triple meter serves
to main- (Example 124)'
(8) (e) (10) (r1)
tain tension without creating metric dissonance'
In the second phrase the tenor has exactly the same temporal scheme as it had a

in the first phrase and the cantus begins as it did in measure 1' This clear 3+ + 3+3 +2 t3+3

parallelism suggests that the countertenor is not in triple meter U"'


,1.*j.,
that measure 5 involves metric crossing
*-F-). The ,'
J-Jg!;|
in duple meter
'^-"'- hypothesis b
F-s 31 +2 2 + + + + 1*3
is supported by the fact that measure 6
js consonant. The parallelism between
pt rur., (particularly in the tenor) also makes it probable that in measure 7 there
is metric crossing which is resolved measure 8, that is, that the cantus in
in c.
rneasure 7 is not to be understood as a syncopated triple meter but as
a duple
I B + 1ll 2 + 2 tt 3 + 3 ttz + 2 | r!-t-!*12----r
Jl , which crosses the triple meter of the tenor.
on the other hand,
meter
(s +J_J
3)
to
the marked harmonic motion of the tenor, moving in six-three chords, tends d
influence our perception of the cantus, so that it appears somewhat syncopated.
3 1t+2 + 3 ,t2 + 3 rtz + 2 + 3 + 3
The meter of the countertenor is open to question, particularly since the metric
t

crossing is already present in the other voices. However, because of the


prevailing -tta -a-
l\ JT JJ
duple meter in this voice, it has been thus notated in Example 123' Moreover, Tenor
c. T.
from a melodic point of view a return to B on a beat, which would result if the 3 +3 + 3 + 3 + 3 + 3+3
meter were triple, does not seem convincing' That is, the melodic structure EXAMPLE 124

makes more musical sense than , in which the The first alternative to be considered (Example I24a) has two things to
recommend it: It continues the meter of the previous measures-although in a
D acts only as a changing-note rather than as a structural tone. On the other
style in which metric change is normative, this is not necessarily a virtue; and it
hand, an interpretation in triple meter which would parallel the motion of the
fits harmonically with the lower voices. Against this interpretation of the meter
tenor is a definite possibilitY. of the cantus are these considerations: The tone C does not become a point of
Notice, however, that the parallelism between the first and second phrases
is
metric-melodic culmination which it seems intended to be. And because the C is
not exact. Although the tenor is the same in both parts and the procedure of
weak, the A's in measures 9, 10, and 11 are overemphasized. Furthermore, the
metric crossing is similar, the metric structure of the cantus changes in measures syncopation of the C seems doubtful on both stylistic and purely musical grounds.
5-8. In the first half, metric alternation takes place every other measure: triple- In a style without marked pulses syncopation seems to have little function, and it
duple-triple-duple. In the second phrase, metric alternation is by two-measure is particularly pointless in this case, since there is no harmonic motion across the
,rnitr, t.ipl-triple-duplrduple(?). The question mark at the end of the scheme "bar line" to imbue the C with a need for motion. Finally, it should be observed
114 The Rhythmic Structure of Music Rh2tlan and Meter 715
that this alternative has little relationship to either the pattern of changing and no matter which of the alternatives examined above is chosen, two measures of
crossing meters which precedes it or the clear triple meter which follows it. metric dissonance result. Thus measures 9 and 10 function as metric dissonances
The second alternative (Example 724b) does make metric sense when con- resolving to the metric consonance of the final three measures which are solidly
sidered as part of a total metric scheme. In the first half of Section A the meter in duple meter.
of the cantus changes after six eighth-notes-1(3 X 2) + 1(2 X 3); it changes It is clear that both metric change and metric crossing are basic principles of
after twelve eighth-notes in the second half of Section A-2(3 X 2) + 2(2 X 3); organization here. They are so not only in their own right but also in the sense
and, if the cantus were triple in the first half of Section B, then this progressive that they articulate and influence the rhythmic structure of the whole piece.
increase of distance between changes of meter would be continued, resulting in (1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)
this scheme-3(3 x 2) + 3(2 x 3). Thus a very neat over-all metric progression ll'l Lr ---#
of 1:2:3 is produced. Unfortunately, however, Example 124b is not convincing _C-ffi).
dq
"
harmonically and melodically. From a harmonic point of view, the third and
fourth groups of two (those beginning on G and B) are dissonant with the lower ). ). J, t.l .b,l , ) )t
parts. Again the C fails to achieve what rt would seem should be its melodic
importance. And finally the syncopation is, as noted above, stylistically doubtful. l-wt
As indicated by the beam connecting the two c's across the bar, Example
i. r-rwrrw -rl r r- v lTiEl(w w) r t- (w-)El--i=-Ul tv-l
>4
1.
l24c is not syncopated. Furthermore, it does place a metric accent (on the lowest 2.
level) on the C, making it a point of melodic culmination. However, this version 3.
is objectionabie on the ground that the firsr group of three (beginning on G) is
(8) (13) (14)
dissonant. And beginning such a group on a dissonance seems doubtful on the -T--fi#r -77i--1:1
basis of what we know of other works in this style. While this version of the 4+--
passage does make some metric sense in relation to the rest of the piece and to the
a,)
- +l \fip -
practice of the style, its symmetrical organization-
+ 3+ 1
1) .l rr ).
--ffi
.i Jt.=J. ,t\ 4l"r ). J -).
I -t#
-tr r ,l
l, I ---19L
2+. 2+2
3+3 i. -l lv
-uilv-
d I t- wvtl- d r
Ur l-v
1.
not really reflect the mode of progression
previously established.
-does
The final alternative (Example 724d) works both harmonically and melodical-
3.

ly. The groups of twos and threes all begin on consonances and the high c EXAMPLE 125

comes at the beginning of such a metric group. Although this version is metrically - 1, 3, 5, and 7 makes those measures
For instance, metric crossing in measures
more complex than Example 724c, it does reflect the metric modus operandi of tJne anacrustic to measures 2,4r 6, and 8, respectively. Similarly, the metric structure
first section of the piece. Indeed it is in a sense a condensed presentation of that of the first part of Section B forms the basis for the anapestic rhythm which arises
section. That is, its organization (2 + 3,2 + 3,2 + 2,3 f 3) presents an ac- in these measures, while the metric crossing between parts articulates the rhythm
celerated version of the pattern of metric change between measures which took of the second architectonic level. These matters as well as the rhythmic organiza-
place in the opening section. Moreover, the condensation is not only metric bul
tion on all levels should be clear from the analysis given in Example 125.
melodic as well. For the motion from D to C encompasses the basic range of the
first section of the piece. And this emphasizes again the necessity for the C to
arrive on an accent, as it did in measure 6 and as do both the A in measure 11 EXERCISES
and the B-flat in measure 12, to which it moves. I
Finally, the metric complexity of this version of the passage with its intensi- A. Analyze the following passages on all architectonic levels:
fication of metric change seems appropriate in view of the fact that the passage Bach, Chamber Suite No. 4 in D Major, iii, 1-10
is clearly intended to be one of tension and motion. For it is so constructed that Prelude and Fugue in A Major for Organ, Fugue, 1-16

-,
116 Tlu Rhythmic Structure of Music
Bart6k, Piano Sonata (1926), i, 1-13
Violin Concerto, i, 6-75
Beethoven, Bagatelle Op. 119 No. 1, 53-64
-r Piano Sonata Op. 1lf i, 19-29
Binchois, "De plus en plus" (Historical Antltologt oJ Music, ed. Archibald T. Davison
RHYTHM,
-, and Willi Apel [Cambridge, Mass.: ]Iarvard University Press, 1947], I, 74)
Brahms, Sonata in D Minor for Violin and Piano, i, 48-56 MOBILITY,
String Quartet in A Minor, 1, 162-64
Chopin, Etude Op. 10 No. 70,I-76
-, Etude Op. 25 No. B, I-4
AND TE,NSION
Handel,
--, Concerto Grosso No. 7 in B-flat Major, v, 1-7
Haydn, "London" Symphony in D Major, iii,21-26
String Quartet Op.77 No. 2, ii, 1-24 Paradoxically, every analysis of a piece of music is a kind
of synthesis. we have
Mozart, Sonata in A Major for Violin and Piano (K. 305), i, 24-28 seen again and again in these pages how important
to our analysis of rhythm can
Schubert, Impromptu Op. 142 No. 4, 45-77
-, be our understanding. of the melody, the harmony, the tone_color,
Schumann, String Quartet Op. 44 No. 1, ii, 1-6 and the
dynamics of the music in question. In this chapter u.ri
Symphony No. 1 in B-flat Major, iii, 1-16 irr. next, texture and form
will be added to those aspects of music which must be taken into account if
Stravinsky, Suite No. 2 for Small Orchestra, ii, 23-30
rhythm is to be properly understood. Texture and form, however,
B. -r
Write a brief essay about each passage discussing the relationship of rhythm to meter. only-or even the main-topics of these chapters.
are not the
How does your understanding of this relationship influence your interpretation of the
If this were a book about melody, we shourd find that our anarysis of melody
music in performance? Where alternative interpretations seem possible, discuss how
would imply an understanding of many things other than
each alternative would affect performance. melody; it is plain that
harmony cannot be. treated successfuliy apart from texture (ut'irr"
very least);
II and so on, for any kind of analysis directed at one specific
aspect of music. At its
best, the analysis of one musical factor describes the
A. Find themes illustrating the following: (1) stressed weak beats, (2) melodic-metric effects of all the factors in
non-congruence, (3) syncopation, suspension, and tie' (4) potential accents made to
combination. It describes these effects, but it by no
means does each of them full
justice' There are two main reasons for
function as weak beats, and (5) metric-rhythmic crossing. this. h'irst, any system of analysis uses
symbols, and symbols simplify by classifying. This i,
B. Analyze each of these themes and write a brief essay discussing each theme and your iru. even if words alone
are used' One must guard against mistaking Llassification
analysis of it. How does the metric-rhythmic organization affect the character of the for the end of analysis.
The end of analysis is the appreciation of ihe pecuriar, the individuar.
theme? rn fact,
the peculiar individuality of a good piece of music is
the'second main reason whv
III. Write a theme and variations in which each of the following is exemplified in one analysis, however thoughtful, does incomprete justice
to music. Artd.;;;; ;;
of the variations: (1) stressed weak beats, (2) metric-melodic non-congruence, (3) fact that a whole is more than a sum of its parts, the
various aspects of music do
syncopation, (4) potential accents forced to act as weak beats, and (5) metric- not necessarily coincide in their effects on any one architectonic
rhythmic crossing. level.r Indeed,
a conflict of effect can be an important source of the peculiar
individuality oi
which we have been speaking.

RHTTHM AND MOBILITT


Let us examine the familiar opening of the finale in Mozart,s ,Jupiter,,
sym-
phony (Example 126).
Partly because of tonarity, some of the notes in the melody
are more mobile
I see the discussion of
Exa'rple 11, p. 17; see also the remarks on performance, p.
18.

/17
778 The Rh2thmic Structure of Music Rlrytltm, Mobility, and Tension 119

than others. Mobility means the sense that further melodic motion is necessary which makes possible the effect of what happens in the last two. To generalize,
in order to reach a point of relative repose, a goal of melodic movement. In unaccent is an aspect of accent, and a rhythmic group is an accented shape.
the Mozart fragment (Example 726a), the notes D and F are more mobile than Compare c with a in Example 126. In c, melodic goal, harmonic goal, and
the notes with which they are associated, C and E. There are two reasons for rhythmic accent all coincide (measure 3). In a, melody and harmony are mobile
this. First, C and E are notes in the tonic triad. In a passage in C major, there- at the accent (measure 3) and relatively static at the afterbeat (measure 4). For
fore, they tend to be pitches from which and to which melody moves as from one harmony, like melody, has its moments of relative mobility and stability. In
relatively stable point to another. Of course, this relative stability is not a matter Example 126c, the dominant chord of measure 2 sets up, or seeks to move toward,
of melody alone. The harmony may alter the tonal significance of the melody and a tonic chord. Measures 3 and 4 follow as the normal thing from I and 2.
make otherwise relatively stable notes more mobile (see the last measure of Measures 3 and 4 of Example 126c would not, however, follow as the normal
Example 126d). The second reason why D and F are more mobile here than C thing from measures 1 and 2 of Example 126a. There, the II! chord of measure
MoIto b. 2 sets up some kind of dominant as the next chord. That is, one expects a rela-
frFr- it)-- L
ttr-T'T--T--r- sT--r -=# -:r-r tively mobile harmony in measure 3, rather than the far more static tonic which
a)
p
AA
AJ
w9 TV IU ut) occurs at the corresponding place in Example 126c. What happens harmonically
in measures 1 and 2 of Example 726a makes what happens harmonically and
melodically in measures 3 and 4 appropriate. (Let us not make the mistake of
n t-t.l t-Fl-
4;fir-ffi-,f -v-lz, -/"-1: T- saying "inevitable.") The F in measure 3 is appropriate harmonically, because
it is part of a V7 chord which follows as a goal of movement from the previous
- - IIf chord. This V7 chord, in its turn, is highly mobile, and marks the following
chord (VI instead of I) for consciousness. But just as we characterized the goal of
melodic movement here as accented-F-leaning-on-E, so we may characterize the
G-
M4- goal of harmonic movement as accented-v7-leaning-on-Vl. And it is VI on
t) ,') DV DD o
which V7 leans here. If the reader finds this difficult to believe, let him try to put
1--'t,., ltt -t a tonic triad in root position in measure 4 in such a way that the second violins
t# * are forced to do nothing awkward either in voice-leading or in playing technique.
If we add another part as in Example 126b, two things happen. First, we be-
come decidedly conscious of the normal harmonic tendency for the V? chord to
EXAMPLE 126
resolve onto a tonic triad, and of the melodic and harmonic tenriency in this
and E is that the D, following the C, sets up a direction of melodic movement; passage for the F to move to an E, because these tendencies are forced upon us by
it gives us the feeling that the melody is on its way to a point not yet reached: the dissonances between treble and bass. The music becomes tied down and
certainly to E, and perhaps beyond it. The note F sidesteps the direct line of loses the floating, bass-less quality of the original. Second, the texture becomes
motion from C to E. Moving from D to F creates a gap in that line of motion, stylistically more normal. These two things happen without any outward result
and this gap increases the desirability of E as a goal of motion; it makes the F in an analysis on the first level-and perhaps even on the second-but they have
lean heavily on the E and require resolution there. The F is mobile because of its an important bearing on a feeling for the rhythm. Let us examine this feeling
function in the melodic line. before taking further steps.
The mobile F is accented. The goal of motion, E, is not accented. One is The art of analysis is based upon the art of listening. In analyzing Example
tempted to say that the E has a melodic accent, even though it is rhythmically an 126, we are reflecting, with svmbols, upon our experience in listening to the be-
afterbeat. But each aspect of the music qualifies all the others, and it would be ginning of a classical finale (and various distortions thereof). In listening to such
more in accord with one's experience of this passage to say that the E with an a piece we do not leave our knowledge of classical finales out of account. Even
F leaning on it is melodically marked for consciousness. It is the accented-F- if we pretend that we know nothing about this particular movement, we never-
leaning-on-E that is in large part responsible for the peculiarity of these four theless know quite a lot about it. For example, it is going to contain passages that
measures. Notice, however, that it is what happens in the first two measures are loud, in at least four-part harmony, and for full orchestra. What we are
of Music Rfutthm, Mobility, and Tension 721
120 Tfu Rhlthmic Structure
and for violins alone' If some first two measures of Example 126a. There, direction of movement was im-
listening to now (a) is soft, in three-partharmony'
the finale of Haydn's "Farewell" mediately established. Here, there is no such thing. The music is solidly planted
sophist interrupts us at this point to refer to
on the tonic in an end-accented rhythm. It is going to move on a larger scale;
Symphony,weknowwhattotellhim'.Theimportantthinghereisthatweknow
is leading up to something of a it is going to have a slower pace. We anticipate movement, but we do not yet
f..f."tfy well that what we are now hearing the moment.(say' at the end of know its direction. And then, before anything happens to this opening motive,
certain kind. No matter how sure we may be at
measure 4) that this whole phrase is accented,
it nevertheless has about it a we are presented with a violent contrast in measures 3 and 4. This is a contrast
other words, an upbeat feeling. in dynamics, in texture, in orchestration, and altogether in character.
strong background i."iirrg oi anticipation-in
phrase. Perhaps a larger Now measures 1 and 2 and measures 3 and 4 are both connected and discon-
Now this is not the *u*.ihirrg u* "ulling it an upbeat
the beginning will function on a nected. They are connected in part simply because they are next to each other.
section of which these four measures are only
this. phrase will so func- 14, composer's imagination must often present him with such seemingly accidental
;il;; architectonic level as an anacrusis. Perhaps just
this phrase is ready, as a whole,
tion. we do not yet know. we know only that Allogro Vlvace (5)
part of a larger anacrusis, or an 4_J+
to be in retrospect an anacrusis, or an accented -H'Ha

unaccented part of a larger anacrusis'


less mobile; indeed,
In version D the textuie is less abnormal; the harmony is is still ready to be
l-r f-f ' u p r
trr" pt rur. is in itself more stable than the original,
in retrospect an anacrusis or a part of one. The
although it
feeling about it of anticipation is
6-> 1->
-l
j#
d-a
i-F-
A.

can be made lesser: for instance, by making a I ru lJrlv!rrv- vr r*JAr-]4WJ


less, however. And this feeling -2 vl \-./ rtzv-l
to a forte in measure 3, and by orchestrating
lv.

crescendo in measures 1 and 2.?


The analysis on the first level
measure 3 as atutti with various octave doublings. 3.
Version } might be followed -----9.
remains the same, t,,t tt,. rhythm feels different.
bythesamethingthatfollowsversionaintheactualsymphony.Ifso,the
same, but the rhythm' 6------
analysis on the second level as well would
remain the f,* EXAMPLE 127
again, would feel different'
paths' Imagine
In version c both melody and harmony move alo_ng straight juxtapositions of ideas. What becomes of this particular juxtaposition we shall
thepassageforteandwithatuttiaccompanimentfrommeasure2onward.It see shortly. The forte idea and the piano idea are further connected by the very
can still become in retrospect an anacrusis
or a part of one, but in itself-that is'
of the four fact that the piano idea moves both melodically and harmonically, and we had
until something else foliows-it is the most positively accented
anticipated movement. Besides that, as the analysis on level 1 shows, the rhythms
versions.
accented of the four' Both a of the two ideas are simllar: each is end-accented, and each has three "syllables."
version d, on the other hand, is the least positively But we are more struck by the disconnection, the contrast. In place of the tonic
and. dend with u ae"epti""
but the final chord in d is more specifically
"uj.rr"., that something follow d is-more we have the dominant (the tonic triad in the middle of measure 3 being a passing-
mobile than is tt. finul chord in a. The demand chord, very unemphatic rhythmically); in place of the uncompromising C's we
specificandclear;thedernandthatsomethingfollowaismoregeneraland have the movement from C to G-Ieaning-on-F.
.rrug.r", however strong it maY be'
we are by im- The quasi-turn around C followed by a leap to F preceded by an appoggiatura
In making un unut'y,i* on th€ primary rhythmic level' then' should be compared for its effect of relative mobility with the melody of Example
higher levels, and, in a real sense'
plication making u fJt "tiuf analysis.on the 726a.Therc, C was relatively stable and F was highly mobile. Here, C is highly
;; ;t" beginnirig to assess the individuality of the whole' mobile, although it is the tonic, and F, because it is the goal of motion, is rela-
Letusnowturntoamorecomplex*tr,r"trr.inthesamework,thebeginning tively stable (within these two measures, that is), in spite of its general tonal
analysis and camy it to higher
of the first movem.*, i" order tomake a similar mobility and, more important, in spite of its being the seventh in a dominant-
architectonic levels (Example 127)' we have a feeling seventh chord. Broadly speaking, the relative mobility of the notes C and F is
In the reiteratei "rrd po*.rf,.,I c's of the first two measuresfrom c to D in the more peculiar here than it is in the finale.
melodic motion
quite diflerent fromih. orr" g.rr".ated by the
Rhythm, Mobiliflt, and Tension 723
122 The Rtrythmic Strtnture of Music
mark there in the analysis. Although the two forte passages together and the
How does this reversal of the normal tonal roles of c and F in c major coms two piano passages together will both be end-accented groups, the two groups
about? The first c (that at the end of measure 2) is underplayed rhythmically:
it
it is a dissonant will have different feelings on the next level. The forte group will feel like an up-
is an anticipation. The second C is underplayed harmonically:
it is a beat, since it moves harmonically from tonic to dominant, and the piano group
appoggiatura. The third c is again underplayed rhythmically: although will be ambiguous, since, although it moves harmonically from dominant to
subprimary level'
,.*l,rt1on, and consonant, it occurs on an unaccent on the tonic and is to that extent accented, it is only partially complete melodically:
(because unexpected-
So much for the C. What of the F? The melody leaps boldly the movement from C to F in measures 3-4 and continuing, again from C,
ly) to G, and this G is perfectly consonant with the rest of the harmony. Yet not
through F to G in measures 7-8, only succeeds in pointing to the higher C as the
only is it treated u, un uppoggiatura; one feels it has to be so treated' The
next desired note and thus tinges the harmonically accented aspect of the group
,"uron for this feeling ties i"ittiihythm on the subprimary level' There we have,
with a feeling of anacrusis.
first, two attempted amphibrachs. The dissonances on the first and third beats of The skeletal analysis on the bottom staff of Example 127 is intended to show
are de-
measure 3 would normally be resolved on afterbeats, but the resolutions that both the piano groups and the forte groups point melodically at C's for
layed in such a way as to iurn them into upbeats' Our desire to hear an afterbeat
is only their sense of completion. With the entrance of the tutti in measure 9, the
is frustrated in measure 3, and when the melody moves to G, this desire ambiguity we have been previously feeling about the grouping is brilliantly re-
forces the G to
the stronger for having been twice frustrated. The rhythm, that is, moved, and we are able in retrospect to make a satisfying grouping on the third
resolve op the F. AnJ this rhythmic effect is so powerful that it
makes us hear
level. The analysis on that level under Example 727 indicates both the somewhat
a consonant note (G) as a dissonance and a dissonant note (F) as a consonance-
unstable antecedent-consequent relationship mentioned earlier and the retro-
so powerful, indeed, as to make the nominal V$ chord in the
middle of measure 4
spective grouping with measure 9 as its accent.
stand in no need of resolution onto a tonic triad with the third on top' In the discussion of Example 126, mention was made of the important part
It would be insructive at this point to look ahead to Example l28a,in which played in our feeling for rhythm on higher levels by our general knowledge of the
the amphibrachs on the subprimary level are "allowed." You will notice that, particular style in which a piece of music is written. It is worth while to revert to
try as you may, you cannot flttow the final G of this example with an F without this notion in considering Example 127. Is it not true that while we are Iistening
having it sound like a ludicrous error. to measures 1-8 we are aware in the back of our minds that there is going to be
It is not, however, only the rhythm on the subprimary level which creates the (we do not know exactly when) a tutti in the full texture of the Viennese style?
peculiarity of the F as a melodic goal of movement' It is also the bold leap to G, When we hear that tutti enter in measure 9, it is not an unexpected event. But
pr.rr-ubiy part of what was given by the composer's imagination. Let us sup- notice that it has a rhythmic function, that of clarifying the grouping of measures
pose the composer to have bein a far lesser one and glance ahead at
Example
1-8; it is motivated. It is not only normal; it is also right. Of such congruences
129a. There we can see the same rhythmic effect, as far as the analysis would
is music made.
show, as in the original, but the rhythm has done nothing at all drastic to our
in It should be clear from this example that dynamics, texture, orchestration,
feeling for melodic- *otitity. Given the melodic direction set up the first
and character, as well as melody and harmony, can play important parts in the
,rr.usir. of Example 129a, there is nothing in the least peculiar about having a analysis of rhythm. Indeed, a satisfactory rhythmic analysis can be said in some
D as the goal of movement in the next measure' sense to summarize the effects of these factors. This is not to say that rhythmic
If we now go on and listen to the whole of the first eight measures of the move- analysis is the only tool of generalization in thinking about music-as we shall
ment, *. .roii.. at once that we are uncertain of how to group them on the see in the next chapter-but it is an important one, and we can gain some further
second level. On the one hand, measures 1-4 and 5-8 stand in an antecedent- insight into its generalizing powers by worrying the bone of Example 127 a bit
consequent relationship; they use the same motives in the same order, and
the
more before going on.
by
harmonic movement from tonic to dominant in the antecedent is answered
What would be the consequences of replacing measures 3-4 of Example 127
group
a movement from dominant to tonic in the consequent. We can therefore with Example 728a? The only satisfactory answer we could get to the melodic
measures 1-4 together, and measures 5-8. On the other hand, the contrast
be-
question C-G would be the G-C of Example 128b, which would then appear in
tween the two motives is sufficiently strong so that one cannot help also grouping
measures 7-8 of the original. This would be embarrassingly feeble. The piano
the two forte passages together and the two piano passages. The grouping
on
parts would be both harmonically and melodically finished, while the forte
the second level is J-Uig.ro"*. It is for this reason that we have put a question
724 Tlu RlAthmic Structure of Music Rhythm, Mobilitlt, and Tension 725

parts remained up in the air. The continuation in measure 9 would be satisfying we have made would result in exactly the same analysis on the primary level as
as far as the forte parts are concerned but redundant otherwise. (Never mind the in Example I27. It is not so much what occurs rhythmically on the primary level
fussy, waspish character of Example 128.) that determines what we feel on the higher levels, as it is what occurs in the other
Or let us try replacing the piano parts of the original with the entirely normal aspects of music. This is why rhythmic analysis has such a strong generalizing
and perfectly logical a and b of Example 129. The same difficulty would be upon power.
us as in Example 128. Perhaps we could create a feeling of anacrusis by inserting Before leaving the first movement of the 'Jupiter" Symphony, let us look at
a B-flat as in Example 129c. So far, so good, but then we could not go on as in a genuine alteration of the material we have been considering-genuine, because
measure 9 of the original, since the next thing indicated would seem to be a six- made by the composer himself in the course of composition. In Example 131
four chord over C in the bass. All that these remarks signify is that what Mozart
a. b. (24) I
-:H4- --C#

rU-Vllu-VllV rv -wll*Jl-J
a
? col
I

8va bessa
r t l-l

EXAMPLE 128 A^ t ,--lT-. ..l+


b. c.
4F + -re.- lb.
illvl l-l
a) ( t- f,-/l 2.
EXAMPLE 131
E_b &O S->r
# +------f----r- -+-------f-----*-
changes have been made which result in complete clarity of grouping on the
EXAMPLE I29 second level. The dynamics, the texture, and the orchestration have been made
more homogeneous, with the result that the original contrast in character has
b.
been softened. Furthermore, the contrapuntal accompaniment, with its com-
plete continuity of rhythm (see analysis 1a), has bound together the whole four
measures into something that can be heard as a single group without forcing the
EXAMPLE 130 original rhythms on the primary level to lose their individual identity. Of course
this new rhythm for old material will allow a difierent continuation, as an
did in Example 127 is right and that we have been unable to tinker with it with- examination of the score will show.
out making it wrong. This does not mean at all that Mozart might not have
done sorrrething else that would have been right. It means only that measures RHTTHM AND TENSION
1-8 as Mozart wrote them have a certain rhythmic quality (compounded of all
the various aspects of the music) and that this quality makes the continuation of We have been discussing movement in music and the issue of this movement
the music in measure 9 convincing. in the generalized feeling we call rhythm. The time has come to examine the
Perhaps we should have tried doing something to the forte parts, as in Bxample
feeling more analytically.
130. This time, the forte parts will be finished and the piano parts will remain The possible elements in a rhythmic group are upbeat (or anacrusis), accent,
in the air, making the tutti of measure 9 again partly and unpleasantly redun- and afterbeat. No one of these feels like the others, nor does any one of them it-
dant. Or we could go all the way and combine Example 729a and D with Example self always feel the same. But before attempting to generalize further, let us
130. Now nothing will follow. Measure 9 will simply be more music; it will no' proceed inductively by examining selections from four pieces.
Ionger be the right music; there would be no right music. The first is Chopin's Prelude in E-flat (Example 132). Throughout most of
The reader should notice carefully that all these unpleasant alterations which this Prelude the fundamental rhythm is a middle-accented one. But this rhythm
126 Tlu Rh2thmic Structure of Music Rlrythm, Mobilitlt, and Tension ln
seldom appears as a grouping of three quarter-notes; usually, the anacrusis.is In the Chopin excerpts we have just been considering, the elements of melody
lengthenei. At the UJgi""i"g of the piece, for instance (Bxample 132a and a'), and harmony conspire in a single, simple rhythmic effect, and the dynamics are
move- those most natural to this effect: the slight crescendo on an anacrusis and the
thJanacrusis sails up from the B-flat to the G with a sense of continuous
ment through an unobstructed arpeggio and without any minor groupings
except gentle decrescendo on an accent followed by an afterbeat. In the following
for that of the opening B-flat and its anticipation. One might call this anacrusis excerpt from the same composer's Etude op. 10 No. 9 (Example 134), the case
from it
a lyrical one, or, perhapr, a contemplative one. The feeling which arises is quite different. Instead of serene lyricism we find a dramatic conflict which has
is rather like that which arises from seeing a speeded-up moving picture of a much to do with the rather petulant character of the music.
bud gradually opening into a flower. The tension2 with which we await the
ap- This conflict appears first on the primary and secondary rhythmic levels. The
p.u.irr"" of tire iull-bl,own flower is rather like the tension with which we await
(5)
the ,e,rerral of movement (to F and E-flat) in measure 3 of the Prelude;
it is a
is no
tension of calm rather than one of agitation. Other than rubato, there rE'.jr-# --J-----l--J---
utLll
temporal differentiation here. Nothing happens harmonically until measure
3' oLlt . liJ l-l
--
I

There is no dynamic change except the slight crescendo which seems to be :=_-
+ +
natural in a rising melodic line.
+++ +- I - + h- -- PL
b.
a. Vivage

EXAMPLE 133
p
senpre legato
ar
br Allegro, molto agitato
etc
---
tv - vllv
1 11.
EXAMPLE 132 1. t- v vt- |

-n+cDl
p
Obviously, the performer must see to it that the E-flat (second beat of measure ^+
1) is not closer in time to the preceding B-flat than it is to the following G;
if
beyond its
,r.".rrury, the B-flat on the first beat of measure 1 can be held slightly 2b. e
time in order that the following E-flat will not sound like the end of a
group' 3.

And, in general, the performer cannot afford to play according to the unthinking 4.
ar-I
principif "stress the first beat of the bar," or he will readily fall into a constant 5.

subsidiary amphibrach grouping. Chopin, for him, might as well have


written -
as in Bxample l32b and.i' .The contemplative tension of the initial
anacrusis is an
essential part of the character of this piece.
As we *.. in Example 133, Chopin can achieve a lengthened anacrusis by
means other
"u.r than unchanging harmony and uninhibited upward melodic rlove=
t w
ment. In measure 5, there is the auxiliary note D in the melody; two measures
later, this D is associated with a B in the accompaniment. These dissonances
compel us to make two-measure groupings. of course, the performer merely
has
2b
he has
to think in the groupings indicated by the analysis; he will then, provided 3.
adequate technical habits, automatically achieve correct results. 4.
5.
of
2 We wish we could use the word "intensity," but it has been pre-empted for the description
dynamics.
EXAMPLE 134
128 Tlu Rhythmic Structure of Music Rlrytlm, Mobility, and Tension 129
harmony consists in each measure of a set of appoggiaturas which resolve onto a there is a return to the piano of measure 57, after an intervening pianissimo, and
tonic triad, over a tonic pedal which persists-and has persisted for some time- this piano is followed by a crescendo; the dynamics of the echo are stronger than
in the bass. The rhythm of the harmony, therefore, is trochaic. This becomes those of the echoed. And the situation is more striking in measure 63, with its
particularly clear if we examine the simplified version in Example 135a. But fortissimo following upon the forte-pianissimo of the previous measures.
the melody of the original (see Example 134) is middle-accented in rneasures Finally, on the highest architectonic level indicated in the analysis, the second
57-58 and 6l-62 and end-accented in the other measures. And each of the four measures echo the first four; yet it is in the echo that we find forte and
accents in the melody falls on the afterbeat of the accompaniment. Indeed, in fortissimo. Furthermore, it is here that the upbeats on the primary level are more
measure 59 the conflict is aggravated by the crescendo placed on the anacrusis, loaded with tensionl there are simply more notes in the upbeats. In the case of
which only succeeds in stressing the conflict in accent. the G's, the increased reiteration and the downward leap of an octave indicate
a greater frenzy, one might say, in reaching the accent, and this accent can never
J.--1+E+L----1----1- #
be a positive one, coming as it does against the feminine rhythm of the harmony.
In like manner, the upbeats in measures 63-64 may move farther on their way
a) tla t*z v.-*i v'ti a)
,p
I
V
from E to F, but it is still from E to F that they move.
sotto voce
-2 i one can learn a good deal about the peculiarity of this passage by some inter-
-ff ;t"tffi. changing of parts. For instance, the reader might pray the same notes but inter-
change the dynamics of measures 57-60 and 6l-64. He will find that because
2. l- w I l- v ll- u I l- v I 2
the effect of the lyrical afterbeat of measures 61-64 is not contradicted by the
(60)
dynamics the character is significantly different. or, again, he might interchange
measures 57-60 and 6l-64 completely. In this case, the result will sourrd #
fensively wrong, because measures 57-60, thus misplaced, become an insipid
a) 1- lt- 1:.-X- __--/ oversimplification of measures 6l-64, instead of a relaxed echo of them.
I t b+ .C -bz O Not every afterbeat on a higher architectonic level is of the sort we have been
describing as "lyrical." A lyrical afterbeat is one which echoes a point of arrival,
a goal of motion. In the example under discussion, the goal of motion is reached
in measure 57. compared with what happens in that measure, there is, we may
EXAMPLE 135 say, no further motion at all.3

The reader will notice that if we rewrite the passage according to the pattern
AN EXTENDED ANACRUSIS
of Example 1356 the conflict disappears, for both melody and harmony are now
end-accented. At the same time, the character has changed radically. The out- In the excerpts we have examined so far in this chapter, the effects of rhythmic
bursts in measures 61 and 63, for instance, have become assertive and positive, grouping have extended over relatively short spaces of time. We now turn to a
and have lost their air of fiying to escape from enforced suppression. The much longer excerpt, which, in spite of its length, is readily heard as an anapest
rhythmic conflict of the original is essential to the character of the music. which embodies an exciting climax. The excerpt (Example 136) is from the
On the third architectonic level there are lyrical afterbeats; the even-num- introduction to Act II of Wagner's Tristan und Isolde.
bered measures echo the odd-numbered ones. These echoing afterbeats have the The large group to be considered begins in measure 33, but a few of the
effect of lowering the "temperature" of the music-of releasing the tension of the previous measures have been included in the example in order to make clear
one
music while they echo. And the dynamics on this level are not in conflict with the of the peculiarities of the rhythm. This peculiarity is that there is a conflict in
rhythm. The echoes are softer than what is echoed. On the next level, however,, the sense of movement which arises from the passage beginning in measure 33.
there is again dramatic conflict; for again we have echoes, but this time the dy- Both the characteristic quarter-note motion of the cello melody in the previous
namics are against the echo effect. Measures 59-60 echo measures 57-58 with ..3
Example 92 in chapter iii contains an excellent case of a lyrical afterbeat. Note the
effect of
variations; likewise, measures 63-64 echo measutes 6!-62. But in measure 59 all four afterbeats on the second level.
Fl. T-+---+\---.1++;--l!-- ----+)-----a--G
Sehr lebhaft -9ffi
.T fr
p (40)
/ rnolto crcsc,
Poco a Poco cr?s(:.
(27) Li ?++
p J+:r__]_
i. l- u I
l-vt l-wll-v I

L. -v-

J.
- ,/ \- - - -
(30)
-f
1l-). r l'\
2',
Vln

a) VO F
atet ''
'lfr
)_ D
K. B. &Vc
I, lU- VllV ur lv
I
1. v)
2a,

\./ 3.
1. v
r--u-Er vr Fl. 3

cI _F .J\

---r-\
a)
tpt- p
(50)

, ,
:lffi a
pp
, ta l.
1
2b. like mm 33ff
3.

CI Vln.
rHtr- 'r*;- :ffi)Ih".'J --t<-
+
ar
ffi.:-T--
I -t'--zv -14 >a
FD--!_P t*-/
piu ? "F Y:Vlt'
Variant
t , 1L --rr JL-,a r

;t-.- __GF.- trI-

2b. t/-

EXAMPLE 136 EXAMPLE 736-Continued

1il 131
Rh2tltm, Mobility, and Tension 133
(60)
ob Vfn
measures and the agitated accompaniment figure in the upper strings suddenly

-- F5? disappear, leaving a rapid, largely eighth-note motion in the melody and a much
slower motion in the accompaniment. This conflict between a slow substratum
, 'o ^olto "r"r", .La
L-{}
and an agitated surface is appropriate to the dramatic situation which will be
--Y n s-iFJ
disclosed when the curtain rises, inasmuch as it sets the agitation in high relief.
ia.l '
The analyses on level 1 and level 2b are to be read simultaneously as an indica-
r. I tion of the two rhythms. The analysis of the rhythm on the subprimary level
2.t (which is easy to make) has been omitted in the interest of clarity.
Now the underlying rhythm (level 2b) of measures 33-38 is primarily de-
Vln. pendent upon harmonic movement. We have first a diminished-seventh chord
acting as dominant of II, followed by 116 with an appoggiatura, F-sharp, which
.IIiF I f f 1L)t sempre
lf
resolves to G. This makes the amphibrach of measures 33-34. After this, the 116
chord is altered (E-flat to E and C to C-sharp) so as to lead toward a If chord
piir ./' -tF-- which would, of course, tend to resolve onto a dominant of some form or other.
h l. ___ba_1,)____-+L
,,'-urlr,-VllU-Vl
This sequence of chords, along with the resulting underlying rhythm, is indicated
ia.
lu - vl in the variant in Example 136. We see that the rhythm of measures 33-38 would
v rV
1. be two amphibrachs pivoted together into one group; the second amphibrach
2. would take twice as much time as the first. In the actual music, however, the
3.
(70) I! chord does not appear; the harmony moves directly, in measure 37, to a
dominant-ninth (flatted). The reader might object that of course the chord in
measures 36-37 moves to a dominant, inasmuch as the C-sharp is really a D-
P(' ,49- F flat. But that would be to overlook the melody in the oboe, which would have
tr Strlngs 1f, winas ft Strings
ff Winds
to read in measure 35: F-double-flat-D-flat-C-E-B-flat-G-E-E-double-flat ( !).
More important, it would be to overlook the tremendously liberating effect of
-NY- the I[ chord in measure 71, which is preceded by four measures of this very
1 1 chord (E-G-B-flat-C-sharp) with the C-sharp, to be sure, written as a D-flat.
la, l- w ll- v F!ul rn v
This Il chord, with a subsequent dominant, is the harmonic goal of the whole
I I

ib. tlU I

VV -
1.' passage.
t
3.
Because of the omission of the I! chord, the rhythm in measures 35-38 remains
VIa. VcI,
(7 incomplete (level 2D): there are simply two unaccents. But the whole six measures
are a single pivoted group, in spite of the incomplete rhythm. This is partly be-
--t--[-f=.-nTfTl -fitr1\"i1 cause of the rhythm on the subprimary level; but more of a part is played here
p by the overlapping instrumentation-flute, oboe, clarinet.
ff dim. It is the very incompleteness of the rhythm which is primarily responsible for
LA A h- turning measures 33-38 into an anacrusis (level 2D)-rather, into part of an
anacrusis. To be sure, the monstrosity in the variant would be an anacrusis also,
l-v v I l-v v I l- v ,--V---J l-v ,vl-l because of its dominant ending. But notice that the variant is an antecedent
i. Ij
v r l-
1.
J 44 phrase. Upon hearing it, one would not anticipate a long wait before hearing the
2, consequent phrase designed to answer it. In the actual music, measures 33-38
are not an antecedent phrase. Try to imagine a consequent starting at measure
EXAMPLE 736-Continued 39, and you will see that the rhythmic difficulty-that of "answering" an incom-

132
-x
I

The Rhythmic Structure of Music


Rhythm, Mobility, and Tension 135
134
here is a pure transposition of measures 33-49. For the piano of measure 50 is both unexpected
plete rhythm-puts it out of the question' No, what we have
(in view of the restless fortissimo anacrusis which precedes it) and prepared (by
utu"rtrir, not a phrase from a formal theme' The theme' if one wishes to stretch
the piano-crescendo-pianissimo of measures 29-33). Furthermore, we can see
the meaning of the term, extends over the entire excerpt'
from this passage that tremendous tension is not necessarily associated with loud
The passlge we have been Iistening to turns out to be an anacrusis to an
the second upbeat dynamics. The piano of measure 50, with all its attendant air of "having to go
anacrusis. Measures 39-42 are the two upbeats of an anapest'
measures 43-44, so through it all again," has the tension of delay, the tension of long lengths of time
being continued in a syncopation which stretches through
in measure 45' But this being welded into a single group, and, by the time we have reached the end of
as to lead to the heavy second-Ievel accent beginning
to the end of measure 46, loses itself in, measure 66, we are aware that we have heard the two large anacruses of an over-
accent, after being clearly shaped almost
in turn' to what riding anapest on the third level.
again, a., irrco*piete rhythm of enormous tension which leads'
50. Thus, measures 39-49 No sooner have we reached measure 67, however, than we realize that the
w:e expect to be an accent on the beginning of measute
large accent itself begins with an anacrusis (level 2). The fortissimo is there, and
huv. gorr. the way of measures 33-38; that is, they have begun with a clear
the chord of measures 34 and 50 is there, but the resoiution is yet to come. The
rhythiric shape, but they have ended with an incomplete rhythm, and what was
anacrusis is built up by echoes (level 1) in measures 67-70 without any harmonic
going to be a rhythm turns into a part of a rhythm, namely' an anacrusis'
- BJore leaving this anacrusis, a comparison should be made between measures movement. If we compare this passage with that in measures 39-42, we may say
39-42 and Example 134. Both passages contain echoes, but wagner's
echoes are that they have a certain rhythmic similarity, in that both are upbeats in an
This difference comes about' however, anapest. But there the similarity ends. In the earlier passage, the music is moving
parts of upbeats; Chopin's are afterbeats.
the beginning of a group somewhere, we know not where until we arrive. In the later passage, we know
rot si*pty through position, that is, through coming at
where we are going: we are going to a I! chord-finally. The quality of the
rather thun ut the end of one. Harmony is in fact vital in creating the difference'
tension is different here. It is the tension which arises from repetitions of a har-
Put in the simplest terms, chopin's harmony is all tonic; wagner's is all am-
motion a whole- mony that is mobile and has a specific direction. Yet this tremendous tension is
biguous. Wugn.1. has two diminished-seventh chords in parallel
of tonality first appears capped, even at the very moment of its release, by the powerful appoggiatura G
ste"p apart; th.y harre no clear tonai function; a sense
on the first beat of measure 71. And then, just as the G resolves, through G-flat,
with tire augmented-sixth chord at the beginning of measure 43.In Chopin, all
to F, we are back where we started from in measure 33.
has been reached; in Wagner, all is yet to be reached'
How is the goal of movement, the I! chord, to reach the dominant on which it
We think we are goin! to reach something when we get to measure 50 of
un- Ieans? It cannot do so immediately, because one cannot find release from excite-
Example 136. But, alihough the chord is the right chord, the dynamics are
ment by pricking its balloon. Time is needed. And time is provided by means of
expected.
itis, of course, the same as the one introduced in a series of echoes, decrescendo. These echoes consist of truncated amphibrachs
First, about the chord;
go directly on the primary level. That much of the rhythm which is within measure 72-
measure 35. We can see how right it is if we omit measures 50-66 and
hear the resolution the accent and the afterbeat-is repeated three times in measures 73-75 in such
from measure 49 to measure 6i , aftet which we shall shortly
reach' The a way that the sequence of harmonies-IV (measure 72), V (73), and twice I
of this chord to the If which the harmony of measures 35-36 failed to
(74-75)-is created over a dominant pedal very low in the horns, each harmony
chord, then, annountes what the goal of the large anacrusis will in part be like'
being preceded by appoggiaturas. Thus the accent itself trails off, to begin with,
It also announces, however, that it is not that goal. It does this by its dynamics' in an incomplete rhythm on the second level. The incompleteness of the rhythm
(Notice also the subtle absence of tremolo in measure 50')
Now, about the dynamics: if we look back to measures 29-32, we find, in the
is emphasized by underplaying the continuity on the subprimary level. For, al-
' though there is overlapping among the winds, which double the strings here, the
top line, a.r introduction to the passage beginning in measure 33. The introduc-
change of string color is striking, the string parts do not overlap, and the breaks
tion is piano crescendo. The crescendo leads to a sudden pianissimo at the be-
between measures are clarified; therefore one does not hear a series of pivoted
ginning of measure 33. This sudden pianissimo is in part responsible for our
feel-
amphibrachs, with the afterbeat of one becoming the upbeat of the next, nor yet
i.rg tt ut something begins here. Now, when we arrive at measure 50 and find
a series of trochees, but rather a series of echoes of ends of amphibrachs. The
that it is unexpectedly piano, we remember the previous feeling of beginning at-
violins, which play the melody starting in measure 7I, are cut off at the end of
tached to measure 33, and. we have the feeling of beginning again. This
feeling
note-for-note measure 72; there follow the violas, entirely within measure 72; violins again in
is fully borne out by the fact that measures 50-66 are an almost
136 The Rlgtthmic Structure of Music Rlrythm, Mobility, and Tension 7A7
74;finally, the cellos in 75 are allowed to run over to the first beat of 76, where in stretching the lengths. The third-level accent is, itself, an anapest in
which the
the awaited dominant appears, piano. Notice that measure 75 echoes measure 74 anacruses consist, on the second level, of incomplete rhythms. And
now we see
exactly in melody and harmony (though not in color and range), and that there- also that the second-level accent of measures 77-76 itself contains incomplete
fore a small anapest is created on the second level with the dominant chord at the rhythms which, however, are finally ailowed to give way (in measures 74-76)
beginning of measure 76 as its accent. In this manner, the second-level accent of to
another anapestic grouping. rn other words, the rhythmic procedure is homo-
measures 67-76 begins, as did the large third-level anacrusis, with a complete geneous throughout: although the fundamental rhythm is
the amphibrach, the
rhythm which is succeeded by an incomplete rhythm; only, this time the incom- fundamental shape is the anapest; and the fundamental means
plete rhythm is itself succeeded by another complete rhythm at the end, so that
ol stretching is
incomplete rhythm.
the rhythmic impulse may conclude with a final accent. In the wagner and chopin excerpts, we have been examining the feelings as-
Two things remain to be noticed about this excerpt. sociated with anacruses, accents, and afterbeats, by analyzing .ur", i1
First, the amount of energetic tension which has been built up in a wavelike rirrict
each of these aspects- of rhythmic grouping is prominent either be"a,rs.
fashion in the two upbeats (measures 33-49 and 50-66) on the third level is too of having
been stretched on the primary level t, b."u.rr. of being observable
great to be released in the relatively short accent (measures 67-76) which fol- on highei
architectonic levels' We have considered each of these things-anacrusis,
lows. Yet more time is needed. If the reader will refer to the score, he will find accent,
and afterbeats-as phases of movement: movement toward a goal, goal of move-
that the dominant pedal is sustained for a long time after our excerpt ends, while ment, and subsidence of movement. We have described the feelings associated
over it the six extra horns play their hunting calls behind the scenes. There then with these phases of movement as varieties of tension and release considered
comes another wave-a receding one, as it were-made up of the same motives
to be
located somewhere between the ,,lyric,, and ,,dramatic,, poles.
as are to be found in the excerpt, whereupon the music reverts to the hunting The agitated mobility of the upbeats in the wagner is succeeded by the dra-
calls over the dominant pedal. The subsidence lasts quite long, and its generally matic mobility of the accents, and the upbeats themselves are increased in
static quality makes for a successful and much-needed anticlimax. their
dramatic tension by their echoing afterbeats. Against this we may cite
The other thing which must be noticed has to do with the relationship betiveen the serene
tension, the calm movement, of the anacruses in the chopin prelude,
the goal chord (If) of measure 77 and its chord of resolution (V) in measure 76. the calm,
releasing effect of arrival at a goal of motion in measure 9 of the first
This relationship is unlike any to be found in all the previous examples in this movement
ol
book, with the exception of Example 86 in chapter iii. If the reader will turn the'Jupiter" Symphony, and the characterizing mixture of lyrical afterbeats
with the dramatic melodic rhythm-dramatic because of the conflict-in the
back to that example (p. 69) he will recall that the accent on the third level Chopin Etude.
consists of an amphibrach which arises from pivoting. The accent of this amphi- we have also examined some relationships between rhythm and dynamics:
brach falls on a I! chord, and the succeeding afterbeat consists (on the primary the "normal" dynamics of the Prelude; the characterizing, dramatic dynamics
level) of an iamb on the progression V7-I. Obviously, the afterbeat consists of
the-Etude; the unexpected-expected dynamics of the wlgrrer, with their
harmonically of the resolution of the accent. But the chord of final resolution (I) sense
of delayed arrival.
is separated from the accented chord (I!); it does not follow immediately. Now
imagine this relationship stretched in time so that several measures intervene
between accented, mobile harmony and resolution and return to Example 136. AN ACCENTED REST
Here, we repeat, the I! chord in measure 71 is the (initially) accented goal of We now turn to a case in which the relationships among the various factors
motion, a goal, however, which leans on a dominant, and this dominant, which studied so far have such a startling result in the rhyihm as tJ make it at the least
is harmonically stable as compared to the If chord, is widely separated from it in rare, and possibly unique. The example is the climax of the development
section
time. It has to be so separated because of the great stretches of time which go in the first movement of Beethoven's "Eroica" symphony (nxampte t:z).
into the anapest on the third architectonic level. But beyond that, the way in
which it is separated is appropriate to the pyramided grouping of the whole ex-
. Just before the passage to be discussed, two previously established rhythms
have come together (Example 138). The fi.st of these (a), initially heard in
cerpt. In this pyramided grouping, incomplete rhythm has a prominent role. In the measures 45 ff., has an anacrusis made heavy because of temporal relationship;
two third-level anacruses (measures 33-49 and 50-66) we have seen the im- the second (D), initially heard in measures 25 ff., has an erren heavie.
u.ru.r.rri,
portance of incomplete rhythm in creating a feeling of motion toward a goal and because of its characteristic sforzandi. These two rhythms have established
for
138 The Rh2thmic Structure of Music Rfutthm, Mobilitlt, and Tension 7Jg
the passage in question a prior organization-an end-accented one. But, when And what does this anacrusis lead up to? of all things, to a rest-to an accent,
we get to measure 272, the grouping is ambiguous: is it beginning-accented or that is, which is unheard. It is this rest, this silence, which has been marked for
end-accented? The accent which previously followed the anacrusis (cf. Example consciousness by the movement in the preceding measures. So intense is that
i3BD) now occurs in a part of the texture separated from the part in which the movement that this accent is expected to be powerfully stressed. The first beat
anacrusis appears, and the separation makes the expected end-accented group- of measure 280 must be the loudest silence in musical literature; one might say
ing insecure-more accurately, vividly potential-with the result that the accents that it is so unbearably loud as to be inaudible. This powerful accent then re-
in the bass feel like fresh beginnings of incompleted rhythms, and the whole bounds in the repeated string chords, decrescendo, of measures 280-83, as an
passage up to measure 280 becomes an anacrusis of almost unbearable tension. anacrusis to the E minor triad in measure 284. Thereupon we hear a regular,
This effect is heavily supported by the harmonic movement: the diminished- balanced, antecedent-consequent theme in, of course, an iambic grouping.
seventh chord which is mobile (but where is it going?); the six-four chord which In this passage we may observe again that incomplete rhythm can help create
is non-harmonic; and the clangorous seventh chord of measures 276-79 the func- a long, intense anacrusis, and that ambiguity of harmony is a factor in building
tion of which is analyzable only in retrospect, when we reach the dominant of tension; also that a mobile goal of movement (the first beat of measure 280) re-
E minor in measure 280. requires time before it is succeeded by its goal of resolution, if it has been led up to
con
(280)
(685) i
a_
)h# .]*-
'fri -#--* 1#'#
Jh-#

a)
d .2f {
i

J' / / I 'r-T
-l -T
_t' t t' i 'r# ll -J t
f4 f,4- .t --+- f aec c.-----
** 2t * t '-. lJrr J t,t tt
.tf , {* ,:tt[ , -T_
_+ Wi *
-#
-.1.)
#-
-W ---i-f_,+
d o ,t lw-l
\a l9------- --

by a long anacrusis. We may further observe here that the place where the most
stressed accent is expected is a rest, but that the accent, the goal of movement,
rl
with the strongest attractive power in the passage (indeed, in the development
section up to this point) is a piano triad (the E minor triad on the first beat of
a
rylr r \ft.
p
rr measure 284).
Ll I I
=_lF
'ffu
--f-t--
Ptzz- d The piano triad, which completes the rhythm in this passage, should be dif-
+ ferentiated from the piano chord in measure 50 of the Tristan excerpt. There, the
-) piano accent turned into a fresh beginning of the same rhythm; here, after com-
---- -l
pletion of the rhythm, a new one is introduced. It should never have astounded
anybody that the new rhythm is of a new theme: after the intense feeling of
EXAMPLE 137 going some place that arises from measures 272-83 (and what precedes them),
b. (25) .|fi where could one possibly go unless it be to a place where one has not yet been?
4.. c1 14. I

This feeling of going some place, followed by the feeling of being some place
(measures 284-BB), gives one a vivid example of the rhythmic side of the dif-
dolce
/_ ferentiation of passages according to function, which is such an important aspect
lv I
t- vt
I. of formal progression in music.
l. It will no doubt have occurred to the reader that the movement from which
EXAMPLE 138 Example 137 is excerpted ends with almost the same rhythm, but fully expressed
ln The Rfuithmic Structure of Music Rh2thm, Mobilit2, and Tension 141

As we move from the individual impulse to groups of impulses, to larger


in sound (Example 139). But this satisfies us by connecting in retrospect with
rhythms, the importance of melody and harmony in establishing the rhythmic
the electrifying ilimax in the development section; it is not foreseen' For the feeling becomes even more clear.
rhythm of Example 137 is complete; it requires nothing further to make it seem Consider the second foot on the primary level in Example 140. The first foot
finished. It is a moment of crisis, tonal and rhythmic, in the piece. But the crisis has already established the rhythmic norm and the meter, and we know that the
is over with the entrance of the B minor theme. The end of the movement' on the second foot is a middle-accented grouping with a stressed upbeat. The foot be-
other hand, is a moment of triumph. Clarity has replaced confusion. Instead of gins with an anacrusis (B) which moves toward the accented note (A) which, in
the subsidence of measures 280-83 we have the climbing anacrusis of measures turn, is followed by an echo or afterbeat falling away from the accent. Within
685-88. What was broken has now become positive. That is, within what is the foot itself, the anacrusis is melodically and harmonically mobile and the ac-
fundamentally the same rhythm, we have two diametrically opposed characters. cent and afterbeat are static. If we change the foot to read as in Example I41b
Again, the connection between the two passages is one discovered in retrospect- we still have a middle-accented rhythm as in the original (a), but the kind of
and it is one of the most brilliant strokes in the piece; but there was no prodpect movement is different because the anacrusis is of a special kind, an anticipation,
that the defeat of the one passage would turn into the victory of the other' the accented note and chord are mobile, and the final afterbeat is their resolu-
tion; accent, and melodic and harmonic goals of movement, do not coincide. Or
SUMMART EXAMPLE change the foot to read as in d, and melodic center, harmonic center, and accent
Finally, in order to make clear by way of summary the most important prin- all occur at different times.
ciples applied in this chapter, let us revert briefly to a previous example, the b. c. d. e.
Minuet from Haydn's "London" Symphony (Example 140). In the individual
Allegro
lv - vl
f r= =
+
I

EXAMPLE 141
l!l-----=Y:llv - v I ru - vtlv -vl
o
2a. In all cases, the A, whether accented or not, is the melodic center of gravity,
3. the D major triad the harmonic, and the second note the rhythmic. Notice that,
EXAMPLE 140
in a, mobility of melody and harmony coincides with the stressed upbeat, and
accent and afterbeat are static; in D, mobility of melody and harmony coincides
rhythmic impulse, the peculiar shape of the grouping has, as we have seen, cer-
with stressed upbeat and with accent, but the afterbeat is static; in c, melody is
tain aspects, among them accent, stress, tension, and their opposites. Although static throughout, mobility of harmony coincides with stressed upbeat and with
these aspects are analytically separable, the various notes of the impulse are not
accent, and the afterbeat is static; in d, mobility of melody and harmony com-
separable without destroying the quality of the shape as a whole'
bines with upbeat but is separated from stress, which occurs on the accent, and
This quality is of a completed rhythmic, melodic, and usually harmonic move-
the afterbeat is static; finally, in a, there is what one might call a crescendo of
ment which either implies further such movements or constitutes an end of mobility-anticipation followed by accent-independent of stress. The reader is
them. The sense of melodic and harmonic movement is a function of the rela-
advised to consider very carefully the changes of character which result from
tive mobility of individual notes and combinations thereof peculiar to the par-
these minute changes in the relative positions of stress, 'melodic mobility and
ticular style. goal, and harmonic mobility and goal. In each case, the foot is an amphibrach,
Each impulse has not only a rhythmic shape with its center of gravity, its but how different these amphibrachs are !
accent, but also a melodic shape with its center, its main note or melodic goal,
Let us revert to Example 140. The A, though melodically static within the
and in non-monophonic music a harmonic shape with its center upon a main second foot, is mobile after the foot has been completed and has become part of
chord. Each of these centers may be on its own scale of relative mobility, and
a series of feet. Indeed, the mobility of the A, from a higher standpoint, has been
they frequently do not coincide in time. But the melodic and harmonic aspects of
made clear within the first foot. In retrospect, after the first four measures are
the impulse are part o{ its rhythmic shape.
Rh1ttfun, Mobilit2, and Tension 143
142 Ttu Rfuithmic Strrcture of Music
D. Analyze measures 1-36 of "Grillen" from Schumann's Fantasiestiickc. In an essay
over, what has been accomplished melodically? Two things: first, the melody discuss the conflict of accents between voices in measures 1-24 and the opposition of
has gone easily up the tonic arpeggio from D to A and remained there; secTnd, different rhythmic organizations in measures 25-36. In what ways does the rhythmic
the Jescending anacrusis of the fourth foot has set up the most likely directicin of organization affect the character of the piece? What role does the title of this piece
melodic continuation, namely, downward. We expect movement downward play in your interpretation of these rhythms?
from the A to occur in what follows. And it does in the next four measures' E. Analyze measures 258 (last beat) through 317 of the first movement of Tchaikovsky's
Because of the feeling of implied movement toward something, followed by the Symphony No. 6. Write an essay comparing this passage with those from the "Eroica"
feeling that the goal of that movement is being reached, the first four measures Symphony and Tristan und Isolde analyzed in this chapter.
u". uri anacrusis io the second four and the first eight measures have the rhythmic IL Using the first eight measures of Brahms's Intermezzo Op. 116 No. 6 as a model,
shape of an iamb on the third level. On the second level there is also a definite compose a series of variants which, though involving changes in melody, harmony,
rhyihmic shape within the second four measures, that of an anapest. and dynamics (including stress), do not alter the rhythm of the primary level. Be
i\ow, although the general sense of tension-what one might call the feeling prepared to discuss in class the effect of these changes upon the character of each
tone-is ,rot lr.ry high in this piece, the first four measures have a relatively high variant.
tension, because of the feeting of suspended but anticipatory motion which arises
from the initial arpeggio followed by the centering of motion upon A and the
tonic triad. This is *ubtt. tension, when compared with that which arises from
measures 67-70 of the Tristan excerpt, but it is there' And the melodic and har-
monic movement of measures 5-8 of the Minuet satisfies us by completing the
anticipated movement.
The lengths of the rhythmic groups on higher architectonic levels in this.piece
*.i*rrr"r, four measures, and eight measures-are not accidental' In
-two
most, perhaps all, styles there are lengths of time in which the various significant
,.gr*nt, of the music tend normally to take shape as rhythmic groups, a.nd
g*rrpr of groups. This fact is an aspect of form. And it is to form and matters
Io.rn"ct.d-with it, and their relationship to rhythm, that we turn in the next
chapter.

EXERCISES
I
A. Analyze Mozart's 'Jupiter" Symphony, first movement, measures 1-19, on all
architectonic levels. What changes occur in rhythmic organization as a result of
changes in melodic movement, harmony, orchestration, and other musical elements?
B. Analyze measures 78-93 of the first movement of Schubert's C Major Symphony'
Write a brief essay discussing the rhythmic structure of this passage and its relation-
ship to the other elements of music'
C. Ivfake a rhythmic analysis on all architectonic levels (including the subprimary) of
measures 1-25 (first beat) of the first movement of Bruckner's Symphony No. 7'
Write an essay discussing the anacruses on various architectonic levels in this passage'
What must be done in performance to insure that these anacruses will be heard as
such? For instance, what would happen if the grouping on the primary'.level at
the

beginning were interpreted thus: )' lv)l ) J J I I .JJ' t


- -lE=lv -l
Rlrythm, Continuitlt, and Form 145

is an iamb. But notice that the phrasing 2 + 2 : 4 does not necessarily imply
an end-accented grouping; it could just as well be associated with a beginning-
RHYTHM, accented one (see Example 90, level 3); or, as we shall see (in Examples 143 and
l44b), with the upbeats of a longer, anapestic one.

CONTINT]ITY, The first four measures of our tune are rhythmically and melodically com-
plete, or "closed off." We anticipate that more music is to come, simply because
tunes in our culture do not tend to be this short. But we have no notion of how
AND FORM the tune will continue except for the vague one that there will probably be some
obvious connection between what we have heard and what we are about to hear
and except for the clearer one that the already established morphological lengths,
2 + 2 : 4, are likely to continue.
Let us say that a musical theme is articulated into three parts, two measures, The actual continuation, of course, is a mere repetition, emphasizing the
two measures, and four measures in length, respectively' We are talking about closed-off quality of the first four measures by means of an echo. fn retrospect,
the phrasing of the theme. Are we also talking about rhythm? Or let us say that therefore, the rhythmic shape of the first eight measures is that of a trochee on
a whole piece or movement is articulated into four parts which, traditionally, we the fourth level. In this case, 4 + 4 : 8 means a beginning-accented rhythm.
label A A B A. We are talking about the form of the piece or movement. Are we But it will be unnecessary here, certainly, to go through the steps of showing that
also talking about rhythm? measures 9-16 arc also, morphologically,2+2:4,4*4:8, and that
4 + 4 : 8 means, in this case, an end-accented rhythm.
RHTTHM, FORM, AND MORPHOLOGICAL LENGTHS Let us imagine that we are hearing "Au clair de la lune" for the first time, and
In order to answer these questions, the reader is asked (for the last time in this that we have arrived at the end of the first eight measures. What do we know
book) to begin by examining a traditional tune-"Au clair de la lune" (Example about the rest of the tune, and what do we Jeel about the rest of the tune?
t42).lt is on the second architectonic level that the structural pattern ofthe We rnust pause here and examine the difference between knowledge and feel-
(5) (15) ing in this sort of situation. Think, for example, of the usual minuet and trio of
the classical symphony. When we reach the end of the minuet, we know that we
l-vll=/l | -v[-v I
are about to hear the trio, unless the composer is playing tricks on us. But the
1. t---Y-J l-J l=--l2Jl=l etc. minuet is closed off. The melodic, harmonic, and rhythmic issues which have
I v v -t
-ll
3. been raised during the course of the minuet have been resolved. There is no de-
4.
EXAMPLE 142
mand for more music. That is, there is no feeling that the music has to go on in
order to arrive at a goal toward which it has been pressing. There is simply the
tune is established. The opening motive is two measures long. The second mo- knowledge that more (and different) music is about to follow. If, as in Mozart's
tive, which is clearly related to the first and complements it, serves to confirm
Quintet in G Minor, the trio turns out to be melodically connected to the
the two-measure length as the unit of organization. And so on throughout. minuet, we are surprised.
Structural lengths of time such as these will be called "morphological lengths."
Now, at the end of measure 8 in "Au clair de la lune," we know, for stylisiic
Just as there are hierarchies of rhythm in a piece, so are there hierarchies
of
reasons, that we are almost certainly hearing a tune in the form A A B A, but
morphological length. The movement away from the G in measure 1 of our tune
there is no feeling that the melody must move on in B to a particular goal. The
imparts a melodic mobility which, combined with the greater length of the ac-
tune has no mobility left in it at this point. The situation is similar to that at the
cented B in measure 2, causes the first two measures to group as an iamb. But this
end of measure 2 in the first movement of the 'Jupiter" Symphony: on the high-
iamb ends on the mobile note A and thereby assumes the character of an
anacrusis on the third level. The two two-measure lengths become a four- est architectonic level perceptible at that point, nothing has yet happened in the
measure length. So far, then, in talking about phrasing we are also talking about music. We know that something will happen, that movement will occur, but
rhythm. For 2 * 2 : 4 here means that on the third architectonic level there there is no direction of movement established which causes us to feel that some-
thing particular must occur.
1A
146 Tlu Rhytlrnic Structure of Music Rhltthm, Continuitlt, and Form 147

When we arrive at the end of measure 12 in our tune, however' we not only rhythm. But we have no right-at least as yet-to conclude with Riemanna that
know that 14 is going to be repeated; we also feel that it has to be. This is another any other kind of phrasing implies a lengthening, a compression, or a suppression
way of saying that measures 9-12 are an anacrusis to a particular accent or goal. of symmetrical phrasing in morphological lengths of powers of two. Here is
In this case, then, the form A A 8,4 is not one rhythm but two' Or, in terms another case in which it is far too easy to throw out the baby with the bath
of morphological lengths, (4 + 4: 8) * (4 + 4 : 8) is two rhythms; and the water. For what Riemann observed was the norm in the phrasing of that kind of
rhythms are different: the first is a trochee and the second is an iamb. But the music the chief ancestor of which is the song and dance. To see that this is the
whole lline is not a rhythm, though it is a form. In another case, the forrn A A B A case, all that one has to do is to suppress measures 9-10 in Example 142, or
might well be one rhythm (see Example lt 5) and then rhythm and form would measures 10-11 in Example 143; the result is not antimusical, but it is felt im-
be the same thing. Whether or not this occurs depends upon the actual move- mediately to be "odd" or "special." Of course, Riemann felt also that all group-
ment and implications of movement in the music' ings by measures were end-accented, and his readers are often disturbed by the
For instance, if we rewrite the tune as in Example 143, there is established on contortions he is at times forced to go through in order to make end-accented
the fourth architectonic level, by the end of measure B, a definite melodic move- rhythms; yet end-accented rhythm on the measure level is certainly more com-
ment from G to A (secondarily from B to C) which we feel must continue to B mon than not, especially at the end of a piece.
Suppose that we change the first four measures of the tune into the first four
measures of Example 144a. We now have an anacrusis demanding, normally,

4.
EXAMPLE 143

(and D) and return to G in order to form a complete group. At the end of


measure B of this version, we feel more than know; we are in a group rather than
at the end of a part. AAB A is a rhythm' And 4 + 4 + (4 + 4:8) is both
a form and a rhythm.
We must conclude that a form may be a rhythm, but that no form is necessari[t
a rhythm. Alfred Lorenz, who contributed more than one valuable tool for
musical analysis, maintained that form was macro-rhythm (Grosvhlthmi&).1 It is EXAMPLE 144
often the case with important insights into the nature of music that one can
four more measures to close off the larger grouping, and the form A A' be-
easily go astray by denying their validity because they are found not to be uni-
comes iambic. Or suppose that, as in Example 144b, we repeat the first four
versally true. It is much more likely that they are part of an as yet undiscovered,
measures. We should have a 4 + 4 anacrusis requiring a melodic continuation-
more comprehensive truth. A form often is a rhythm, and it is most certainly
an accented phrase-eight measures long, if the morphological lengths are to
stultifying to think of form as a static thing. It is not too much to say that this
remain normal; here, the lengths, 4 + 4 + (4 + 4 : 8), and theJorm, A A B,
book would not have been what it is without Lorenz, in spite of the fact that his
are in rhythm an anapest.
conclusions are not accepted in toto. Incidentally, this book, or any book that
Form, then, may coincide with and be a rhythrn, or it may not. And in the
deals with melodic analysis, has been influenced by Heinrich Schenker.2 One
kind of music represented by our tune, morphological lengths are normally
does not have to accept a whole gospel in order to learn from it. It is probably
symmetrical: that is, the length of an anacrusis or of an afterbeat is normally
very rarely, if ever, true that a method of musical analysis is wholly wrong' It is
equal to the length of an accent, on the highest level.
often true that it simply goes too far.
We may also tentatively conclude that morphological lengths tend to be sym-
metrical: that, for example, two measures of anacrusis will tend to be followed
CONTINUITT AND FORM
by two measures of accent in an iambic rhythm; or that two two-measure Now form is one kind of continuity. Continuity in music is the sense of connec-
anacruses will tend to be followed by four measures of accent in an anapestic tion between any one point of time in a piece and the next point of time. Put in
I Das Geluimnis der Form bei Richard Wagner (Berlin: Max Hesses Verlag, 7924), Yol, I. 3 Hugo Riemann, S2stem der musikalislun Rh2thmik und Mctrik (Leipzig: Breitkopf und Hiirtel,
2 DerJreie Satz, ed. and rev. OswaldJonas (Vienna: Universal Edition, 1956). 1e03).
748 The Rfuithmic Structure of Music Rlgtthm, Continuity, and Form 749
a more general way, if, while listening to a piece, you feel that there is going to that its beginning seems to have been omitted. Immediate continuity has been
be more music than you have already heard, the piece has continuity. Con- obtained at the price of being ludicrous. And the mind is confused about the
tinuity exists on all architectonic levels, and it arises now from melodic or har- phrasing, as it was not in measure 4. This is a fake overlapping. Does seven result
monic mobility, now from rhythm, now from form, and often from a combina- here from 3+ + 3+? what happens in measure 4, however, is a familiar oc-
tion of some or all of these. currence.
In Example 742, the continuity between the two halves of the tune arises from We have tried both links and overlappings as means of increasing immediate
form only. In Example I43, tlr'e continuity between the two halves arises from continuity and have seen that they do not really affect rhythm, except insofar
melodic and harmonic mobility, from form (including its aspect of morpho- as overlappings affect morphological lengths. But suppose we try interrupting
logical lengths), and from the resulting rhythm. The same thing is true of both continuity. Will this affect rhythm?
a and & in Example 144. But this is to speak of "Au clair de la lune" and the A case of interrupted continuity that comes readily to mind is found in the
distortions of it only from the highest architectonic level. classical concerto, in which the resolution of a If chord onto a V chord may be
On lower levels, the temporary cessations of movement represented by the delayed for several minutes by the insertion of a cadenza. The effect of this kind
whole-notes are quite prominent. That is, none of these tunes has strong in- of interruption (and of similar kinds) is perhaps even more surprising than are
mediate continuity, as we may call it; many pulses are suppressed. the effects of links and overlappings. For two things happen. First, suspense re-
Suppose that we alter the tune as in Bxample 145 in order to give it more sults, and suspense intensifies whatever continuity there may be. It may be
several seconds after the lightning flash that the sound of thunder reaches our
a- ears, but when we see the lightning we know the thunder will follow. Second,
there is a certain unreality about the interruption. It is not part of the,,real,, piece,
which will resume as though nothing had happened whenever it is allowed to.
EXAMPLB 145 In one sense, of course, the cadenza is part of the piece because we expect it to
(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (e) (10) (11) (12) (13) (14) be there; in another very real sense, however, it is not. There are somewhat
analogous cases in both art and literature. A painting is supposed to have a
frame; the frame is not part of the painting-but it is. A story with a frame-
for example , a play within a play or a novel with a flashback-includes and does
EXAMPLE 146
not include the frame. Extraneous comic interludes are and are not parts of a
serious play. A ballet in an opera does and does not belong to that opera. And
obvious and immediate continuity. The results may be surprising, inasmuch as so on.
the inserted "links" have no real effect on the phrase rhythm. They seem to be we may test this by performing a final operation on "Au clair de la lune,'
parenthetical (as indicated in the analysis). The listener feels just as strongly as (Example 147). Here two insertions have been made.
he did while listening to the original tune that the first phrase ends on G, that Notice first, in Example 747a, the effect of resuming in measure g an activity
the second begins and ends on G, and so on. Continuity is analytically separate which stopped in measure 4. We still relate, both melodically and rhythmically,
from phrase rhythm, though the two may coincide. measures B-1 1 to measures 7-4. In fact, we group them together in spite of inter-
An even more surprising effect results from overlapping phrases, as has been vening measures. Furthermore, we are more aware of morphological length in
done in two cases in Example 146. Here, the first G in measure 4 is simuitaneous- the real tune than we are in the interruption, which in effect is somethinglik" u
ly the end of the first phrase and the beginning of the second. Paradoxically, composed fermata, a delay. And we may say the same thing of measures 12-21 .
movement starts and stops in one note. Notice, however, that the morphological our sense of grouping has not fundamentally altered; it is only that delay has
lengths are really altered: the sense of entering a new length of time at the be- been introduced. This delay may be easily increased. For instance, substituie the
ginning of measure 4 is sufficiently strong that we feel the first seven measures three measures in D for measures t6-17 in a. Or take the last note of D and add
to be really the sum of 3 * 4 rather than 4 * 4 with one measure in common. a coloratura cadenza to it before resuming the tune at measure 18.
The case in measure 11 is somewhat different. Here, one phrase ends on D Then notice that no matter what may be the case on the lowest architectonic
(but that note is allowed insufficient time) and the next starts on G in such away levels, on the highest level the insertions do not group. There is no accent. Every-
750 The Rhythmic Structure of Music Rhythm, Continuity, and Form 151

thing is anacrusis. There is no real rhythm. The insertions in the example are in- have an opportunity later on in this chapter to examine incomplete rhythm more
complete rhythms. The reader may feel that the last three notes in measure 7 carefully.
(or the corresponding passages in Example 145) find their accent on the first note Before we leave our tune, one more thing about it needs to be pointed out.
of the next measure. This may be true on the subprimary level-perhaps even There are only two (very similar) rhythmic motives in it, and each of them is
on the primary one-but certainly on the highest level there is no group of which exactly two measures in length. Repetition of these motives, then, establishes and
any of these insertions is a part. This is precisely because they are insertions. They maintains meter. The morphological lengths are measured by these motives. Not
interrupt a group, the character of which has already been established' In Bx- all music has this coincidence between length of motive and morphological
ample 747a, for instance, we know that measures 1-4 will be followed by an length. But before we analyze a piece in which this fact is demonstrated, Iet us
afterbeat, not by another accent, which measlrres 8-1 1 would become if the revert to Chopin's Prelude in E-flat in order to examine within a larger scope
interruption were an anacrusis to them; and we know that the anacrusis of the interrelationships among rhythm, continuity, and form.a
measures 12-15 will be followed by the accent of measures 1 8-21-this for formal In the discussion of this piece, measures t-16 will be referred to as A, measures
a.
17-32 as B, measures 33-49 as A', and the rest of the piece as the "coda."
1 3 4 5 6 7 9 l0 11 As is so often true of pieces with a powerful homogeneity of character and
thematic material, the immediate continuity of the Prelude is high. The con-
tinuity of the triplet figure on the subprimary rhythmic level is not broken until
the last three measures, except in measure 32, where strong continuity is pro-
vided by the harmony; on the third beat of that measure there is a dominant
13 14 15 16 L7 19 20 2T
which demands movement to the tonic which begins measure 33. Furthermore,
the rest at that point serves to emphasize the impending return of A as A', arr
(ry ) (v) event we have been anticipating since ,4 closed in the dominant key in measure
16.
b.
ril,..., The rhythm on the primary level also provides continuity. It consists char-
-fr acteristically of stretched amphibrachs which fit the meter. That is, all the groups
start on the third beat and end on the second. The feminine ending of this char-
acteristic rhythm makes it look ahead to an end-accented rhythm which does
EXAMPLE 147 not occur until the cadence of ,4' (measure 49), so that there is constant forward
motion in the rhythm. (The transition from middle-accented to end-accented
reasons-not by another anacrusis, which would make the whole shape of rhythm is made by means of pivoting, as can be seen by consulting the analysis
measures t2-27 that of an anapest, and the shape of those measures is clearly not of measures 45-49, primary level.)
that; the difference in motivic material between measures 12-15 and measures The mobility of melody and harmony is high. There is no authentic cadence
16-77, as well as the difference in length, rule it out. Simplicity favors thlnking in E-flat until the end of ,4', where, also for the first time, the melody comes to
of these incomplete rhythms as composed fermatas, as delays. rest on the tonic.
It may be objected that because our tune is a familiar one, insertions cannot The form is one which, along with the factors already mentioned, provides for
help being obvious as such. Indeed, many a well-known tune, such as "Alouette"
continuity of movement. It is a favorite with Chopin (and, among others, with
or "The Twelve Days of Christmas," actually starts as a pair of phrases bctween
Brahms), and combines the principles of ternary structure with those of binary.
which longer and longer insertions are made as the song goes on and we can
The initial section (r4) is not closed off tonally as it is in the normal ternary form,
hardly think of these insertions as being parts of the tune, though they are parts
of the song. Let the reader think of the first movement of Beethoven's Symphony but instead modulates, as in the antecedent of a binary form. The intervention of
No. 7. Does he not feel that the real beginning of the exposition occurs only when a contrasting section (.8), which delays the expected consequent, insures formal
he hears the main theme, and that the reiterated E's (and, later, C-sharps and continuity.
A's) are introductory and are not in the same sense part of the piece? We shall a The Prelude is printed on pp. 185-87.
152 Tfu Rfuithmic Structure of Music Rlythm, Continuity, and Form /53

In this piece, B is so like ,4 in motive that the form A BA'-wete there no melodic function of again bringing the melody down to the tonic. The passage in
coda-would become a Iarge iamb the accent of which was itself an iamb' But measures 66-69 has already been compared with the one in measures 29-32.The
by the end of A' we are aware of another factor which demands the continuation relationship between the first sixteen measures of the coda and ,B is similar: com-
into a coda. This factor is the one of morphological lengths' pared with -8, the coda is tranquillo; it is a genuine afterbeat.
In spite of the slightly agitated air of measures 29-32, brought about by the In this piece rhythm and meter are almost always at one, as they always are in
metric conflict which almost comes to the surface in that passage, the piece is one "Au clair de la lune." Again, rhythmic groups and morphological lengths co-
of great serenity, and it demands a sense of balance. No disproportion would fit incide. In this case rhythm measures, except in the passage during which the
the mood. The anacrusis of measures 1-16 (see analysis below) is too short for change of rhythm occurs (measures 45-49) . And the effects of melody, harmony,
the accent of measures 17_-49. To end at measure 49, the transition from middle- and form can all be made subject to the summarizing influence of rhythmic
accented to end-accented rhythm having just been made, would be too abrupt' analysis.
In a piece with a different chalacter, abruptness or disproportion or both might
have been desirable, but not here. Therefore, more time is needed to make the THEMES, NON.THEMES, AND CONTINUITT
elements of unaccent balance those of accent in length. There is a coda, and the
But suppose rhythms are more varied? Suppose the lengths of the groups are
form becomes an amphibrach (on the fourth rhythmic level) the accent of
not compatible with the number of pulses in a measure? How will phrasing and
which is an iamb.
other aspects of form fit with rhythm then?
A BAt Coda
tY=-l we have dealt with a simple tune, and then with a relatively simple piece. we
4
now turn to the first movement of Beethoven's Symphony No. 8, in order to
observe in the exposition of that movement the various factors that have so far
In the coda, the two rhythms on the primary level are combined by pivoting.
been interrelated and to see them all in a different light. Before going on we must
And, as a coda to the coda, measures 66-69 do for the end-accented rhythm
firmly establish a difference in formal conception between a work such as the
something like what measures 29-32 did for the middle-accented one, except
chopin Prelude in E-flat and a movement such as the one by Beethoven, part of
that they do it in tranquillo, with the harmony tied down to the tonic and ending
which we are about to consider.
with a diminuendo. The coda's coda has in turn a coda, the two chords of
The difference hinges upon the notion vaguely expressed by the word "theme."
measures 70-7L
The phrasing, as set forth in the following table in terms of morphological
In conlormity with what appears to be the most common practice, "theme"
will be used here to mean a musical idea sufficiently complete in itself that if it
lengths, should be compared with the analysis under the score, because such a
were removed from its surroundings it would make sense as an individual, small
comparison will make quite clear the interconnection among form, rhythm, and
piece. Of course, the kind of sense a theme would make when heard out of context
continuity, as exemplified in this piece.
is not identical with the kind of sense it makes in context; the point is that a
A 4(3+1)+2+2:8 theme can be viewed as a diminutive but complete piece. Thus, a fugue subject
4(3+1)+4 :
B ?ITI^^::Z
Ll4l t
B
is not a theme; a motive such as the first four notes of Beethoven's symphony
No. 5 is not a theme; and so on. The fugue subject, the motive, and the like, are
not lacking in shape-they are recognizable in their individuality-but one can-
A', 4(3+1)+2+2:8
not think of them as little pieces which happen to be imbedded in larger pieces.
4 closely linked with a(*):8(+)
Coda 2+2+4:8 They are more like seeds of pieces.
))-)J-4:B
-l'l
t
To avoid misunderstanding, it should be pointed out that there is such a thing
4 as an incomplete theme. T'hink, for example, of how Mozart's familiar Sym-
2 phony in G Minor begins. we first hear half a theme, a passage ending with a
is not strange that the rhythms of -4 and /' should be so much alike, but it
strong semi-cadence. This half is an "antecedent," and we anticipate a "conse-
It
quent" which will close on the tonic and complete the theme. But no sooner has
is interesting that those of .B and the coda are. B is a passage of tonal movement
the anticipated consequent started than it is deflected from G minor to B-flat
and of upward melodic striving; the coda stays close to home tonally and has the
Rhythm, Continuitlt, and Form 155
754 The Rhythmic Structure of Music
finished. And there move from D to C (42-45), in which key the melody starts to repeat. The accent
major; new material is introduced, and the theme is never
them' We shall meet with which we thought we had reached has turned into an anacrusis.
are oth.er varieties of "near-themes," as we might call
Notice the play with the appearance and the reality of theme so far. The
one shortly.
(or' if one in- movement begins with a real theme, a sport (we might say) in the antecedent-
Now, the Prelude does not haue a theme; rather' itis a theme
of Symphony No' B contains consequent family. It continues with what appears to be a codetta theme of the
sists, a theme plus a coda). The first movement
or parts of themes' It is , self-repeating family. The deflected repeat, however, causes the appearance to
themes. It also contains passages which are not themes
Let us first investigate the break down and reveal the anacrustic reality. Next comes another apparent
everywhere motivic, but it is r-o, .rr.ry*here thematic.
therne-again of the antecedent-consequent family-which, in its turn, is re-
rhythmic consequences of these facts'
first phrase vealed as an anacrusis.
The movement begins (1-12)5 with a theme in three phrases. The
by the second' The move- We now reach a passage which is non-thematic from the start (52-69). It is
is an antecedent whiJh is incompletely complemented
by a movement clearly a passage of harmonic movement with the function of establishing the key
ment from tonic harmony to dominant in the first is answered
but the sharp end-accented cadence (4) of C major. In rhythmic terms, it is an anacrusis to a C major accent. And this
from dominant to tonic in the second,
one (B)' The third phrase dme, the theme is a theme (70-90), an accent.
is in strong contrast with the gentle middle-accented
movement' then, be- This theme is of the self-repeating family, with each half containing sharp
(g-12) is the real consequent Jf tn" first (cf. 190-97). The
of themes) is a place contrast. It will pass the test of standing on its own feet if removed from its con-
gins with a twelve-meu*ir.. u"..nt, for a theme (or a series
or a home base away from which text, although its feet will not be very stable. This is because of the violent nature
of arrival or of departure, a goal of motion
of the contrast here, a contrast in dynamics, in melody, and especially in rneter.
motion is about to occur.
The metric contrast woUld let us know, if we heard this theme out of context,
The next phrase (13-20) is also a theme-this time with an anapestic
shape.
like a that it is of some later-in-the-piece variety. It could not begin a movement. In
But it is of a different kind. It has an affirmative, conclusive function,
its of harmonies context, however, it is definitely stable; it is a place, a goal of motion, an accent.
codetta. It is melodically centered on the tonic, F, and sequence
Like the opening one, this theme has an afterbeat or codetta (90-103). The
(I-IV6-IIg-V-I) is cadential. It is an afterbeat of the opening th-eme. Imagine
more measures' each with a codetta is melodically allied to the theme. Compare the movement E-D, F-E of
it repeated without change and followed by two the theme (70-71and 80-81) with the same fundamental movement (in different
or of a
tonic triad on its first f631' Does it not sound like the end of a section rhythm) of the codetta (90-92). The by now familiar process of change into an
movement? It does start to repeat (21 tr) but soon breaks down
into fragments
anacrusis occurs here too. It is the added B-flat in measure 103 which makes this
onaharmonywithoutspecificdirection.TheF-A-flat-B-flat-Dchordisnota change definite. The conversion of the last four measures of the exposition (100-
dominant of E-flat in tt is uncompromisingly "F-ish" context.
It is a mobile 103) into an anacrusis has important consequences in the development section,
into an upbeat
chord without specific direction. The afterbeat theme has turned as we shall see in the next chapter.
passage-but an upbeat to what? When the harmony moves to a dominant
of
Broadly speaking, form in this exposition is an aspect of differentiation among
b 1:+;, the goal of movement is in part clear' It is something in D' The mode is a passages according to function: theme, codetta, passage of movement, and the
A-flat in the
surprise (38j, for the expected mode was minor. In retrospect, the like. Rhythm follows function. The continuity which arises from aplay with func-
harmony of the prerriorls passage (24-33) is interpreted as a G-sharp
in an aug- tion is clear. We wish to hear themes, and our wish to hear them is guided by the
m.ented-sixth chord. force of tonality within the very generalized scheme of sonata form. But there is
The whole section from the end of the opening theme down to the
entrance of another important source of continuity here, and it is to this that we now turn.
Dmajorbeginsasathemebutcontinues,throughfragmentation'asanon- What of morphological lengths? The opening theme falls within normal
theme; it begins as an afterbeat but becomes an upbeat. The upbeat,
of course, lengths, 4 + 4 * 4. The important word here is "within." For it may be said
weexpectwillleadtoanaccent-inotherwords,toatheme.And,astheviolins quite baldly at the start that thernes are completed within morphological
hearing' It is not lengths but that non-themes are not, unless they have a coda function. The sig-
start out in D major (3s-41), a theme is what we are apparently
The harmony is on the nificance of this statement will become apparent as we go on.
a theme for long, however, because its tonality is unstable.
is printed on The next theme (the codetta, or whatever one wishes to call it) is regularly
b In this discussion, the numbers in parentheses refer to measures. The score
constructed, 2 * 2 + 4, and so might be supposed to fall within its morpho-
pp. 188 fl.
756 The Rlrythmic Structure of Music Rlrythm, Continuit2, and Form 757

logical lengths, but it does not. For while the first violins are playing the last status of a beat. This is observable in the passage starting in measure 28. In this
note, F, of the opening theme (12), the second violins and violas are starting the passage each measure begins with an accented D, and these D,s force them-
accompaniment of the new theme. This new theme does not overlap melodically selves on the attention by their regular recurrence, so that one subconsciously
with the previous theme, since it does not begin until the next measure (13), but counts them. It for analytical purposes, we count them consciously, we do so as
it does overlap in accompaniment. As this accompaniment begins (12), we can- follows (28-33): 7 2 3 4 / 2 (empry measure). Going on (34-38), we counr
not help feeling, quite simply, that something new has started. Ihis new some- / 2 3 4 / (beginning of D major "theme"). Notice that the measures as metric
thing is a morphological length. units are so well established that the end-accented rhythmic figure which is the
fhe new morphological length (12-19) begins on an accent, the final one of motivic substance of the passage has lost its upbeat at the end of measure 33.
the end-accented opening theme. The new theme (13-20) is end-accented on the This loss signalizes the beginning of a new morphological length, a new higher-
measure level, as was the old. Therefore, the accents continue to fall on the even- level measure, at measure 34. The reader should test this statement by altering
numbered measures, but these measures become, starting at measure 12, the odd- the music to read as in Example 148. He will see that the rhythmic figure is noi
numbered measures in the morphological length. This means that the end of the
new theme (20) comes at the beginning of a new length, and a powerful kind of ? (35)

continuity has been established. The new theme cannot be completed within its
own length, for its length overlaps the length established by the accompaniment. lu tu lU
-t -t -l
Notice the reinforcement of the beginning of a new length in measure 20 by EXAMPLE 148
means of the sustained wind chords and the drum rolls.
Allegro vivace e con brio
In short, an end-accented rhythm is combined with beginning-accented (40)

morphological lengths. This means that, no matter how conclusive the theme
may sound in melody, harmony, and rhythm, the music r,r'ill be pushed forward
tenpre It I v v
with great force by accompaniment and orchestration, because the end is at the -
I

same time a beginning.


This phenomenon can be profitably regarded as a metrical one. As was a
pointed out in chapter i,6 meter exists on several levels, although the kind of
notation in use at Beethoven's time limits the apparent number of levels. The
passage we have been considering firmly establishes a duple meter on the
measure-to-measure level. And we are shortly going to be made quite conscious BXAMPLE 149
of the fact that a measure may be reduced to the status of a beat.
For the sake of clarity, let us review the first twenty measures metrically on functioning as such at all; it is a mere unit in the real rhythm, which is proceeding
the measure-to-measure level. We begin with a rhythm which goes across the on the measure level. More accurately, the accent of the rhythmic figure is the
bar line of a 2 X f, meter: ?-1, ?-L, etc. The note F in measure 12 is, thus, on unit, the beat, in the real rhythm; a change of harmony and a shift to a new
a downbeat. If the rhythm here is to behave in a normal fashion, as was the case length such as we find at the beginning of measure 34 becomes awkwardly un-
in the Chopin piece, this F will be the end of something. But a new idea, the ac- clear by pretending, as in the example, that the end-accented motive is the pri-
companiment in the second violins and violas, starts in a rhythm which stays mary rhythmic fact of the passage. The primary rhythm has no accent. It is an
within the bar lines: !-!, 7-2, etc. Therefore, although the new melody (13-20) incomplete rhythm. This incompleteness is the rhythmic aspect of reduction to
continues to move across the bar lines, it cannot come to an end, because the new meter.
accompaniment begins again. The importance of the measure as a metric unit in this piece is amply illus-
Once the existence of meter on a higher level than that indicated by the time trated by the passage which follows (38-51). In the first part of this passage
signature is clear, it is easy to notice the phenomenon which we shall call "re- (Example 149) the rhythms begin by fitting rhe measur.r 1u* in the bnopi"
duction to meter"-the phenomenon, that is, of reducing a measure to the piece). But when we reach the high A (41) this is no longer the case. Th; A
6 See pp, 4-5; also pp. 109-15. enters too soon. Indeed, from that point on through the next four measures,
758 The Rh2thmic Structure of Music Rlgtthm, Continuitlt, and Form 759
question,
where do the rhythms begin and end? Although we cannot answer that theme from the start and illustrates the statement that non-themes are not com-
we have no doubt at all about when the measures begin and end' Put in another pleted within morphological lengths unless they have a coda function.
way, the lengths in this passage are clear in terms of measures, but not in terms of Lest the reader suppose that the last phrase in the passage is really a five-
fi.rt-l",r.l rhlthms (e*..pt at ttre start). There is a tendency, not for feet to measure phrase which overlaps with the beginning of the ensuing theme, let him
measure (as in the Chopin), but for accents (or measures) to measure. The reader
add a measure between 69 and 70 containing octave C's on the first beat. The
may wish to observe the same thing in the opening theme of the movement. effect is to prick the balloon of this powerful passage. Here again we have a re-
congruence of rhythmic grouping on the primary level and _measure is
far less
duction to meter. Take the last four measures (66-69) and count: / 2 3 4. This
in
impJrtant here than in either of the two pieces considered earlier this chapter' means that the following /, the downbeat, is not the end of what has been going
The-o.phologicallengths(38-45)upp"uttobe(1 + 1 + 2) + (1 +1+2)' on, but rather the beginning of something new, something beginning-accented.
In spite of the symmetry Lf th"*" figures, however, the two sections they describe Better yet, take the last eight measures (62-69) and omit measure 66. Now
ur. qrrit" different in function. The first section (through the B in 41) is a phrase, count: 1 2 3 4 I 2 3-the octave C's will be needed for completion, but the
a., u.rt.cedent phrase. It is part of a theme which is a member of the waltz passage will acquire a feminine ending where it obviously wants an accent.
family, part of some alla tedesca tune such as Example 150' The statement that non-themes are not completed within morphological
lengths (unless they have a coda function) can now be seen to mean that non-
themes cannot be completed at all when they function as anacruses. They are
1
anacruses to themes. When non-themes have a coda function, they are afterbeats
3
and can be closed off. See, for instance, the last two measures of the Prelude
EXAMPLE 150
discussed in this chapter, or the last two measures of Example 139.
c.
a.
(40) b. :+ w
etc,

EXAMPLE 151 b. I v -rL-!z--:J


EXAMPLE
In this example the phrasing is normal and the shape is clear' The tune is, in
152

fact, a theme. But in the actual music the entry of the high A (41) destroys the As far as we have gone in the movement, morphological lengths have func-
congruence of rhythmic group and meter, and, by pivoted groupings, extends tioned to provide continuity in three ways: by non-congruence with the phrase
theihrase through two more measures. The phrase becomes then, in lengths, rhythm (12tr.); by reduction to meter, whether partial (23-28, 52-59) or com-
..aliy 1 + 1 * 2lextended into two more. Now it ceases to be a proper phrase plete (28-37, 60-69); and by extension and link (41-45). The rest of the exposi-
for a theme, not on account of its lopsided morphological lengths, but on account tion will provide little in the way of novel procedure.
of its very morphology: it has no cadence' It moves by means of a two-measure The theme arrived at in measure 70 begins in f, meter with a phrase 1 + 1 +
link (44-45) inio the next phrase, but it has become part of what might be called (1 + 1 : 2). After the rebound on the third beat of measure 72, there is an
an ex-theme rather than a theme' eight-measure phrase which genuinely overlaps with the beginning of the repeti-
The extension initiated by the early-arriving high A (41) is important to the tion at measure 80. When repeated, this eight-measure phrase is allowed to
phrasing here, which is as follows: (1 *l+21-2:6) +Z (link)' If the complete itself on the first beat of measure 90, but there is no la.ck of rhythmic
music is altered to make the A arrive politely, as in Example 151, two things continuity at this point for two reasons: first, the accompaniment (violas and
happen: the alla tedesca character is emphasized by the regularity, and measures cellos) begins a new morphological length, just as it did in measure 12; second,
+z-is altogether become a link. AII the peculiar grace of the passage is gone' the new motive (Example 152a) has a different placement in the measurel it
Thenextpassage (52-69) is, inmorphological lengths, 3 + 4 +2 +4 + 4' begins on the second beat rather than on the third and therefore robs the previous
The motivic relationship among the parts is obvious' This passage is a non- phrase of a beat.
160 The Rhlttlanic Structure of fuIusic Rlrytltm, Continuity, and Form 767

The new placement of the rhythm in the measure continues, as in & of the Not all textures, however, support so many architectonic levels of rhythm as
example, untrl the rhythm is shorn of its afterbeat and is compressed into a ; do the pieces we have considered so far in this chapter. Although, as a whole,
"Au clair de la lune" has two rhythms, the other two pieces can each be compre-
-.t., (see c). The f and f meters are congruent on the first beat of measure
hended as a single rhythm. The Prelude, as we have seen, is a large middle-
100, and the remaining four measures represent a reduction to meter. The
morphological lengths of the coderta (90-103) are normally duple: (1 + t
: accented rhythm the accent of which is itself an iamb. The Beethoven move-
2)+++4+4. quasl allegretto
There is a striking effect when the exposition is repeated. If we colrnt the last =63

four measures of the codetta (100-103), we get a meter (l 2 3 4) such as to bring cl. _____rll_____ __*____{_
.) (10) -.--{#
a powerful accent on the first measure of the piece. But the opening theme is end- -
accented on the measure level, and the sudden shift brings an extraordinarily Tamb
P"ffil rn .r ffffn I f-Tl FF+I=F I r--l v i"fttl I |T]
tPW
forceful accent to bear upon measure 2.
The continuity in this movement is less dependent upon low-level rhythm
Vla.
con gord.
'tffrfr, m L l-l-l lt --l-l lTl L, f' -l- L,
l-
-\,l-l1
.') .r
Vc.& Cb
than was the case in the Chopin Prelude. But the continuity is not broken by such Pizz, m lv
a thing as the pause in measures 32-33.It is even furthered by it because of the 1.
2
emphasis placed thereby on the role of the morphological lengths. 3.
--+, -f-fi-
RHTTHM AND TEXTURE a)

In the Beethoven excerpt we have been considering, accompaniment is clearly q FFFIfi I l-l-1 , ffi] rrr -.-l q l'l-l'1-ll I -T-1 e ifffl.'l I f\
important to continuity. What may be said generalty of the rhythmic relation-
L r-J,- L r-,Lll nl--
tL)) L,
qq-l

ship among parts in a texture?


1
*.1
__--_::_-t____f_
tt l.r
io begin with the obvious, accompaniment has the function of accompanying,
and it will therefore normally be congruent rhythmically with the melody' In 1. I . ri tl w \tI \t

that kind of relatively homophonic textr-rre which includes a striking accompani- 2. __J-(, - -
ment) the accompaniment not infrequently appears before the melody' More 3.

often than not it is rhythmically ambiguous until the melody appears' and even EXAMPLB 153

then it may be in itself incomplete rhythmically as a whole or in part' ment, on the highest level, is an anapest the accent of which is an amphibrach,
In the third movement of Rimski-Korsakov's Schth1razade, the alternating tune as follows:
is introduced by its accompaniment (Example 153). The figure continues the
previously established 8- meter. It could just as well have established it- Exposition Exposition repeated Recapitulation Coda
rhythm need not necessarily be either complete or unambiguous in order to \-/ \-/
establish meter. But we do not know how to group it as yet. The cellos and basses
(The anacrustic character of the development section in this movement will be
have only downbeats. The violas have only unaccents (except on the subprimary
considered in the next chapter.)
level); are these upbeats or afterbeats? The drum fits with the violas, but its part
Now, these large rhythms associated with forms are possible because of clearly
does not help to define the grouping. This accompaniment could support an end-
articulated phrasing associated with morphological lengths. A style which sets a
accented rhythm or a beginning-accented one just as well as it supports the
premium on the immediate continuity derived from overlapping tends toward
middle-accented one with which it does in fact go. When the middle-accented
an almost constant pivoting of rhythms. This results in a continuity different in
melodic motive enters in the clarinet, we group the accompaniment accordingly.
kind from that which we have seen displayed in the pieces so far considered in
The whole texture is now quite clear rhythmically, even though no one or-
this chapter.
chestral color in the accompaniment carries a complete rhythm. And it is clear
Such a style is that of most Bach fugues. In these, morphological lengths are
on every architectonic level.
762 The Rhythmic Structure of Music Rhythm, Continuity, and Form 163

seldom important. Indeed, the measure itself is frequently of so little importance the previous case, for we can hardly anticipate the very individual upward-
as a unit that repeated rhythmic groupings are not alwa;'5 placed in the same resolving tied notes. But by the middle of measure 3 the two voices together are
position with respect to the measure and this does not alter the effect, so long as beginning to group, and the music of the next measure establishes the meter
the relative position of weak and strong beats remains constant. In such cases, clearly. With the entry of the third voice, we finally see that the subject is an
counting by two's or by three's is more important than the number of two's or intense anacrusis to its final note (Exampie 155&).
three's to a measure. In both these cases, the counterpoint has created with the subject an un-
At times, the relationship between subject and countersubject is the opposite equivocal grouping by two means: completing the rhythms necessary to estab-
of the relationship between melody and accompaniment in the Schthbra<ade ex- lish the meter, and clarifying the harmonic status of the various important notes
ample. That is, the rhythm of the subject itself may be incompleted or unclari- in the subject. In the F-sharp minor fugue, the subject then becomes rhythmical-
fied, and the contrapuntal accompaniment provided by the countersubject may ly and melodically finished and remains so. In the A major fugue, the subject
be required to complete or clarify the rhythm. retains its melodicaily tentative ending until the last measure of the piece, in
which the melody is finally closed above a repetition of the end of the subject's
a. # rhythm in the middle voice (Example 155c). Notice that this fugue ends on an
'----t-
unequivocal downbeat which is not the first beat of the written measure.
r- \ll
i v v -llv -llt'z 'Yl V -l
-tl a,
b. 1. v- lv
-l l-v -__l
{--i-tttFri--
7ry -+F -1+Y
r
i.
.t-

=rn ni .fll) ) ?J!+fljl b.

'"& .' , : I e nt+ E-*c P Q


1. r.v -J Lll--=J r-J t-J

EXAMPLE 154

t. 6;--F;-
For instance, in the Fugue in F-sharp Minor from Book II of Bach's Well-
tempered Clauier, the opening could appear to have the rhythm of Example 154a.
For stylistic reasonsr we know this is incorrect, because it does not establish a
1ttfiffiE
a) ft--
regular meter. Furthermore, we Suspect from our previous experience that the F-!--=<
+' 2
dotted quarters are suspended, and that the descending melodic motion is thus -+
brought about by resolving dissonances. With the entry of the answer, the two
voices together make the grouping quite clear. The rhythm is as we suspected t.
(&). Nothing in what follows, whenever the subject is present, alters this organ-
iz ation fund amentally.
In the Fugue in A Major from the same book, the rhythm of the subject alone
is more doubtful, and clarification (rather than confirmation) is necessary. The
subject appears to be grouped as in Example 155a. Again, we know because of
the meter that this is incorrect, but our suspicions are less firm than they were in EXAMPLE 155
164 The Rhltthmic Structure of Music
Rh2thm, Continuitlt, and Form 165

In both fugues, the rhythms of the individual voices in the texture together
A-flat-G A-B-flat (-A)
issue in a rhyihm which is without conflict' But conflicts of rhythm often occur
B-flat-B B-C
in some contrapuntal styles. in the subject, in which the movement initiated on the lower level (B-flat-B)
For instance, in Beethoven's Grosse Fuge, ttle subject, as announced in meas- has to wait for its continuation until the upper level has been activated (A-flat-G).
ures 26-30, conflicts with the meter (Bxample 156). We suspect this (although This brings a strong accent to bear on the C in the subject (measure 34) which
the meter has not been established at this point) because of the nervous manner does not fit with the previous strong accent on the E-flat in the countersub-
ject (measure 33). Also, it takes the subject longer to reach A-flat than it
in which the performer, under the visual influence of the score, inevitably plays
cer- does the countersubject (see measure 32). These A-flats and their continuations
these measures. As the fugue proper begins (end of measure 30), we become
are very prominent to the ear.
1. One result of this rhythmic conflict is that we have to hear in units of four
I r- measures: that is, with widely spaced rhythmic "consonances."T Another result
Allegro
of the conflict is stylistically interesting. The music of Beethoven is normally
f t-'41 " ffi 1=aEJfrfr
t Allesro
1+ a J
d t9 !! WJ v3 ft lvoftr v

.fu-(T--
w tv

Eertuprc w
(30)
A )trnrk tr VofV V

tf
t
1. (34)

v
Vla. f f
*17) -1. q. t VI V.t
W -J
EXAMPLE 157

fE r fr,J7
(35)

th nr thD t ffi
articulated in clear morphological lengths and is normally homophonic in tex-
ture. We may expect, then, to hear the conflict resolved later in the piece by means
- II. of more frequent rhythmic consonances, resulting from harmonic clarification,
EXAMPLE 156
and by means of simpler phrasing. In this version of the subject and this coun-
tersubject (Example 156), the conflict is resolved late in the coda (Example
tain that the subject has been syncopated, inasmuch as the countersubject estab- 158). In the foliowing excerpt from the coda, we can hear, in order, the con-
lishes a very clear meter with which the subject does not "fit'" flict (716-24) and its resolution (725*33).
The conflict is, however, not merely a metric one. Each of the lines falls into In the passage of resolution, the odd-numbeied measures are all accented in
two halves with an introduction, labeled int., I, and II in the example. Not only both contrapuntal parts (level 2). The phrasing is clear: 2l-Z f 4, going on
do these halves not coincide for the obvious metric reason; the subject is me- to the first beat of the next measure, because this is a non-theme with a coda
function. And the kind of continuity is primarily that of homophonic music with
lodically and harmonically slower than the countersubject. The harmonic im-
rhythm on several architectonic levels.
plication of the countersubject is V of II-II, V-I (with a proper fugal twist to
The two Bach subjects and the Beethoven subject plus countersubject present
the dominant at the end) divided between the two halves; that of the subject is
what might be called "problems." In the case of the Bach subjects, these prob-
the same, but with more time allowed for the C minor portion (see brackets in lems are promptly solved by the addition of counterpoint. The problem of
Example 157). This harmonic implication is emphasized melodically by the Beethoven's material has to await solution until almost the end of the piece. But
simple anteced.ent-consequent symmetry of A-fl at-G-F-B-fl at, and G-F-E-fl at- 7 See p. 108, above.
D (turning to F) in the countersubject, against the two-story line,
166 Ttu Rhythmic Structure of Music Rhltthm, Continuity, and Form 167

in all three about the initial rhythm which implies later


cases there is something EXERCISES
developments, either proximate or remote'
In the final chapter, we shall consider four unlike cases of rhythmic develop- I
ment. A. Make an analysis of the rhythm of Beethoven's Bagatelle Op. 119, No. 1 and
t'Warum" from Schumann's Fantasiestilcke, Op. 12.
Allegro molto e con brio . (720)
B. Write an essay discussing the relationship among rhythm, form (including morpho-
[^l N^I
(716) /-) )^l -4.' t_ 6) ra. Jd J}^l [.-r
4, 1
J logical lengths), and continuity in these pieces.
== II. Find two pieces of homophonic music in which the accompaniment begins before
?t? the melody and the rhythmic structure of the melody serves to clarify the rhythm
ILJ of the accompaniment. Be prepared to anal,vze and discuss these pieces in class.
w (rcsc. -- -
III. Following the methods and procedures employed in the discussion of "Au clair de
la lune," write a set of variants on folk tune "C" given in Exercise I at the end of
chapter ii. Your variants should illustrate the following: overlapping, inserted
anacruses, and rhythmic structures which are not complete within the morpho-

H
(725\ logical lengths. In each case write one or two sentences stating what you have done

4
l'xt
1, 1
==
H -FC H It
a.ta ,bJ J-J J'J JI and how it has affected the rhythmic structure of the variant.
IV
A. Analyze the A-minor fugue from Book II of the Well-tempered Clauier. What is the
rhythmic relationship between the first and second halves of the subject? Do the
countersubjects enhance this relationship or oppose it? How is rhythmic continuity
achieved in this fugue? Why is the end of the fugue rhythmically satisfactory?
B. Analyze the exposition of the G-minor fugue from Book II of the Well-tempered
2. Clauier. How does the relationship among the voices serve to clarify the rhythmic
3. structure of the subject?
(730)
v
^J JJ J-J J! A. Write a fugal exposition in three (or four) voices on the following subject by Andr6
T Gedalge. Your counterpoint should be such that it clarifies the rhythmic structure of
the subject.
al
f
FT114.+
111
+++ -1
+-- )
B. Make a rhythmic analysis of your fugal exposition.

EXAMPLE 158
Rhythmic Deuelopment 169

companiment, especially in the wood winds and violas, und,erlines the peculiar
quality of the impulse.
RHYTHMIC The two chords in Example l6la accompany the two accents, or the one
distributed accent, leaving the ambiguity unresolved; the viola figure empha-
DE,VELOP- sizes the primacy of the second accent (Example 160a) or the continuous pressure
of the distributed accent (Example 160b), as one will. Both the feeling of fused
rhythm and that of distributed accent have consequences in what follows.
ME.NT a. b.

I:J
It is obvious that the way in which a piece-or a section thereof-begins, has con-
sequences for the way in which it continues. The beginning by no means deter- EXAMPLE 160

mines the continuation, but it does set up certain possibilities of development. In a


b
this final chapter, we are concerned with four examples of these possibilities and
their consequences. The reader is advised to have the complete score at hand for
the study of each example in its proper setting. Vla. ,,,ffi_1_
'1'

AMBIGUOUS RHTTHM
How are we to understand the opening rhythm in the third movement of
Brahms's Symphony No. 3 (Example 159)? The first two measures might be EXAMPLE 161

Poco Allegretto (5)


(5)
-----€
rU -r rv -t rv
tu rv
-r -l
1.
2. (10)

lv -l lv -l lv -l lv - I
---------------.-t v - v V------ r_
EXAMPLE 162

EXAMPLE 159 After the first two feet (measures 1-4 of Example 159), the opening iamb (so to
speak) appears separately (measures 5-8), although its security as an end-
superficially analyzed as consisting of two groupsi an iamb and a trochee (see
accented rhythm is gently undermined by the wood winds and violas, which
analysis in parentheses). But there is really only or'e impulse. As the cellists play give femine endings to the accompaniment (Example |6lb).
the E-flat, we realize that the pressure of the impulse continues in tension and This combination of circumstances gives us a strong sense that the peculiarity
that we are not listening to a true end-accented rhythm. The whole impulse be- of the opening impulse is likely to be resolved finally by an end-accented rhythm,
haves like two feet fused into one group with the first accent as anacrusis to the There would, for instance, be nothing technically impossible about the melody in
second (Example 160a), or like a middle-accented rhythm in which two notes Example 162.rtwould merely be poor; the feminine ending of the opening rhythm
receive the single accent-a "distributed" accent (D in the example). The ac- would have no consequences after its repetition in the second impulse; it would

168
770 The Rhythmic Structure of Music Rhlthmic Deuelopment 17/

simply be replaced by masculine endings; the tension of the beginning would we do not necessarily expect, an afterbeat to the D in measure B. But as the
,rrff., u flabby letdown. Furthermore, there would be no real rhythm in meas- players move from each of the ensuing notes to the next (D-E-flat-E-F) the
ures 5-12 as a whole. There would be no temporal differentiation on the second pressure continues, underlined by the accompaniment. The rhythm, which we
architectonic level. Therefore, the four groups of measures 5-B would not become every moment feel is about to have a feminine ending, continues to be masculine
a single groupi neither would the four of measures 9-12. Measures 5-B "want" until, as we reach the high B-flat with its penetrating cello tone, we realize that
to be an anacrusis to measures 9-l2,but they have insufficient coherence to be- the distributed accent, which was one of the feelings implicit in the opening
come one. rhythm, has here been stretched with a tension more than merely satisfying
Here we can see again the intimate connection between rhythmic and melodic our expectation.
movements. The opening four measures are anacrustic for melodic reasons; they But the feminine ending is changing. It was secure in the opening two feet
imply certain further linear movements, as in diagram a of Example 163. The because of the stressed downbeat and the stationary harmony. The fact that the
,rpp.r movement (to G) is implied by the gap between F and A-flat and by the B-flat-A-flat-D of measure 4 was feminine in rhythm establishes a prior organiz,a-
characteristic appoggiaturas; the lower, by tonality as well as by melodic direc- tion which allows us to feel here (in measure 9) that the same three notes have
tion. Furthermore, there is a rise in tension in the second foot' The double the same kind of rhythm. But the stress on the second beat of measure 9 and the
anacrusis, then, implies an accent of yet greater tension. This tension cannot change of harmony between this measure and the next prepare us to hear the
come solely from an adequate morphological length and from consequential three-note motive come to a masculine conclusion. That is, J-J.|; is prepared to
rhythmic activity (in spite of their importance); it must depend largely upon the change its feeling from trochaic to iambic. Indeed, this is what happens in the
final measures of our excerpt. Notice the subtle pivoting here; it assists the change
4,.
of rhythm. Notice also the beautiful clarity of melodic movement (occasioned
( chiefly by the lengthening of the notes B-flat and G in mea.sures 9 and 10) and
theway in which the two linear movements join as indicated in Example 164.
b

EXAMPLE 163
EXAMPLE 164

nature of the melodic line. And how does that line continue in our fictitious The ultimate G (in measure 12), which itself initiates a strongly anacrustic figure
version? in the next measure, now makes in retrospect all that has been heard so far an
In measures 5-B (Example 163b) there are lwo gaps: E-flat-C, filled at the anacrusis to the next twelve measures.
cadence, and F-D, left open; in measures 9-I2,the F-D gap is filled by the E- In this example, the opening rhythm has two implications (a and D in Example
flat appoggiatura, which leans on the weak penultimate D. This is unconvincing, 160). Each of these has consequences in the ensuing development of the melodic
because the E-flat is a non-essential note. Also, the tension of each of these four- line (measures 5-12). What happens in that development is clearly related to the
measure groups is lower than that of the second foot in the melody: there, the opening rhythm, and it ends with a positive and unambiguous accent.
B-flat-A-flat (measure 4) was strongly accented; here, it is a short upbeat.
In the actual melody (Example 159), measures 5-B are almost exactly like the RHTTHMIC VAGUENESS
corresponding onesin the fictitious version. On the higher level, the rhythm is
not yet completed. we have something like this: .1-? u 5-6. But what In the above Brahms excerpt, both rhythmic ambiguity and the ultimate re-
moval of that ambiguity through development are part of the character of the
follows sets this right and thereby gains tremendously in tension. music. Let us suppose, however, that the ambiguity of rhythm is such that to
In measure 7 we see the new process initiated. First, there is an anacrusis remove it would destroy the character of the music. In "Des pas sur la neige,"
strongly emphasized by a turn about C. (Notice in this context the importance from the first book of Prtludes by Debussy, we find a piece with such an ambiguity.
We are prepared for, though As we think of the ostinato figure (Example 165), we are not sure of whether
of the smoothed-out rhythm-rfl instead
"f .ffi.)
172 The Rlytthmic Structure of Music Rlytthmic Deuelopment 173 \
the essential melodic movement is D-E-F, with F as a resolution, or E-F-D, douloureux,but it has lost its air of vague desolation. Now, it is precisely this air
with F as an 6chapp6e. Analyses a and D are equaliy possible, for the treble which is to arise from the piece as a whole. If the vagueness and ambiguity dis-
melody in measures 2-4 supports both groupings (Example 166a). The heavy appear, so will the character of the piece. One might well say that what is
anacrusis in measure 2, which ends in no accent, Ieads us to hear the ostinato as wanted here is clear vagueness and unambiguous ambiguity. The ostinato needs
ending on D (beginning of measure 3); the rhythm which is complete on the ac- to be thought of and played without grouping, almost mechanically.
cented A of measure 4 leads us to hear it as ending on F. This accented A, to be As we have seen, the accentless rhythm in measures 2-3 is followed by a rhythm
sure, is not altogether satisfying as a goal of end-accented rhythmic motion, be- which comes to a masculine ending in measure 4, but on a beat established by
cause it is at variance with the meter. The meter has been unequivocally estab- the ostinato as metrically weak. Put in another way, the heavy anacrusis of
Triste et lent
measure 2 seeks an accent on the downbeat of measure 3, but does not get it;
1J =
thereupon, the even heavier anacrusis of measure 3 (and part of 4) gets its

pp (20)
a.
or.
b.
rv--------,vtl__lj__Jr l=-!-J l- v ll- vl
EXAMPLE 1651
EXAMPLE 167
(2) r (3)
Li
3
a.
b.
'P expressif et douloureux ?
T 3 3 3 3 (5)
-3-

a.
a)

#, tr^, At//
,,AJl1 4r t!
Fl- I
r''-
b.

etc
ant. ,
EXAMPLE 166 "--vt O-

Iished by the ostinato. Though the grouping of the ostinato is ambiguous, it is EXAMPLE 168

clear that the D receives the accent and that the F is unaccented. Therefore, when
accent, but not in the right place. In this manner, the rhythmic goal of the music
the melodic A in measure 4 is felt to be accented, it coincides with an unaccent
is set up. But to reach that goal would be to destroy the character of the piece.
in the ostinato.
Consider the effect of the unequivocal grouping of the ostinato which would
In order to retain this character, the rhythm has to be developed in such a
way as to be end-accented but without satisfactory accent. We must be prevented
result from rewriting the melody of measures 2-4 as in Example 166b. Now the
from hearing feminine rhythms, and we must be made to hear anacruses
E (measure 3) and the A (measure 4) are both accented, and they both fall on the
everywhere.
metric downbeat established by the ostinato in the first two measures' The group-
For instance, out of context, measures 20-25 would probably group as in
ing of the ostinato becomes unambiguous; it is iambic, as in analysis a of Ex-
Example 167. There would be feminine endings on the primary rhythmic
ample 165. Also, the accented A in measure 4 coincides with the metric accent.
level. How does the composer set up a prior organization such that the accents
But notice that the character of the music has been altered. It is still expressiJ et
in this passage sink to the subprimary level, and the rhythm on the primary
r Permission to reprint granted by Durand et Cie, Paris, France , copyright owners; Elkan-Vogel
Co., Inc., Philadelphia, Pa., agents. level becomes completely anacrustic?
Rhythmic Deuelopment 175
174 The Rhythmic Structure of Music
(measure 2). This rhythm is marked "F.R." (for "fundamental rhythm") in the
The answer lies in the treatment of measures 5-7 (Example l69b). We know analysis.
from measu res 2_.4 what the goal of movement is. Now, in measures 5-7, we find The second impulse, because of prior organization, legato and staccato marks,
built up the tense and long anacrusis out of which most of the rest of the piece afld written meter, could be grouped as in Example 170. But the motivic organ-
grows. This is the crucial passage in the development of the piece, and the con- ization, as indicated by the symbols m and m'in Example 169, will not support
nectio.r among rhythm, melody, and harmony is important to observe here.
this grouping.
As the melody moves up stepwise from A, it creates a series of dissonances with
Rasch, a.ber lofcht. (J
the harmony until it reaches D, whereupon, in relationship to the melodic move- ) m ml m mr(inc. )
'7"
ment from D to E in measures 2-3, it goes immediately to E and remains dis- rlt.
.ry.--jt4l--t--------:
sonant throughout measure 7. In this way, the melody is constantly on the move
toward a goal; it is anacrustic. For a brief moment' on the D of measure 6, the p I'li r. v
a. b. w-
goal appears to be attainable; the D anticipates it. But what is anticipated, an (1) (3) (4)
accented consonance, is not reached. The downward melodic motion of measure
7 indicates that the unsuccessful climb of measures 5-6 has to be attempted again, F R. F
1.
if the goal is to be reached.
2.
Suppose the melody had been written as in a of the example. The tension would 3.
have teen dissipated in a series of consonant afterbeats to the ostinato in measures
5-6, and the only dissonant note-the C in measure 6-would have resolved im- ml m mrtransformed
mediately onto the consonant D, which would then have become, melodically /i
and harmonically, the goal of motion. An accent, instea.d of a renewal of an
H#|+i-

anacrusis, would have fallen on the first beat of measure 7 and the rest of the a
measure would have become almost an afterbeat. The accent would have been (5)_ loicht
'^&
^l
insecure (hence the "?" in the analysis), because of its tied-over anticipation,
and such an insecurity would have been in character. But the serenity of the ris-
ing line of the preceding measures, and the lack of definition in th.ose measures of
1. _t
an anacrustic feeling, would have been decidedly out of character. Once the
anacrustic nature of measures 5-7 in the original piece has been established, the 2.t
3.
listener has the proper feeling for all the subsequent groupings, and the accentless
rhythm can be maintained to the end. ab cd ef
We have been discussing a piece in which ambigrrity of rhythm must be pre-
served. Let us turn to one in which failure to understand the grouping will create tt
an ambiguity where there is none. F trf
(13)

RHTTHMIC TRANSFORMATION
In the fourth piece of Schoenberg's Sechs kleine Klauiersti)cke, Op' 19 (Example 1.
,trt! ! t
169), there is a striking difference in character between the beginning and the F,R.
t
end. Clearly, much happens here in little space, unless one makes the mistake
3.
of assuming a merely whimsical juxtaposition of moods. The piece is very tight,
EXAMPLE 1692
not at all a fantasy or improvisation.
The opening impulse is a middle-accented rhythm with a two-unit anacrusis z Permission granted by Universal Edition, Vienna, copyright owners; Associated Music Publishers,
Inc., United States agent.
and an ifterbeat bolstered by a shocking forte upbeat on the subprimary level
ri

776 The Rh2thmic Structure of Music


Rhytlmic Deuelopment 777 I

Now a false ambiguity could be created here by stressing this group in such
a way as to make it conform with the first group and with the written meter. But eighth-note after it in measure 9 (Example 171). A feminine ending ;f rather
the composer is careful in all his expression marks, and he indicates no stresses than theJ.hof measure 2 is now ready to enter the melody.
here; more important, if the piece is correctly heard mentally, the motivic In the final transformation there is first an extremely intense and rapid review
structure will create groupings without any ambiguities. And only when that of the rhythmic and melodic essence of the first two impulses (measurel0) . Here,
happens wili the piece contain a chain of rhythmic development by transforma- because of prior organization, the rhythmic effect is complex. For instance, the
tion which connects the whole sequence of impulses in a particularly clear way. distance from point a to point D in this measure is a sixteenth-note (cf. the small
The fundamental rhythm (F.R.) is that of the opening impulse in measures upbeat to measure 1), and that from D to c is the now familiar eighth; and the
1-2. Then comes the first transformation. Here, the reader will have noticed in time-spaces between d and e and e and f are, in diminution, those of measure
the analysis (Example 169) that an overlapping occurs in the motivic structure of 3 (Example 172). All this serves as anacrusis to the last three measures, in which
measure 3. The last note of m' , F, is at the same time the first note of rn at the end the afterbeat of the original rhythm (measure 2) has finally itself become a
of the measure. This means that a rhythmic shift is taking place. As one reaches strong anacrusis, the whole being fused into the shape of the fundamental rhythm
the G on the second beat of the measure (an accented note on the subprimary without the afterbeat (measures L1-I3).
level), it is the nucleus of the second unit in the fundamental rhythm's anacrusis.
Not But

EXAMPLE 171
EXAMPLE 170
(3) (10) d e f
when, however, one reaches the D in measure 3, that note becomes the nucleus
of a new second unit in the anacrusis-a unit which, in effect, replaces its prede-
cessor. As one starts moving through the group, then, the G (marked by a in
measure 3) appears to be leading to the accent directly. As one goes farther,
ttv
aaa
a
I
a
V
however, the D (marked by 6) takes the place of the G in retrospect. EXAMPLE 172
It will turn out that the eighth-note distance between a and b in measure 3
plays a part in the process of transformation. Indeed, at the end of this very
ft was said above that the grace note in measure 6 formed an important link
impulse (in measure 4), the afterbeat is distant only by an eighth-note from its
to subsequent developments. If we compare the rhythm of the F-F-sharp in
measure 11 with the rhythm of the D-C-G in measure 6, we can see the connec-
accent, instead of by a quarter-note, as it was in measure 2. This afterbeat is in
tion between the motive mt and the powerful anacrusis of measures 1l-lz.
the accompaniment and leaves the rhythm of the melody somewhat suspended.
In the next stage of development, the accompaniment (pianissimo), which is The lightness of the opening measures has turned into the grimness of the
closing ones before our ears. In retrospect, we can understand the forte shock in
motivic, takes up the suggestion of eighth-note distance in a transformation of the
measure 2. A process of rhythmic development can support not only a preserva-
motive of measure 3 (^'). As this transformation, which starts at the end of
measure 5, moves into measure 6, the grace note (on the beat) provides an im-
tion of character, as in the Debussy Prelude, but also an utter transformation
of character, as in this Schoenberg piece.
portant link to subsequent developments.
At the same time, the main melody begins with the feminine rhythm of meas-
ure 2, but holds the afterbeat (F in measure 6) so long that it becomes an ANACRUSTIC DEVELOPMENT
anacrusis and stretches the impulse, merging with the next stage, which begins Our final example wilt be the development section in the first movement of
in measure 6. The distance between a and D (measure 3) is important here, be- Beethoven's Symphony No. B (see pp. 195-ZO2).
cause this time the melody echoes the afterbeat, not on the next beat, but an The exposition ends, we recall from the previous chapter, with four measures
178 Tlu R@th.rnic Structure of Music RlAthmic Deuelopment 179

of anacrusis with no change of harmony (except for the addition of a B-flat) and lowed by measures 100-103-tutti, fortissimo with sforzando, constituted an
no fundamental melodic motion. It is the end-accented rhythm on the primary anacrusis to the return of the main theme (measures l-I2). Here, this sequence of
level which measures here. This anacrusis leads to its accent as the repetition of events is delayed until the end of the passage, making the first two large segments
the exposition begins. of it incomplete in rhythm. Finally, the tutti, fortissimo with sforzando, in
Now, by means of repeating the exposition, the measuring rhythm of three measures 140-43 refers forcefully to the meaning previously established for meas-
eighth-notes followed by a quarter-note has acquired the specific meaning that ures 100-103.
the opening theme of the symphony will follow. The development section is, In each of the first two segments of the passage, the small melodic movements
among other things, an exploitation of this meaning. are closed out, throwing the attention on the segments as wholes. The A of the
last segment then intensifies the feeling of impending movement.
The bass in Segment I is static. Indeed, it is a prolongation of the bass in
Allegro vivaco o con brlo J measures 100-103, where it had already acquired the meaning of impending
movement-a meaning made particularly forceful by the repetition of the exposi-
.) -l tion. It moves in Segment II when the linear direction of the whole passage can
p l- be seen-as soon, that is, as the F-B motion in the fundamental melody of Seg-
(115)
(104)
ment I is about to continue through E-flat to D. It moves in Segment III when
tll +.
-rFF
the tonal tendency of the passage-the key of the eventual accent, D minor-can
be clearly anticipated; the E introduced in measure 130 above the B-flat makes
an A major triad the most likely continuation. But the accent thus extensively
prepared becomes itself an anacrusis (Example 174).
The expected motive of measures 1-2, introduced with the stress of a syncopa-
tion in measure 143, is provided with afterbeats in the accompaniment. (Notice
the sforzando in the violins in measure 144 and the wind chord in 145.) These
make the rhythm ready to turn into an end-accented one with a strong anacrusis.
Se gmentsJ--18---ll5--, The original pivoted rhythm of the motive now has a strongly stressed unaccent,
and the last half of the foot has become middle-accented. The end of this foot is
ready to become an inverted iamb, as it does, starting in measure 148, where the
melodically detached C-sharp helps the next four notes to lead to the A of the
following measure.
104 115 L2+L'L 1g2 - 143 The melodic direction set up in measures 144-47 (A-B-flat) is continued by the
EXAMPLE 173 C-sharp in 148, so that measures l4B-5L begin as the accent to which the previous
four measures are an anacrusis, but two factors turn this accent itself into an
It begins
by not moving. The weakness of the accent resulting from the failure anacrusis: the measuring quality of the repeated foot, and the modulation to G
to move is brought out by the absence of a sforzando on the first beat of measure minor. The whole passage, then, which began as the goal (accent) of measures
104, and therhythm becomes forward-looking. In the ensuing passage (measures 104-43, has itself taken on the quality of an anacrusis. This quality is now intensi-
104-43) the reduction of rhythm to meter-to measuring-puts the emphasis fied by repetition in the following two passages, measures 152-59 and t60-67.
in the listener's mind on the larger rhythmic, melodic, and harmonic move- Notice here the underlining of the upbeats by means of imitations, and the sense
ments. Example 173 shows a diagram of this passage. There are three twelve- of beginning afresh at measures 152 and 160, only to find each time that the ac-
measure segments in it, the third of which is stretched by four more measures. cent is part of a general anacrusis. At measure 168 there is no such fresh be-
Above the diagram is a reduction of the first segment, by an examination of ginning. We are now in the final stage of this general anacrusis. Before discussing
which the reader can understand the method used in making the diagram. it, however, two more remarks on measures 144-67 may be helpfut.
This whole passage is an anacrusis. In the exposition, measures 96-99, fol- First, the 4 + 4 * 4 : 12 morphological lengths characteristic of the be-
180 The Rhltthmic Structure of Music
(170) (175)
ginning of the development have given way to the more rapidly moving lengths,
2 + 2 * 4 : B. Second, the whole melodic and harmonic motion has been -+-J!i--- lFt--J---F-

stepped up so that there is an increase in urgency here.


a)
'tf ,tf d tf 'tf
Now when we enter upon the final stage of the development section, yet an-
other means of increasing the anacrustic quality is used: that of complete reduc-
A
fb=l'--f-f-t-tr
F-l;-- 'r'i F-i t bi ;zit bi i-sii \i ',r"it
--+----------{++-
tion to meter. The passage in measures 144-67 was incomplete in rhythm and was =E =E
a)
tf d €f ff {
A-ltr+-q---_{---L--L- --a------)B- +ii I-FF
---]--JlL---+---l_
r-'i i i
----t*€J---t----{-
-T|TI
Etl
a)
7 7
(145) I t
- - +.,++ + + + - t?tltlI t-
-#-
u LLLt_+_f , *++lF (180)
F-tt ,)-'ttl d

& 4
d'f '| df- r,ilp- 'F- 4#LlF
,{f
( Ct aaz
^ L ui-*i; lbplcp th*tt* friPlte f]lr.cEe c t-r +-

- a)
"tf
qf ,Y
sf vl
fir 'l-iii n-rrTl .iTrTl 'rii-iiTi
^{.. .
Lb---t------1-- --+---1-t1g- nifl t) l)
a)
r VfrT vt
.Y

(150)
v"l >}i-.+}---l-

2+-I +- t4 ffr- r-H,


(1e0)
d F)x { LJ
d Itppp Fpp
=! t-f_tJ
+{] - ). 2 2222
---itti- tf
a)
'tf d pru/
'{" tt1 l*- + Ect JC+ C+ e-e ?
-, ..: - t
,rl- l- +- tLL.L frfr cE EEcFEE .E
E

EXAMPLE v

a
174 a) t..
piu.f lffi
'+t
neither a theme nor a series of themes, but its structured motion made it "theme-
like." Now only undifferentiated repetition of motive within each morphological
tJ lJ t) l) 1J IJ ]J]T]I.I rIJ]
-....t
length is the method of development (Example 175), and the effect of such
a method is like that of the last four measures in the exposition.
In this final passage, the morphological lengths are first reduced to four meas- EXAMPLE 175
ures. Each length contains only one harmony. Notice the incomplete rhythms of
the wind and timpani chords within the lengths. Each time the harmony changes,
182 The Rtrythmic Structure of Music \

the rhythm is completed, only to emphasize the sense that


the ending is but
another beginning.
move harmonically
The nexistep in intensifying the ana.crusis is the failure to
upbeat, the reduction
at measure 180. Added to ihi* u.. the lengthened, loaded
of the morphological lengths from four measures to | 2 2 : 4 (finally to 1 f
1+1*1:4inmeasu"re184),andthereductionoftherhythminthestrings
measures
to p,rl*., (against the meter, as in measures 96-99). Then,
in
,t*o* with all the force
lg4-87, the motive of measures 100-102 returns in the bass
of its original meaning. Above it is a rhythm which, for
all its apparent beginning- E,XTE,NDE,D E,XAMPLES
end-accented with
accentei quality, has" to be felt in the context as potentially
electric ,il.r..s. It is at once complete and incomplete.
The final intensification
is furnished in measures 188-89 by the completed version of this rhythm, with
its strongly stressed upbeat, ..rd ty the powerful anacrusis in the cellos and
basses.
The whole development section, then, becomes a gigantic anacrusis
to the
recapitulation.
movement,
As movement of all kinds, especially melodic and harmonic
and larger spans of
beginning on the lowest architectonic level, grows into larger
configuration in time
time with cumulative effect, the shape of a piece-its tonal
which is
emerges. And it is this ,lrup., on all its levels of movement,
-gradually
thJ object of the irt which we call "analysis." To practice this art, we
need as
"good perhaps reshape'
many implements as we can get, and we need to sharpen'
them with use'
But because
Rhythmic Structure is, of course, only one aspect of this shape.
of rhythm-perhaps more than
it is a summarizing aspect, an understanding detail as
that of any of tfre olrrer organizing forces of music-reveals
in delicate
which create
well as in dynamic develof,ment ihe fult richness of the processes
and mold musical exPerience.

EXERCISES
first movement of
L Make a rhythmic analysis of the recapitulation and coda of the
recapitulation be-
Beethoven,s Symphony No. B. In what way does the fact that-the
gins on u ,i*-iorr chord affect the rhythm of the highest level?
Il.Employingtheconceptsandprocedurespresentedinthis-book,analyzethefirst
movement of Mozartis Sy*phorry No. 4O in G Minor. write
an essay discussing
form, continuity, and rhythmic development in the whole movement'
A
f iyace.

a)
{0. Iegato

lffi=F
I gi e/"
(
v
-
v/ v :, v-...-...sr
I 2

4 See text p.159

C
ffi
_...ffi
T =-rf--+l-
U
----..-'*
-t rr LJ_,J ,
C
F I +h.j L^ +^
a\tui-f=ff -
C
l- - v. e/'-'---.v
2 (
2
3

$Ft1'-F+tsfPf,t=E
Qt
- --EEr EIIII
lrJ tjj L!-
Ftt-- ),)
F tbt-
--EI-
FF.FF F Fr Fr
r FF'tsF
lt
br. I
--ffi '
t - v''
-
2

3
-
-,-IFl-..-t {_.F-]----]___t_4
a,t a,- T a
LE 4)',*t t ii I"t T-
al b, , C --a :ffi --2e.. -
t-r F- lr -

1 -
- -:-l! 1w - v,rv
2

EXAMPLB 176.-Chopin, prelude in B-flat,


Op.24

/85
--1-f------l--l9:!--te -_r-1rfi ra ta ta tffil5ttr- ft=E=tf2uztr- #*--4 -F---1r{-t.]-..---|.
va t V' V' f;+ '1-
t LJJ r.rt tlJ r E lliJ rl,t !r t+J
crPs". -
be
ffi+
f ? b-)+
{ F-r
-*e It
? t -- --: ffi L+'1 En rn rTl rfl rfi fi rtl M Ififi
-tt
-
-t.]".-l_]-].=H+ --+---a---.t---1-- --1--14-1-t-J -w-4=--J
- .\Jv vv
I r LJ t!,ii , ,- rV tv \J L,
-
2-1 2 J .rF

3 3

A
-l-4-+--# -
'!.J..=g
5 E IJJ IJJ IJJ llLJ lr]r,t
Ci- - l,r,
Fh. F. F lt fi E''1 lTl -1 rrE1
F f -
1#4#{_I- ---.1----o---1--- ---+---1-t----J
e
lv V llJl.l-rrW-V
*t r
-
I v' 2 v
2
3
3
, 'l II!::
)-+
ir HsliJ -
irFl. F*
?t Ff ?+?tt , I tl-,
--=t-:. mm El rn l.l I

FF.FF

iffi
F
'|)
,
t
-
2 2
3
3
a a @
-*
v LIJ LIJ IJJ LLJ IJ L! IJJ IJJ t=U LIJ LIJ
crese. -El IJJ dtm, .tf,
rn ll1 rr
-

rtt 1lt--
!+
lltr
n1 rr rrt rn rn lTl m
-
l-+l
_l-*_r#
-i+Ji---:F
O
r- vr V
t -,{ I

(
-
2

186 787
Symphony, No. 8 1.

{Co I L.van Beethoven,Op.93


ta1 0 - 1827
Fl.

Co Allegro vivace e con brio (J..es)

ob.
2 Flauti

ct.
2 Oboi

2 Clarinetti iD B F6.

2 Fagotti
Coi
(F)
2 Cmi inF
(F)
zTrombe in F

limD.
Timpani iD Fc

Violino I

vl.
Violino IJ

Viola
Ut.
violoncello

Contrabasso cb.

==:::=-.:-- -
2 -:-'"
3.

EXAMPLE 177'-Beethoven, Symphony No' 8

20

Fl. Fl.

ob- ob.

cl. cl.

lA.

Oc.r Coi
@ (F)

Tr. Tr.
(F) (F)

nEE. Tiip.

vl- vl.

Vla.

cb.

t ,11 ,, g
- -

\Oo 2, \-
3
I Fl.
zu

ob.

cl,

F6.

Col.
F)

Tr.
(F)

Tinp.
t. {o
FA.

vt.
vl.

Yh

cb. cb.
'tf

rit. &
Fl.

t.
ritard. &

ob.
Fl.

ob.
FA.

rt.
Cor:
(F)

vl.

vr.

VIL
gl..

cb.: Vc.
cb:

r. _r=gE::(-)sg-g&=1it, _,,-
-r./\,/ -9 -9\J-l!,lr,
2. v-/
3 (\J)

b
o -o
gt
(t G,

fi )

)
I
I
I
Jt ;

Jt t]
D
Nr ,J
,J
dl
r, ) )
t!
E 6 6 "* $ere,q r ji6
F
t 6 d *",ie de,i F sisd

It
.-t|

.-1t
ri ?l
)
it
) )
)

lrl
jci
a€c *65er:eg ; is6 c,
n i g, g !
E
= o o q6i !^ ;
> 6lF,F
Bg

1n 19.1
16r
96t

s 3 *;i FF € r e9r F I
orrer p f ,s Esrgg fi, 9I F
p

t
tr
ff
(

C
C

cI
,I
fi?
Lr G

(
IC

3<root
!':$I;i ErregF'PFF
(
I
(s
(
C I
c I
C c
C I

C q I

( I ( C
C
C I
C
C I(
I
(= c C
.t (
C

I
C
(
(
I
(
C C
C
s

F1.

ob.

cl.

Fg.

cor.
(F)

Tr. Fl.
(F)
ob,
?inr,
cl

vl. Fg.

vl.
Vl..

Vlr-

cb,

t 'i:,,u
\./\J\-/ - u -
2
3
\t

Fl, l.
Fl.
ob.
ob.
cl.
cl.
lg.
FA.

Cor
(r) Cor'
(F)
Tr.
(F) tr.
(F)

vl. vl.

Yla.
Vli.

Vc.
e cb. e cb-

!" - lr-rw - rllgrrr \'/ - , L!!t ,L/-,,v -,,


t -3lgt1:/
2 vL/v\,i ---
3.

{!o
\
J) P
l{

# )
) F

) D
D
U
L
I J
\ )

t;
)

t )
I

t ) \ -)
I I

:E n
O
i
O ;
G I
-
Ji
,t) s d *u 50 *eF , iq
D

D D
G'
!
)
E
g
6l

t
)
t
) ) t
D

ll-
jljJ*i^i^
j stJci F; ;iibi,:^;
EoukJe>
';
s: 6{ G'

798 799
r( bt {b, b,
l" )
)
bq b sb. -
!, b' ) ) .l

q
b rb, l*
h
i i
D )

rl \ bb
)
)
fb
i, b, b )

r'l t\ l*
b b :) )

r6d**,!aieF ts
tjE o

)
.t

b( b, h )
!

u$ b ) )
)
5{ !, )
.t

D )
5d b -\, D

bd >' \ b
,l
l ir
c 6 d ,s 5o iE g E6d,65edei F
id
>5 ; ..i c;

200
20/
N rr.
t\) r1.

ob.
ob.

cl
ol.

rE.
Fg.

Cor.
(F)
Cor.
(d
T!.
(F)

Tr'
(F)

nbP.

tinp.

vt-

Yl.

Vlr.

{la.

ecb
1t

2.
\J
3.

-t

EXPOSITION EXPOSITION (repeat)

1 - t2 13 - 37 38 - 61 62- 69 70 - 90 91 -103 1-103

4d,.
/
4b.
-:
5.

DEVELOPMENT RECAPITULATION CODA


104 - 109 I t90 - 19? 1r8 - zog zr.o - 2g4 ,3s -----30f G;:]?B
r_--.ll-.ll-l as in exposition.
>2,
4a

4b

5b

t$ EXAMPLE l78.-Rhythmic analysis of the first movement of Beethoven's Symphony No. 8


v
$.J

I
LIST
OF
SYMBOLS INDE,X

Accent Nora: The entry "Rhythm" interrelates the key terms used in this book.

Weak beat or group Accent: Bar line, 88 f'


(-X- Felt but unperformed beats or groups; or a parenthetical beat or defned,T {, Beat placement, influence of on grouping, 15
)
Ji.rthe.r defned, 118_f .,125 ff. (see esp. 137) Beginning-accented groups, 10. Sei alsoDactyl;
group distributed, 168 ff. Trochee
v Beat or group at first presumed to be accented, but retrospectively Accented rest, 137 ff.
Accentuation, forced, 92 f. Character of particular rhythrns, 26 ff,
understood to be weak
Accompaniment: Closed groups, 39
\L Beat or group at first presumed to be weak but retrospectively under- function of, 160 ff. Closed trochee, 30
stood to be accented influence of on rhythm, 17 Composite subgroups, 22 f.
Accent fused to a weak beat or group Afterbeat, 125 ff. (see esp. 137) Compound groups, 65
Ambiguity, rhythmic (ambiguous grouping), Consonance, metric, 108
Weak beat or group fused to an accent 25, 32 tr.,54 ff., 168 ff.; introduced,9
Continuitv:
Fused weak beats or gtoups Amphibrach: and rhythm, 144 ff. (see esP. 155)
introduced, 6 and form, 147 ff.
Extended anacrusis on louer architectonic leuels, 78 ff,, 23 ff., 53 f ., and style, 161 fl.
-J"-'--" Extended anacrusis at first presumed to be an accent 54 (inverted) interrupted, 149
on higher architectonic leuels, 80 ff,
group: / ; stressed weak beat or group: J)
Stress (stressed accent or "attempted," 722 I)actyl:
t-J Grouping, manilest or dominant; except where the analysis is above "truncated," 135 f. inlroduced, 6
the example, when it indicates the latent grouping Anacrusis, 9,73 f .,125 Il., 177 ff.: on louer architectonic leuels, 78 {1,,23 ff,, 43 fl,,
extended, 50 f ., 129 tr. 45 tr., 31 (inverted)
Grouping, latent; except where the analysis is above the example, proximate vs, remote, 91 on higher architectonic leuels,75 ff.
when it indicates the manifest grouping Analysis, rhythmic: Development:
as interpretation, 9 anacrustic, 177 ff.
Grouping without a definite conclusion or which blends into another rhythmic, 168 ff.
and style, 57,706 ff., 119 f.
grouping summarizing aspect of, 717, 723, 182 Dissonance, metric, 108
Grouping without a definite beginning point Analytical devices. See Reduction; Variation "Distributed" accent, 168 ff.

Overlapping or pivoted rhythmic groups Anapest: Dominant organization (or rhythm), 73, 17 f.
,tt
introduced, 6 Duration, influence of on rhythm, 10 f., 13 f.,
U:rt Splitting of one rhythmic level into two on louer architectonic leuels, 78 fl., 23 [1., 57 f.,
24 ff ., 29 , 67 . See also Temporal relationships
52 f., 32 (inverted)
t

on higher architectonic leuels, 68 ff.


Dynamics:
influence of on rhythm, 16
Anticipation of future and evaluation of past, interrelationship between and rhythm, 137
62, 120 "forced" vs. "natural," 727 {1.
Architectonic levels:
introduced,2 f. Echo, introduced, T6
relationship among, 120 End-accented groups, 70. See also Anapcst; Ianill
Articulation and structure, 17 f. "Ex-theme," 158

205
204
206 Tlte Rlythmic Strtuture of Music
Index 207
Form: Middle-accented groups, 70, incomplete, 79, l3g,
See a/.ro Amphi- f ., I57 (see also influence ofon rhvthr 2o.f
and continuity, T4T ff, brach Kecluctron to meter).I49 '' 24 ff'' 29, 39
and rhythm, iii
, tii' n. (see esp. 1 55) Mobility,
117 tr., 140ff. related to:
influence or on *,iti.fi
and syncopation, 101'f,
Fused groups, 64 f.
Morphological lengrhs, 144 ff. anticipation of future and evaluation of
past, 62,_120 (sec also Dominant o.eini- Structural gap, 22, 91, 170
Goldthwaite, W. S., 108 f. zationl Latent organization; p.ioi o._ Style: "
Non_-corgruence of rhythm and meter, 89 fl., ganization) and analysis, 106 tr.. 119 f.
Gombosi, O., 108 f. 164 tr. beat placement, 15 and continuity, 161'ff.
Group inversion,2g f . "Non-theme" and continuity, ff, character, 34
753 Subprimary rhythmic levels, 2
Grouping, 1, 8 ff., 68 ff. continuity, 144 ff, Superior rhythmic levels, 2
Open groups, 39 duration, temporal relationships, 13
-" ",
f., Suspension, 99 ff.
Harmony, influence of on rhythm, 14, 19 f ,. 39. Ornament_ation, influence of on rhythm, 21 f., . 24 ff., 29, 61: n6, M7
dynamics, 16, 137 Symbols, list of, 204
,_ 51 f., 67. See also Mobility' 44, 49, 54
Hemiole, 4, 81, 88, 89 Overlapping, 148. Sce also Morphological form, 139, 144ff. Syncopation, 31, 32:
lengths
harmony, 74, 19 f ., 39, 5l t., 61 defned,99 ff.
instrumentation, I 6, 6'I and melodic-rhythmic structure, 102 ff.
Iamb: rnterpretation and performance, 1, g,
introduced, 6 Pace, 3 n.7,66 11, and stress, 101 f.
15, 35, 164
on lower arehitectonic leaels, 77 ff,, 29 ff,, 47 ff., Partial inversion, 32 .melody,
14, 18 f .,24 ff., 29, 36 f . Tempo, 3; influence of on rhythm, 22 f.
ff. (inverted)
49 Performance and rhythmic structure, g, ll,75, meter, 42, 96 f .
on higlur architectonic leuels,70 ff. 35, 764 Temporal relationships, influence of on rhvthm.
mobility, 117 tr., 140 tr.
Incomplete rhythm, 6, 79, 85, 139, 749 f ., 157 . Pivot, rhythmic, 23, 27, 62 tr. morphological lengths, 144 ff.
61,736, 147. See'"i*
Principle of
M;;i; fi;t;;i#;l
See also Reduction to meter ornamentation, 2l f ,, 44, 49, 54
Primary rhythmic level, 2 Tension, 125 ff.
Inferior rhythmic levels, 2 stress, 8, 20 f ,, 24 ff., 29, 39'
Prior organization, effect of on subsequent or- suspension, 99 ff.
Instrumentation,_ influence of on rhythm, 16, ganization, 13,33,171
Texture and rhythm, 160 ff.
67, See also Style, and analysis syncopation, 37,32, gg tr., I02 ff, Theme:
Pulse, 3 f, tension, 125 ff.
Interpretation and rhythm, I and continuity. 153 ff.
Pyramidal groups, 65 ff. t.*jgl._(including accompaniment) 17, incomplete, 153
Inversion of grouping, 21, 32 (patial) 160 tr
Inverted groups: Reduction, as analytical device, 69 f .,84 tr,,123 tie, 99 ff. Tie, 99 fl.
amphibrach, 54 Reduction to meter, 156f, See a/so Incomplete development of, 168 ff. Transformation, rhythmic, 174 ff.
anapest, 32 rhythm individualiry of, 22 Trochee:
dactyl, 31 summarizing aspect of, 117, lg2 introduced, 6
iamb, 49 ff. Rest, accented, 137 tr.
transformation of, 174 ff, on l.9wy, architectonic leuels, 17
trochee, 40 Reversal, rhythmic, 77 f. ff,, 29 ff., 3g ff
Riemann, H., 147 30 _(closed), 40 f. (inverted )
Rhythm: 19 fl.,
on higfur architectonic
Rubato, 8 leuels, i4 f.
Latent organization (or rhythm), 13, and other musical elements, 1
18
as one of the temporal phenomena: pulse, Upbeat.
Links,148 meter, tempo, rhythm, 2 ff. Sachs, C., 108 ,Sea Anacrusis
Lorenz, A., 146 defned as grouping, 6 f. Schenker, H.,70, 146
Vagueness, rhythmic, 171 ff,
Lower architectonic levels, 12 ff. elements of (sea Accent; Afterbeat; Anacrusis; Stress:
Echo) Variation, as analytical device, 73 ff., 5g, 776,
defned, S
architectonic nature of: 123 tr., 143, 167'
Melgdi,c-rhythmic structure and syncopation, inferior levels, 2
102 tr.
primary level, 2 (see also Composite sub-
M9l^od1', influence of on rhythm, 14, IB f.,24 ff., groups; Compound groups;
- Metric
29. See also Mobility
_ Equjvalence, Principle of)
Meter: lower levels, 12 ff.
architectonic in nature, 2 f. higher levels, chaps. iii-vii (see also Ftsed
defined, 4 f. groups; Non-congruence of rhythrh and
and rhythm, 42, 96 f,, 88 tr. meter; Pivot; Pyramidal groups; Re-
latent, 89 ff. versal)
reduction to, 156 f. types of:
shifting and stable groups, 94 ff. be8nning-accented (see Anapest ; Iamb)
Metric: middle-accented (see Amphi6rach)
consonance, 108; dissonance, 108 end-accented (s e e D actyl; Trochee)
crossing, 81, 89, 106 fl. closed and open, 39
ambiguous, 54 ff., 168 ff.
Metric Equivalence, Principle of, 22 f, vague, 171 ff.
=T

Index of Music 209


Grosse Fuge (Examples 156-58), 164-66 Example:
Overture to Egmont, Allegro (Example t.!6f in D Maior, ii (Example
52a),47 fi1:.,et
String Quartet Op. 130, AIla danza Brahms, J,:
tedesca (Example 57), 44 Examples:
String Quartet Op. 131, iv (Example 706) Concerto in D Major for Violin, ii (Ex-
52 f. ample 60a), 46
Symphony No. 2 in D Major, Scherzo Sonata in A Major for Violin and Piano,
(Example 59),45 ii (Example 57c),40f.
Symphony No. 3 in E-flat Major, i (Ex- String Quartet in A Minor, Finale (Ex-
amples 137-39), 137 tr.; Scherzo (Ex-
INDT,X amples 88-89), 71 ff.
Symphony No. 4 in B-flat Major, Scherzo
amples 96-97), 81 tr.
Symphony No. 3 in F Major, iii (Examples
159-64), 168 ff.
(Example 106), 92 t. Symphony No. 4 in E Minor, iii (Example
oF' Symphony No. 5 in C Minor, iv (Example
64), 48 f.
112), 99 f.
Variations on a Theme by Haydn Op. 56a,
Symphony No. 8 in F Major, i (Examples 6th Variation (Example 49),39
MLISIC 1
ii
48-52, 77 3-7 5,
(Example 110),97
177

Symphony No. 9 in D Minor, Scherzo


-7 8), 1 53 ff ., 777 tr.
f.
; ReJerences:
Horn Trio, i, 53
Intermezzo Op.76 No. 3, 83
(Examples 93-95),77 tr. Piano Trio Op. 101, i, 52
ReJerences: Rhapsody Op. 119 No. 4, 40
"Archduke" Trio (Op. 97), Scherzo, 42 String Quartet in A Minor, iii, 80
Bach, J. S.: Orchestra Suite No. 2 in B Nlinor, i, 54; Concerto in D Major for Violin, iii, 68 String Quintet Op. 88, ii, 51
Examples: Rondeau, 53 Leonore Overture No. 3, 47 Symphony No. 1 in C Minor, ili, 64; iv, 49
Brandenburg Concerto No, 5, i (Example Orchestra Suite No. 3 in D Major, iv, 47; Piano Trio Op. 1 No. 3, iii, 54; iv, 40 Symphony No. 2 in D Major, iii, 45, 51
74),55 Gavotte, 53 Piano Trio Op. 97 ("Archduke"), iii, 80 Symphony No. 4 in E Minor, i, 48
Orchestra Suite No. 4 in D Major, lst Serenade Op. 25, vi, 40 Variations on a Theme by Handel, i, 40;
Chamber Suite No. 1, 2d Bourr6e (Bx-
ample 73a), 54 Bourr6e, 54 String Quartet Op. 18 No. 3, ii, 49;iii,54 ii, iii, vi, 57
English Suite No. 1 in A Major, 1st Bour- Sonata in C Minor for Unaccompanied String Quartet Op. 18 No. 4, i, 47;ii,51, Variations on a Theme by Paganini, iii, 54
r6e (Example 85), 68 Cello, Sarabande, 43 52 Exercises:
English Suite No. 3 in G Minor, Prelude Sonata No. 6 for Violin and Clavier, iii, 45 String Quartet Op. 95, iii, 45 Intermezzo Op. 116 No.6, 143
(Example 68a), 51 Toccata in G Minor for Clavier, 74 String Quartet Op. 127, iii, 70, 80 Sonata in D Minor for Violin and Piano,
French Suite No. 4 in B-flat Major, Sara- Well-tempered Clavier, Book I, Prcluclc String Quartet Op. 130, i, 57; ii, 64 i, 116
bande (Example 51a), 40 f. XII, 54; XX, 45
String Trio Op .9No. 1,i,65 String Quartet in A Minor, i, 116
Mass in B Minoi, "Laudamus Te" (Ex- Symphony No. 1 in C Major, ii, 51, 54 Bruckner, A.:
Exercises:
ample 37c, d, e), 30 f . Symphony No,, 3 in E-flat Major, i, 57;
Chamber Suite No. 4 in D Major, iii, 115 Scherzo, 83, 93
Example:
Partita in D Minor for Unaccompanied Prelude and Fugue in A Major for Organ, Symphony No. 5 in C Minor, i, 48, 753;
Symphony No. 9, i (Examples 107-9),
Violin, Chaconne (Example 66b), 49 f . Fugue, 115 94 tr.
Passion According to St. Matthew (Ex- iv, 93
Well-tempered Clavier, Book II, A-lvlinor Symphony No. 7 in A Major, i, 150; iii, 43
Exercise:
ample 78), 62 f. Fugue,167 Symphony No. 7, i, 142
Six Little Preludes, 3d Prelude (Example Symphony No. 9 in D Minor, i, 49; Scher-
Well-tempered Clavier, Book II, G-I\4inol zo, 5
54),42f. Fugue, 167 Chopin, F.:
Exercises:
Twelve Little Preludes, 5th Prelude (Ex- Bart6k, B.: Examples:
ample 53b), 42 Bagatelle Op. 119 No. 1, 116, 167
Exanple: Piano Sonata Op. 111, i, 116 Etude Op. 10 No. 9 (Examples 134-35),
Well-tempered Clavier, Book II, Fugue in 127 tr.
A Major (Example 155), 162tr. Piano Concerto No. 3, iii (Exarnples 120- Symphony No. 3 in E-flat Major, i, 143
22), 106-8 Symphony No. 8 in F Major, i, 182 Mazurka Op. 41 No. 3, 43
Well-tempered Clavier, Book II, Fugue in Mazurka Op. 50 No. 2 (Example 72b),
F-sharp Minor (Example 154),162tr. ReJerences: Berlioz, H
Sonata for two Pianos and Percussion, i, 48 53 f.
Example:
References:
String Quartet No. 3, Mazurka Op. 56 No. 1 (Example 56&),
Brandenburg Concerto No. 1, iii, 51 89 Symphonie fantastique, iii (Example 66a), 43 f.
Brandenburg Concerto No. 2, i, 64 Exercises: 49
Piano Sonata (1926), i, 116 Nocturne Op. 48 No. 1 (Example 67a),50
Brandenburg Concerto No. 3, i,48 ReJerence:
Prelude Op. 24 No. 1 (Example 46), 36 f .
Brandenburg Concerto No. 4, ii, 52 Violin Concerto, i, 176 Symphonie fantastique, ii,73 f Prelude Op. 24 No. 4 (Example 46), 36 f .
English Suite No. 4, Sarabande, 43 Beethoven, L. van: Binchois, G.: Prelude in E-flat Op.24 No. 19 (Examples
French Suite No. 2 in C Minor, Gigue, 48 Examples: Exercise: 732-33,176),125 ff., 151 ff.
French Suite No. 5 in G Major, Bourr6e, Bagatelle Op. 119, No. 10 (Example 48), 39 t'De plus en plus," 116 ReJerences:
681 Gavotte, 64, 70; Gigue, 75 Concerto in B-flat for Piano, iii (Exanrple Borodin, A.: Etude op. 10 No. 11, 51
Fugues, 161 f, 37a, b),30

208
-Y-

270 The Rhythmic Structure of Music Index of Music 277

Chapin, F.-Continued ReJerence: Symphony No. 45 in F-sharp Minor Piano Quintet in E-flat Major (K. 452),
References-Continued Symphony in D Minor, i, 47 ("Farewell"), Finale, 120 iii, 7 5
Etude op. 25 No. 5, 40 Gedalge, A.: Symphony No. 94 in G Major ("Sur- Quintet in G Minor (K. 516), Trio, 145
Fantasie-Impromptu Op. 66, 70 Exercise: prise"), ii. 40; iii, 64 Serenade in D Major (K. 239), i, 47 ; iii, 54
Mazurka Op. 50 No. 3, 45 Fugue subject, 167 Symphony No. 97 in C Major ("Salo- String Quartet in G Major (K. 387), iii,
Nocturne Op. 9 No. 2, 75 mon"), Minuet, 71; iv,57 48
Nocturne Op. 15 No. 3, 48 Handel, G. F.: Symphony No. 101 in D Major ("The String Quartet in D Minor (K. 421), iii,
Nocturne Op,37 No. 1, 40 Examples: Clock"), i, 74 40; iv, 68
Polonaise Op. 40 No. 1, 83 Concerto Grosso No. 1, iv (Example 76), Symphony No. 104 in D Major ("Lon- String Quartet in C Major (K. 465), iii,
Polonaise Op. 71 No. 1, 40, 49 56 f. don"), i, 51 65
Valse Brillante Op. 34 No. 3, 74 Concerto Grosso No. 2,iii,43 Exercises: String Quartet in D Major (K. 499), iv, B0
Waltz Op. 18, 45 Concerto Grosso No. 3, Polonaise (Ex- String QuartetOp.TT No. 2, ii, 116 Strina quartet in D Major (K. 575), iii, 43
Waltz Op. 64 No. 1, 43 ample 680), 51 Symphony No. 97 in C Major ("Salo- Symphony No, 27 in G Major (K. 199),
Waltzes, T3 Concerto Grosso No. 8, iii (Example 50a), rfion"), Minuet, 87 iv, 52
Exercises: 39 f. Symphony No. 104 in D Major ("Lon- Symphony No. 33 in B-flat Major (K.
Etude op. 10 No. 10, 116 Messiah, "Ev'ry Valley" (Examples 113- don"), iii, 116 319), i, 52; iv, 49
Etude op. 25 No. 8, 116 t4), 100-102 Humperdinck, E.: Symphony No. 35 in D Major (K. 385),
Prelude Op. 28 No. 1, 87 ReJerences: ReJerence:
iv,74
Concerto Grosso No. 7, iii, 52 Hiinsel und Gretel, 36
Symphony No. 38 in D Major (K. 504),
Concerto Grosso No. 4, iii, 48 ii, 83
Debussy, C.: Concerto Grosso No, 5, v,40 Symphony No. 39 in E-flat Major (K.
Examples: Concerto Grosso No. 6, v, 45l Musette, 52 Mendelssohn, F.: 543), i, 52; iii, 45
Grosso No. 7 , i, 651iii,70 Example: Symphony No. 40 in G Minor (K. 550), i,
Nocturnes for Orchestra, "Nuages" (Ex- Concerto
ample 77), 57 Concerto Grosso No. 9,ii,75; Gigue' 68 Symphony No. 4 in A Major ("Italian"), 153 f.
Pr6ludes, Book I, "Des pas sur la neige" Exercise: i (Example 80), 64 Symphony No. 41 in C Major (K. 551)
(Examples 165-68), 171 tr. Concerto Grosso No. 7, v ,776 Mozart, W. A.: ('Jupiter"), iii, 51
Exercises:
ReJerence: Haydn, F. J.: Examples:
Sonata in A Major for Violin and Piano
Nocturnes for Orchestra, "F€tes," 48 Examples: Eine kleine Nachtmusik (K. 525), iv (K. 305), i, 116
Dufay, G.: Piano Sonata No. 37 in D Major, i (Ex- (Example 65), 49 Symphony No. 40 in G Minor (K. 550), i,
Example: ample 63), 48 Horn Quintet (K. 407), ii (Example 526), 182
Missa SanctiJacobi, Kyrie (Examples 123- String Quartet Op. 33 No. 3, iv (Example 41 f. Symphony No. 41 in C Major (K. SSI;
25),109 tr. 47),38 Serenade for Winds (K. 361), i (Example ('Jupiter"), i,
742; ii,57; Minuet, 87
Dvoi6k, A.: String Quartet Op. 54 No. 3, iii (Example 67b),50 Variations on "Ah, vous dirai-je, maman"
ReJerences: 50r), 40 String Quartet in G Major (K, 387), Minu- (K. 26s), 58
String Quartet Op. 51, iv, 80 Symphony No. 92 in G Major ("Oxford"), et (Example 51b), 40 f .
String Quartet Op. 96, i, 68 Finale (Examples 31-33),27 f. String Quartet in C Major (K. 465), iv Purcell, H.:
Symphony No, 94 in G Major ("Surprise") (Example 75), 55 ff.
Symphony No. 5 in E Minor, iv, 74 Example:
iv (Examples 81-84), 65 ff. String Quintet in C Minor (K. 406), i (Ex- Sonata for Two Violins and Continuo
Symphony No, 97 in C Major ("Salo- ample 91), 76 ("Golden"), i (Example 55),43
Faur6, G.: mon"), Minuet (Examples 98-101), Symphony No. 21 in A Major (K. 134),
83 ff.; Finale (Example 71), 53 Minuet (Example 69), 51 f.
Reference:
Symphony No. 40 in G Minor (K. 550), Ravel, M.:
Piano Quartet Op. 15, i, 48 Symphony No. 100 in G Major ("Mili-
ReJerence:
tary"), Finale (Bxample 92), 7 6 f . Minuet (Bxamples 776, 118,'119), Le tombeau de Couperin, i, 75
Folksongs: Symphony No. 102 in B-flat Major, Minu- 102 fr.
"Ach du Lieber Augustin" (Examples25-28, et (Example 72a),53 Symphony No. 41 in C Major (K. 551) Rimski-Korsakov, N.:
30, 34-36, 38-41, 44), 24-26, 29-32, 34 Symphony No. 104 in D Major ("Lon- ("Jupiter"), i (Examples 127-31), Example:
"Alouette," 150 don"), iii (Examples 29, 140-41), 26, 120 tr.; ii (Examples 102-5), 89 ff.; Sch6h6razade, iii (Example 153), 160 f,
"Au clair de la lune" (Examples 142-47), 140 ff.; iv (Example 60b), 46 Finale (Example 126),117 ff.
144-511, Exercise,167 ReJerences: ReJerences: Schoenberg, A.:
"Suse, liebe Suse" (Examples 42, 43, 45), Piano Sonata No. 33 in C Major, i, 49 Clarinet Quintet (K. 541), iv, 40 Example:
33 tr. Op. 19, iv (Ex-
Piano Sonata No. 35 in D Major, Finale, Concerto in D Minor for Piano and Or- Sechs kleine Klavierstiicke
"Twelve Days of Christmas, The," 150
54 chestra (K. 466), ii, 3 n. 1 amples 169-72),174 fr.
"Twinkle, Twinkle, Little Star" (Examples String Quartet Op. 20 No. 5, iii, 75 Divertimento in D Major (K. 205), ii ReJerence:
2-24, 33),9, 12-23,28 String Quartet Op. 55 No. 3, i, 43 (trio),43 String Quartet No. 3, iv, 70
Franck, C.: String Quartet Op.71 No. 1, i, 53;iv,49, Divertimento in B-flat Major (K. 270), Schubert, F.:
Example: 54 iv, 54 Examples:
Piano Quintet in F Minor, i (Example 61), String Quartet Op. 74 No. 3, i, 51 Divertimento in B-flat Major (K. 287),v, Piano Trio in B-flat Major, i (Example 87),
46 f. String Quartet Op. 76 No. 5, iv, 70 80 70 t.
272 Thc Rlrythmic Strutture of Music
Schubert, F.-Continued ReJerences:
Examples-Continued Carnaval, "Valse Allemandertt 40
Davidsbtindler, ii, 65
String Quartet in A Minor, iv (Example
Fantasiestucke, "Grillen," 52; ctEnde vom
79), 63 f. Liedr" 54
String Quartet in G Major, i (Example
Kinderszenen, ivr 75
73b), 54
Sffing Quartet "Death and the Maiden,"
Piano Concerto in A Minor, iii, 88 f. j
String Quartet Op. 41 No. 1, ii, 74
ii (Example 70a), 52
Symphonic Etudes, No. 5,43; No. 10, 48
Symphony No. 5 in B-flat, Minuet (Ex- Symphony No. 4 in D Minor, ii, 68
ample 117), 103 f. Exercises:
Wanderer-Fantasie, Presto (Example 53a),
4t f. "Grillen," 143
Fantasiestticke,
"Warum," 167
Fantasiestiicke,
RcJerences, 5: String Quartet Op. 44 No. 1, ii, 116
Impromptu Op. 142 No. 4, 45 Symphony No. 1 in B-flat Major, iii, 116
Moments Musicaux Op. 94, v, 40 Sravinsky, I,:
Piano Quintet Op. 114, iii, 70 Example:
Piano Trio Op. 99, i, 65
The Rite of Spring, "Dance of the Adoles-
Songs, 73
cents" (Example 111), 98 f.
String Quartet in D Minor, i, 64
ReJerence:
String Quartet Op. 29, ii, 40 Suite No. 2 for Small Orchestra, iv, 80
Symphony No. 8 in B Minor, i, 51
Exercise:
Wanderer-Fantasie, Allegro, 47 Suite No. 2 for Small Orchestra, ii, 116
Exercises:
Impromptu Op. 142 No. 4, 116 Tchaikovsky, P. I.:
Symphony No. 7 in C Major, i, 142 Reference:
Schumann, R.: Symphony No. 5 in E Minor, ii, 64
Examples: Exercise:
Carnaval, "Davidsbiindler" March (Ex- Symphony No. 6 in B Minor, i, 143
ample 56a), 43 f .
Fantasiestiicke, "Des Abends" (Example Wagner, R:
58b),44f. Example:
Papillons, No. 6 (Example 86), 69 f. Tristan und Isolde, Act II, Introduction
String Quartet Op. 41 No. 1, i (Example (Example 136), 129 ff., 139, 142
62),47 Reference:
Symphonic Etudes, No. 9 (Example 58a), Tristan und Isolde, Act III, Scene 2, 4
44 Exercise:
Symphony No. 4 in D Minor, Scherzo Tristan und Isolde, Act II, Introduction,
(Example 115), 102 143

PRINTED IN U.S.A,

3558-ta4-14
22-l$

Вам также может понравиться