Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 9

The Association Between Visual,

Nonverbal Cognitive Abilities and Speech,


Phonological Processing, Vocabulary and Reading
Outcomes in Children With Cochlear Implants
Lindsey Edwards and Sara Anderson

Objective: The aim of this study was to explore the possibility that deafness, including global developmental delay, cerebral palsy
specific nonverbal, visual cognitive abilities may be associated with or autistic spectrum disorder, which may be known preimplanta-
outcomes after pediatric cochlear implantation. The study therefore tion (e.g., Meinzen-Derr et al. 2010). However, many additional
examined the relationship between visual sequential memory span and difficulties that are not apparent before the child has received
visual sequential reasoning ability, and a range of speech, phonological
cochlear implants, are more subtle, or emerge gradually as the
processing, vocabulary knowledge, and reading outcomes in children
with cochlear implants.
child matures. For example, specific learning disabilities such
as language disorders or dyslexia cannot be identified in very
Design: A cross-sectional, correlational design was used. Sixty-six chil- young deaf children; their diagnosis is typically not achieved
dren aged 5 to 12 years completed tests of visual memory span and until much later than in hearing children. To add to the complex-
visual sequential reasoning, along with tests of speech intelligibility, pho- ity, some children make poor progress in developing speech,
nological processing, vocabulary knowledge, and word reading ability language and literacy skills after cochlear implantation for no
(the outcome variables). Auditory memory span was also assessed, and
readily identifiable reason. In this case factors such as the nature
its relationship with the other variables examined.
of the child’s language-learning environment and parent/carer
Results: Significant, positive correlations were found between the visual interactions with the child are likely to be contributory factors.
memory and reasoning tests, and each of the outcome variables. A series Thus there is a pressing need to improve our knowledge and
of regression analyses then revealed that for all the outcome variables, understanding of the factors that are predictive of outcome or
after variance attributable to the age at implantation was accounted for, benefit, and translate these into individualized assessment and
visual memory span and visual sequential reasoning ability together
intervention strategies to achieve optimal cognitive and linguis-
accounted for significantly more variance (up to 25%) in each outcome
measure.
tic development.
One area of research that is showing considerable promise
Conclusions: These findings have both clinical and theoretical implica- in meeting this need has focused on describing, understanding,
tions. Clinically, the findings may help improve the identification of chil- and explaining the neurocognitive or information-processing
dren at risk of poor progress after implantation earlier than has been variables that contribute to individual differences in outcomes
possible to date as the nonverbal tests can be administered to children
and benefit (e.g., Fagan et al. 2007; Pisoni, et al. 2010). Further-
as young as 2 years of age. The results may also contribute to the iden-
tification of children with specific learning or language difficulties as well
more, as Pisoni et al. (2008) state “while some proportion of the
as improve our ability to develop intervention strategies for individual variance in performance is associated with peripheral factors
children based on their specific cognitive processing strengths or dif- related to the audibility and the initial sensory encoding of the
ficulties. Theoretically, these results contribute to the growing body speech signal into information-bearing sensory channels in the
of knowledge about learning and development in deaf children with auditory nerve, several additional sources of variance are asso-
cochlear implants. ciated with more central cognitive and linguistic factors that are
related to perception, attention, learning, memory and cognitive
Key words: Children, Cochlear implant, Nonverbal, cognitive, Outcomes.
control.”
(Ear & Hearing 2014;XX;00–00) Some further clues as to the cognitive processes that may be
INTRODUCTION relevant in accounting for the variability in speech, language,
All parents who choose cochlear implantation for their deaf and literacy outcomes can be drawn from a range of related
child are informed that the degree of benefit cannot be precisely lines of research, which will now be briefly considered.
predicted. Although some factors have been reliably associated
with speech and language outcomes and literacy achievements, Phonological Processing, Verbal Memory Capacity, and
for example, age at implantation, length of auditory deprivation, Reading in Hearing Children
and early linguistic experience (auditory–oral versus total com- First, in hearing children, strong associations are found
munication), there remains a large degree of variability in out- between verbal (working) memory capacity and phonologi-
come (e.g., Marschark et al. 2007; Archbold et al. 2008; Geers cal processing skills, and specific reading difficulties. Verbal
et al. 2008; Lyxell et al. 2009). Some of this variability can be memory capacity refers to the amount of information that can
accounted for by factors such as disabilities in addition to the be stored and “manipulated” in some way, over a short period
of time (i.e., seconds). Verbal memory capacity is typically
Great Ormond Street Hospital for Children, London, England, United assessed using a digit span task giving a measure of auditory
Kingdom. memory span, and deficits on this and other tests of verbal

0196/0202/13/XXXX-0000/0 • Ear & Hearing • Copyright © 2014 by Lippincott Williams & Wilkins • Printed in the U.S.A.

1
Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
<zdoi; 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000012>
2 EDWARDS AND ANDERSON / EAR & HEARING, VOL. XX, NO. X, XXX–XXX

short-term memory have consistently been found in children language and reading skills have been explored, again both
with specific reading disorders (e.g., Gottardo et  al. 1996; de in deaf children with cochlear implants and those using con-
Jong 1998; Helland & Asbjørnsen 2004; Kibby et al. 2004). ventional hearing aids. Johnson and Goswami (2010) used
One task commonly used to assess phonological processing a forward digit span task to assess auditory memory, and the
skills is the nonword repetition task, which tests the ability to Leiter-R (Roid & Miller 1997) memory screen to assess visual
assemble, rehearse, and produce novel phonological sequences memory in implanted children. They found that visual memory
from short-term memory. A number of studies have found a was not correlated with the phonological awareness tasks, but
strong association between deficits in nonword repetition and was with reading comprehension. Auditory memory was linked
both specific language impairment and reading difficulties (e.g., to phonological awareness, receptive and expressive vocabulary
Bishop et al. 1996; Catts et al. 2005; Conti-Ramsden & Durkin knowledge, reading accuracy and comprehension, along with
2007; Rispens & Parigger 2010). speech intelligibility. In a longitudinal analysis with a large
sample of cochlear implant users, Pisoni et al. (2011) examined
Phonological Processing and Reading in Deaf Children the predictive relationships between auditory memory capac-
Second, in children with a hearing loss, with or without ity (digit span) at age 8 to 9 years, and speech intelligibility,
cochlear implants, phonological processing skills are typically vocabulary knowledge, language ability, and reading ability
weak compared with those of hearing peers. These skills have at around 16 years of age. They found strong positive corre-
been associated with a range of speech, language, and literacy lations between longest digit span at age 8 to 9 years and all
outcomes. For example, Dillon and Pisoni (2006) tested non- the speech and language outcome measures in adolescence.
word repetition in 76 children with cochlear implants aged However, interestingly from a theoretical perspective, backward
7 to 9 years. They found strong associations between nonword digit span, which is considered a measure of auditory work-
repetition and three measures of reading outcome (nonword ing memory (verbal memory capacity when concurrent mental
reading, single word reading, and reading comprehension). processing is required) rather than simply auditory immediate
These correlations remained significant when age at onset of memory capacity (rote sequential serial item memory), corre-
deafness, communication mode, and performance intelligence lated significantly with tests of higher-order language function-
quotient (IQ) were controlled for. Carter et al. (2002) also used ing, but not speech perception or recognition measures. Pisoni
a nonword repetition task to examine prosodic representations et al. suggested that this dissociation between the forward and
of words. They found that 8- to 10-year olds, who had been backward digit span results may indicate that rapid phonologi-
using their cochlear implant for a mean of 5 years, only correctly cal encoding and immediate verbal-sequential phonological
imitated 5% of the nonwords. However, 64% of the imitations memory are essential building blocks for the development of
contained the correct number of syllables. They also found that good speech perception and speech-language skills. In addi-
the syllable score was significantly positively correlated with tion, good verbal-sequential memory abilities are needed for
language comprehension and speech intelligibility. Recently, all higher language processing tasks involving encoding, stor-
Casserly and Pisoni (2013) demonstrated that the performance ing, and maintaining representations of speech sounds, spoken
of 8- to 10-year old cochlear implant users on a nonword repeti- words, and sentence meanings in working memory.
tion task was predictive of measures of speech and language at
the age of 16 to 18 years, including speech intelligibility, recep- Visual–Spatial Memory and Reading in
tive vocabulary, and reading ability. Hearing Children
In another early study Dyer et al. (2003) assessed phonologi- While it makes intuitive sense that normal auditory–verbal
cal awareness and decoding skills along with rapid automatized memory skills are important for the effective development of
naming of visual material, in 49 deaf students with a mean age spoken language and later literacy skills, it is less clear whether
of 13 years but whose mean reading age was 7 years. Phono- visual memory abilities would also be expected to play a signifi-
logical awareness and decoding skills were poorer in these deaf cant role, either in hearing children or those with a hearing loss.
children compared with hearing children matched for reading Therefore it is perhaps unsurprising that there is considerably
(not chronological) age and correlated significantly with read- less research exploring this question. Thus the next piece in this
ing delay on the rhyme-awareness task. In addition two recent puzzle is the observation that it is not only the verbal memory
studies have also examined phonological awareness or process- span that is reduced in hearing children with specific reading
ing skills and reading in children and adolescents with cochlear disability; there is evidence that their visual–spatial memory
implants. Geers and Hayes (2010) reported that phonological capacity or visual memory span and working memory are also
processing skills accounted for an additional 19% of the vari- poorer. The findings are less consistent than for verbal memory
ance in literacy achievement after factors such as sex, duration deficits, but the contradictory findings could be accounted for
of deafness, performance IQ, and family characteristics were by differences in participant inclusion criteria and the nature
taken into account. Similarly, Johnson and Goswami (2010) of the memory tasks used (e.g., Olson & Datta 2002; Jeffries
found that reading attainment was significantly correlated with & Everatt 2004; Kibby et al. 2004; Smith-Spark &Fisk 2007).
phonological awareness in 5- to 15-year olds with cochlear Menghini et al. (2011) attempted to resolve this issue by testing
implants, when age and nonverbal IQ were controlled for. children with developmental dyslexia, but no other concomi-
tant disorders and normal intelligence, and used memory tasks
Memory and Reading in Deaf Children minimally demanding on attentional and executive cognitive
In a third line of research (but sometimes within the same resources. They concluded that children with specific reading
studies as described earlier in the article), the relationships deficits do have deficits in visual–spatial memory span, in addi-
between memory capacity, working memory and speech, tion to verbal memory span.

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
EDWARDS AND ANDERSON / EAR & HEARING, VOL. XX, NO. X, XXX–XXX 3

Visual Memory and Reading in Deaf Children a group of very young (aged 2 to 5 years) deaf children per-
There is some evidence that short-term memory span for formed significantly worse on the Sequential Order subtest of
visually presented stimuli is poorer in deaf children, including the Leiter-R compared with their hearing peers.
those with cochlear implants, than in their hearing peers. Khan To summarize, previous research has suggested that reading
et  al. (2005) found a significant difference between deaf and competence (or conversely reading deficit) is dependent on pho-
hearing children in visual memory span for pictures of common nological processing skills, which in turn is strongly associated
objects presented in an array, where the child had to point to the with verbal memory capacity, and possibly also visual memory
pictures in the same order as that given by the examiner (Forward capacity. Deaf children are at risk of experiencing significant
Memory subtest from the Leiter-R). However, there was no dif- difficulties in developing phonological awareness and process-
ference between the hearing children and children who had been ing skills, and have deficits in verbal and visual memory, as
using their cochlear implant for 1 year. A novel visual memory well as possibly visual reasoning problems. However, to date,
span task was developed by Cleary and colleagues based on the no empirical studies have examined the relationships between
Simon memory game, where the task is for the child to repro- the combination of visual memory span, visual, nonverbal
duce sequences of presentations of colored lights. The authors reasoning skills, speech intelligibility, phonological process-
conducted a series of experiments with children with cochlear ing, language and reading outcomes in children with a hearing
implants and found that these children had shorter memory loss or deaf children with cochlear implants. Given the studies
spans and poorer sequence learning when the same patterns were described earlier in this article, it seems plausible that such a
repeated, again in comparison with hearing peers (Cleary et al. relationship may exist.
2001, 2000; Pisoni & Cleary 2004). These authors suggest that
individual differences in outcome in deaf children who receive The Present Study—Aims and Rationale
cochlear implants “may reflect fundamental learning processes As outlined at the beginning of the Introduction, one of the
that affect the encoding and retention of temporal information in major challenges for clinicians working with children with
both short- and long-term memory” (Pisoni et al. 2008, p. 77). cochlear implants is identifying those at risk of poor progress,
Studies that report findings on visual memory span and read- or those who have specific learning difficulties in addition to
ing ability in deaf children are equally scarce. However, two those arising from their deafness, as early as possible. However,
studies provide some relevant results. Harris and Moreno (2004) most tests conventionally used to assess specific learning disor-
used pictures of objects that were visually similar, had similar ders require a minimum level of spoken language production or
sounding names, and were formed with similar hand shapes in comprehension for their administration. This typically excludes
British Sign Language or dissimilar in these three respects. Pic- those very children for whom the need to establish what is pre-
tures were presented in increasingly long sequences, and the venting progress is greatest. It would be of immense value if
child had to name the objects in the order in which they had there were reliable and valid visual, nonverbal tests of neuro-
appeared. In deaf children aged 14 years (but not in the group cognitive processes that were predictive of speech, phonologi-
aged 8 years), visual memory span was a significant predic- cal processing, reading and language outcomes in children with
tor of reading performance when age and nonverbal IQ were
cochlear implants, particularly if they could be administered to
accounted for. It is interesting to note that in these older chil-
very young deaf children.
dren recall span was not affected by the similarity of the sounds
Thus the primary aim of this study is to establish whether
of the objects’ names, suggesting the children were not using
there are links between visual (sequential) memory span, visual
phonological coding when remembering items. Clearly, in this
sequential reasoning ability, and speech intelligibility, phono-
task although the stimuli are presented visually they are ver-
logical processing, reading and vocabulary knowledge in chil-
bally encoded (as the child had to name each object), and the
dren with cochlear implants. In this study we used phonological
response mode is also verbal.
processing as an outcome (dependent) variable, along with
Using essentially the same type of task, but with pictures of
reading and vocabulary knowledge (as an indicator of language
words varying in number of syllables, Kyle and Harris (2006)
found no correlation between visual memory span and a number
of measures including expressive vocabulary, reading, or spell- TABLE 1.  Participant characteristics
ing. However, these children were younger (aged 7 to 8 years),
suggesting that there may be a developmental progression Male: Female 32: 34
Mean age at testing (SD; range) 8.5 yrs (1.9; 5.4–11.7)
involved in the relative contributions of auditory compared with Mean age at (1st) implant (SD; range) 3.7 yrs (1.5; 1.2–8.9)
visual information processing in the acquisition of reading skills. Duration of (1st) implant use (SD; range) 5.13 yrs (2.0; 1.0–10.1)
Etiology of deafness
Visual Sequential Reasoning Abilities in Deaf Children  Unknown 39%
The next question is whether the difficulty with auditory  Genetic (nonsyndromal) 21%
 Syndromal 15%
memory span, which relies on the processing of sequentially
 Meningitis 17%
presented auditory information, is reflective of a wider diffi-  Rubella/cytomegalovirus infection 6%
culty in processing or understanding sequential information,  Meningitis 2%
that is, do hearing children with reading difficulties and deaf Communication mode
children also have problems with reasoning or problem solving  Speech 80%
with nonverbal or visual information that is sequential? To our  Speech and sign 14%
knowledge no studies have addressed this directly in hearing  Sign 6%
or deaf children. However, Khan et  al. (2005) did report that

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
4 EDWARDS AND ANDERSON / EAR & HEARING, VOL. XX, NO. X, XXX–XXX

development) rather than as an explanatory (independent) vari- sequential reasoning ability. Six outcome variables were a range
able. We have focused on sequential visual cognitive processes of speech intelligibility, phonological processing, reading, and
or abilities because language is essentially sequentially based vocabulary knowledge measures.
(phonemes need to be produced in the correct sequence in
words, words in the right order in sentences to be grammati- Participants
cally correct). Therefore the ability to process sequential visual All the children aged between 5 and 12 years during a
information may be hypothesized to be associated with the abil- 6-month period, currently patients on our cochlear implant pro-
ity to process auditory sequential information, and thus with the gram, were invited to participate in the study. This was with
development of phonological, language, and literacy abilities. the exception of children with known significant developmen-
There are a number of possible models of the relationships tal delay or neuro-developmental disorders such as autistic
between all these factors that could be tested, for example, hier- spectrum disorder. The characteristics of the resulting sample
archical models where variables are included incrementally are summarized in Table 1. The participants were 66 children,
in several steps. However, we sought to test the most simple with approximately equal numbers of girls and boys. The cause
models, and include as few variables as possible as to account of deafness was unknown for around 40% of the children,
for the variability in outcomes after cochlear implantation, spe- genetic (nonsyndromal) for approximately 20% and syndromal
cifically focusing on nonverbal, visual measures. This was in an (Waardenburgs, Pendreds, CHARGE Association, Ushers syn-
attempt to maximize the clinical interpretability and value of drome and Brachio-oto-renal syndrome) in 15%, and congeni-
the results. In particular, we wanted to use those tests that can tal infections or meningitis in 25%. There were no significant
potentially be administered to children before the emergence of differences in scores on any of the visual memory, visual rea-
assessable spoken language skills. However, auditory memory soning, auditory memory, speech intelligibility, phonological
capacity (digit span) was also assessed and used as an explana- processing, and vocabulary or reading tests when the children
tory variable because this test is probably the simplest auditory/ were divided into three groups according to etiology (unknown,
verbal one for deaf children to tackle, as it only requires the genetic, or syndromic/infection/prematurity).
ability to articulate the numbers one to nine in a sufficiently All but 1 child was congenitally deaf or deafened before
clear manner for the examiner to be able to recognize them. their second birthday. The preimplant pure-tone average aided
Furthermore, digit span has received the most attention in previ- loss for 2 and 4 kHz in the worse ear was greater than 90 dBHL.
ous research and it is highly established as a predictor of speech
Seven children had bilateral implants: 2 simultaneously and the
intelligibility, phonological processing ability, reading and lan-
remainder sequentially. The majority of the children used spo-
guage in implanted children. This study will therefore allow us
ken English as their main mode of communication, with some
to explore (nonstatistically) the relative strength of associations
also using signs to support their communication (receptively,
between auditory and visual memory capacities and outcomes
expressively, or in both ways). Four children had a preference
after pediatric cochlear implantation, and consider our findings
for using sign expressively, but had sufficient spoken language
in the context of previous research.
skills to enable them to understand the instructions for the tests
Hypothesis  •  Visual sequential reasoning and visual memory administered for this study. English was not the first language
span are significantly positively associated with speech intel- for the parents of 7% of the children.
ligibility, phonological processing, vocabulary knowledge, and
reading outcomes in deaf children after cochlear implantation.
Measures
Memory  •  Forward Memory Subtest: Leiter International Per-
PARTICIPANTS AND METHODS
formance Scale—Revised (Leiter-R; Roid & Miller 1997). This
Design test assesses visual memory span by testing the child’s ability
The study used a cross-sectional, correlational design. The to reproduce the sequence in which the examiner has pointed to
main explanatory variables were visual memory span and visual pictures of objects in a grid, presented at a rate of one picture

TABLE 2.  Descriptive statistics for the visual sequential memory and reasoning, auditory memory, speech, phonological processing,
vocabulary and reading variables

Normalization Sample Cochlear Implant Sample % of Cochlear Implant


Children Lower Than 1 SD
Variable (Test) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Range Below Norm
Auditory Memory Span (CMS Numbers) 10 (3) 7.17 (3.4) 1–15 56%
Fluid Reasoning composite (Leiter-R) 100 (15) 104.3 (15.3) 67–137 9%
Visual Memory Span (Leiter-R) 10 (3) 11.9 (3.4) 2–18 7%
Children’s Speech Intelligibility Measure N/A 69.3 (29.8) 0–100 N/A
Phonological Processing (NEPSY II) 10 (3) 7.61 (4.3) 1–15 34%
Repetition of NonWords (NEPSY II) 10 (3) 10.0 (4.5) 1–18 22%
Receptive Vocabulary (TOWK) 10 (3) 6.8 (3.4) 3–15 54%
Expressive Vocabulary (TOWK) 10 (3) 7.4 (3.7) 3–19 49%
Word Reading (WIAT-II) 100 (15) 86.5 (22.9) 40–128 43%

CMS, Children’s Memory Scale; TOWK, Test of Word Knowledge; WIAT-II, Wechsler Individual Achievement Test–II.

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
EDWARDS AND ANDERSON / EAR & HEARING, VOL. XX, NO. X, XXX–XXX 5

per second. Again the sequences start at two stimuli in length, repeats back each word, and scores one point for each correct
and become increasingly long. Testing is stopped after the child ­syllable. Testing is stopped after four consecutive item scores
has made six errors. The number of correct sequences is con- of zero. The total score is converted to a scaled score where
verted to a scaled score (mean 10, SD 3) based on hearing chil- the mean is 10 and standard deviation is 3, based on hearing
dren norms. The test is intended for children between 2 and 16 children norms.
years of age. Phonological Processing Subtest: NEPSY II. This subtest
Numbers Forward Subtest: Children’s Memory Scale comprises two tasks that assess phonemic awareness. The first,
(Cohen 1997). This is a standard digit span test of verbal mem- the word segment recognition requires identification of words
ory capacity, where the child repeats back increasingly long from word segments, for example, three pictures are named by
series of numbers presented one digit per second, starting with the examiner—“pencil,” “window,” and “ice cream,” then the
two digits. There are two trials for each sequence length, and child is given the cue “indow” and asked to point to the correct
testing is stopped once the child has failed both trials for a par- picture. The second, the phonological segmentation is a test of
ticular length. The number of correctly reproduced sequences is elision where the child is asked to repeat a word and then create
converted to an age-based scaled score. The mean and standard a new word either by omitting a syllable or a phoneme, or by
deviation of the hearing standardization sample are 10 and 3, substituting one phoneme in a word for another (e.g., “say ‘mis-
respectively. The test can be used for children aged between 5 taken,’ now say it again but don’t say ‘mis’”). The child scores
and 16 years. one for each correct response and testing is stopped after six
Visual Sequential Reasoning  •  Fluid Reasoning Composite: consecutive incorrect responses. The test is for children aged 5
Leiter-R. Individual subtests from the Visualization and Reason- to 16 years and yields scaled scores where the mean is 10 and
ing battery of the Leiter-R can be combined into two separate standard deviation is 3.
composites labeled Fundamental Visualization and Fluid Rea- Vocabulary Knowledge •  Expressive Vocabulary: Test of
soning. In this study the latter composite was used, which com- Word Knowledge (Wiig & Secord 1992). Children are shown
prises two subtests—Sequential Order and Repeated Patterns. a series of pictures and asked to either name them (if a noun
Sequential Order includes items such as a cat increasing in size such as a kangaroo) or tell the examiner what is happening (if
where the child has to identify the correct next-sized cat from a verb such as melting). Many of the items have more than one
two possibilities. In the Repeated Patterns subtest the child has possible correct response. Each correct answer is given one
to complete patterns such as red-yellow-blue-red-yellow-?-?. point, testing is stopped after five consecutive errors and the
Testing is stopped after seven errors for Sequential Reasoning total number of correct responses is converted to a scaled score
and six errors for Repeated Patterns. Here, each subtest score (mean 10, SD 3, based on norms for hearing children). The test
is converted to a scaled score, which in turn are combined and is for children aged between 5 and 17 years.
converted to a scaled score, where the norms based on hear- Receptive Vocabulary: Test of Word Knowledge. Here the
ing children have a mean of 100 and standard deviation of 15. task is for the child to point to one picture from four possible
Children between 2 and 16 years of age can be administered this options, which best represents the word spoken by the examiner.
test. In the statistical analyses the Fluid Reasoning composite is The words include nouns, verbs, and adjectives. Other details
referred to as the “visual sequential reasoning” variable. are the same as for the expressive vocabulary test.
Speech  •  Children’s Speech Intelligibility Measure (Wilcox & Reading  •  Word Reading Subtest: Wechsler Individual Achieve-
Morris 1999). In this test 50 words are modeled verbally by the ment Test-II UK (WIAT-II UK). This test for children aged
examiner, and the child repeats each word, which is digitally between 4 to 16 years, starts with assessing the child’s knowledge
recorded. After administration of the test a judge listens to the of letter names and sounds and progresses through to reading
recording and identifies the word he or she believes the child individual words of increasing difficulty. Testing is stopped after
had said from a list of 12 phonetically similar possibilities. For seven consecutive errors. The number of correct responses is con-
example, for the target word “learn” the 12 possible choices verted to a standard score (again, mean 100, SD 15).
are burn, churn, firm, first, germ, hurt, jerk, learn, purse, stern,
term, and turn. An intelligibility rating is calculated by multi- Procedure
plying by 2 the total number of words the judge correctly identi- Ethical approval for the study was received from the National
fies, giving a possible range of 0 to 100. This test was developed Research Ethics Service and the project complied with institu-
for children aged 3 years to 10 years 11 months, so some of tion research regulatory board procedures.
our sample participants were older than the upper limit. How- Parents and children were provided with written informa-
ever, given that the test scores are not converted to age-based tion describing the study and any questions they had regard-
standard scores, and that many of our participants might be ing it were addressed before obtaining written consent from the
expected to have poorer speech intelligibility than their hearing parents. The children also gave verbal assent to participation,
age-matched peers, it was considered acceptable to extend the having been assured that their participation was voluntary and
age range by 1 year. The Children’s Speech Intelligibility Mea- they could stop at any time should they wish. However, no child
sure has good test–retest reliability, and inter-rater reliability on asked to stop before completion of all the tests they were able to
22 randomly selected participants was found to be 0.93. undertake. Children implanted bilaterally were tested wearing
Phonological Processing  •  Repetition of Nonwords Subtest: both implants, and those who usually wore a conventional hear-
NEPSY second edition (NEPSY II; Korkman et  al. 2007). ing aid contralaterally to their implant were tested while using it
This subtest is for children aged 5 to 12 years. The child lis- together with their cochlear implant.
tens to a series of increasingly complex nonwords (e.g., “crum- The children were tested individually in a clinic room at
see” through “indobtreelob” to “splinkdellblee”). The child the hospital, at their school in a quiet room, or in their home,

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
6 EDWARDS AND ANDERSON / EAR & HEARING, VOL. XX, NO. X, XXX–XXX

according to parental preference. Testing lasted a maximum of implantation was entered as the independent variable. Age at
90 min and the children were offered a break part of the way implantation was chosen because previous research has found
through the session. it to be the most robustly significant predictor of speech and
Tests were administered in the same order for each child, language outcomes after pediatric implantation. It accounted
according to the standard procedures provided in the test for between 7 and 15% of the variance across the outcome vari-
manuals. ables. For each outcome variable, the younger the child was
when they received their implant, the better their performance.
RESULTS In the first analysis, in the second model age at implantation
was entered as the first step and then auditory memory span was
Descriptive statistics for each of the variables (visual mem- added in the second step. This analysis showed that auditory
ory, visual reasoning, auditory memory, speech intelligibility, memory span is highly significantly associated with outcomes
phonological processing, vocabulary, and reading) are pre- after accounting for age at implantation. In the case of nonword
sented in Table 2. These results indicate that our sample of chil- repetition, this model accounted for 73% of the variability in
dren with cochlear implants are, on average, performing within scores.
the normal range compared with hearing children on most, but In the second analysis, in the first model age at implan-
not all, of the tests. The notable exceptions, where the mean tation was again entered as the first step. In the second step,
score for the sample was around 1 SD or more below the norm visual sequential reasoning and visual memory span were
for hearing children, were auditory memory span, phonological added simultaneously. Table  5 indicates that for every out-
processing (NEPSY II), receptive and expressive vocabulary, come variable, visual sequential reasoning and visual memory
and word reading (WIAT-II). The percentage of children scor- span together substantially increased the amount of variance
ing 1 SD or more below the standardized mean ranged from 7 accounted for, after that attributed to the child’s age at implan-
to 56% across the variables. tation. Visual sequential reasoning and visual memory span
Table  3 gives the first order, Pearson correlations between together accounted for between 16 and 25% of additional vari-
each of the variables. All the correlations were positive and sta- ance. In each case the increase was statistically significant at the
tistically significant, the majority at the p < 0.001 level. p = 0.01 level or less. Inspection of the β coefficients reveals
To further test the associations between the visual memory that for the phonological processing subtest of the NEPSY
span and visual sequential reasoning predictor variables and II, visual sequential reasoning was the statistically significant
the speech intelligibility, phonological processing, vocabulary explanatory variable. For each of the other outcome variables,
and reading composite outcome variables, a series of multiple visual memory span made the significant contribution to the
regression analyses was performed. Three variables required variance accounted for by the model.
logarithmic transformation to make the data normally distrib-
uted; these were receptive vocabulary, expressive vocabulary,
and speech intelligibility. Two sets of analyses were then con- DISCUSSION
ducted, the results of which are presented in Tables 4 and 5. In Understanding and explaining the considerable variabil-
both sets of analyses, in the first model only the child’s age at ity between individuals in outcomes after pediatric cochlear

TABLE 3.  Correlations between the visual memory, visual sequential reasoning, auditory memory, phonological processing, vocabulary
and reading variables

Visual Visual Auditory Children’s


Sequential Memory Memory Speech Phonological Repetition of Receptive Expressive
Reasoning* Span Span Intelligibility Processing Nonwords Vocabulary Vocabulary
(Leiter-R) (Leiter-R) (CMS) Measure (NEPSY II) (NEPSY II) (TOWK) (TOWK)
Visual Memory Span 0.532
(Leiter-R)
Auditory Memory Span 0.320 0.397
(CMS)
Children’s Speech 0.356 0.390 0.653
Intelligibility Measure
Phonological Processing 0.474 0.370 0.624 0.730
(NEPSY II)
Repetition of Nonwords 0.374 0.426 0.836 0.736 0.754
(NEPSY II)
Receptive Vocabulary 0.388 0.442 0.544 0.500 0.666 0.566
(TOWK)
Expressive Vocabulary 0.309 0.381 0.636 0.593 0.592 0.659 0.693
(TOWK)
Word Reading 0.443 0.417 0.638 0.703 0.812 0.777 0.732 0.723
(WIAT-II)

Figures in italics are significant at p = 0.05 (two-tailed).


Figures in bold are significant at p = 0.01 or less (two-tailed).
*Fluid Reasoning composite of the Leiter-R comprising the Sequential Order and Repeated Patterns subtests.
CMS, Children’s Memory Scale; TOWK, Test of Word Knowledge; WIAT–II, Wechsler Individual Achievement Test–II.

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
EDWARDS AND ANDERSON / EAR & HEARING, VOL. XX, NO. X, XXX–XXX 7

TABLE 4.  Regression analyses of age at implant and auditory memory span on measures of speech, phonological
processing, vocabulary knowledge, and reading outcomes

Children’s
Phonological Nonword Receptive Expressive Word Speech
Processing Repetition Vocabulary Vocabulary Reading Intelligibility
(NEPSY II) (NEPSY II) (TOWK) (TOWK) (WIAT-II) Measure
n = 64 n = 65 n = 65 n = 66 n = 65 n = 65

β coeff. p β coeff. p β coeff. p β coeff. p β coeff. p β coeff. p


Model 1
 Age at implant −1.093 0.002 −0.941 0.010 −0.041 0.026 −0.046 0.011 −5.260 0.005 −0.072 0.109
 R2 0.148 0.101 0.076 0.097 0.120 0.040
Model 2
 Age at implant −0.788 0.005 −0.521 0.011 −0.028 0.083 −0.029 0.037 −3.664 0.013 −0.037 0.338
 Auditory Memory Span 0.717 0.000 1.048 0.000 0.034 0.000 0.041 0.000 3.978 0.000 0.084 0.000
 R2 0.463† 0.729† 0.330† 0.482† 0.464† 0.315†

Significance of the change in R2 between model 1 and model 2:


*p < 0.01;
†p < 0.001.
TOWK, Test of Word Knowledge; WIAT–II, Wechsler Individual Achievement Test–II.

implantation is increasingly the focus of research in this field. skills emerge, are also associated with a range of speech, lan-
Deaf children who receive cochlear implants, even if early in guage, and literacy outcomes after cochlear implantation.
life, remain at risk of delays in developing language and literacy However, before testing the primary hypothesis, it is useful
skills, although many do achieve these at a level comparable to consider the context for our findings in terms of the actual
with that of their hearing peers. Previous research has sought outcomes achieved in our sample, and whether the associations
to identify cognitive or information-processing factors that are between the speech, phonological processing, language and lit-
associated with outcomes, but has typically focused on verbal eracy outcomes are consistent with previous research on this
measures such as nonword repetition, auditory–verbal memory, issue. In our group of children aged between 5 and 12 years,
and verbal rehearsal speed (e.g., Dillon & Pisoni 2006; Pisoni who had been using a cochlear implant for between 1 and 10
et  al. 2010; Casserly & Pisoni 2013), rather than nonverbal years, around half of the children achieved below the normal
visual cognitive predictors. Unfortunately, many of the tests range for hearing children (i.e., 1 SD), in their auditory memory
that assess these abilities cannot be undertaken by young deaf span, and receptive and expressive vocabulary knowledge. One
children, especially those children with specific language or third or more of the children scored below the normal range
learning difficulties, as the tests rely on a basic level of spoken on tests of word reading and phonological processing. Together
language understanding and production for their administration. these results suggest that a substantial proportion of the chil-
Therefore, the primary aim of this study was to explore dren have auditory–verbal memory capacity deficits and are
whether certain visual cognitive processing abilities, which can experiencing difficulties developing the phonological process-
be assessed in very young deaf children before spoken language ing skills that underpin the development of a wide vocabulary

TABLE 5.  Regression analyses of age at implant, visual memory span and visual sequential reasoning on measures of
speech, phonological processing, vocabulary knowledge, and reading outcomes.

Children’s
Phonological Nonword Receptive Expressive Word Speech
Processing Repetition Vocabulary Vocabulary Reading Intelligibility
(NEPSY II) (NEPSY II) (TOWK) (TOWK) (WIAT-II) Measure
n = 64 n = 64 n = 64 n = 65 n = 64 n = 64

β coeff. p β coeff. p β coeff. p β coeff. p β coeff. p β coeff. p


Model 1
 Age at Implant −1.093 0.002 −0.950 0.010 −0.043 0.021 −0.048 0.008 −5.460 0.003 −0.074 0.103
 R2 0.148 0.102 0.083 0.107 0.130 0.042
Model 2
 Age at Implant −0.920 0.004 −0.848 0.013 −0.038 0.021 −0.044 0.010 −4.749 0.005 −0.075 0.081
 Visual Sequential 0.088 0.017 0.044 0.260 0.002 0.320 0.002 0.407 0.337 0.085 −0.003 0.613
 Reasoning†
 Visual Memory Span† 0.254 0.109 0.454 0.010 0.028 0.001 0.022 0.014 1.988 0.021 0.072 0.002
 R2 0.345† 0.293* 0.327† 0.266* 0.332† 0.215*

†These variables were entered into the regression model simultaneously.


Significance of the change in R2 between model 1 and model 2:
*p < 0.01;
†p < 0.001.

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
8 EDWARDS AND ANDERSON / EAR & HEARING, VOL. XX, NO. X, XXX–XXX

and good reading skills. Few of the children demonstrated sub- assessing his or her visual memory span and visual sequential
stantial difficulty with the visual sequential reasoning or visual reasoning abilities may provide useful information in deciding
memory span tasks. It is not surprising that these children expe- whether he or she is at risk of making poor progress in develop-
rienced much less difficulty with these tasks compared with the ing phonological processing skills, language and later on literacy
language or literacy tasks, but more surprising that the percent- skills. Similarly, when presented with a child who is making
age of children scoring more than 1 SD below the mean was less unexpectedly poor progress after implantation, and who may
than that based on normative data. The most likely explanation as yet be unable to tackle the more traditional, ­language-based
for this is sampling bias, as the group was self-selected. assessments of specific learning difficulties, assessing these
Consistent with previous research, we found auditory mem- visual information-processing skills may be informative. If the
ory span (as a measure of auditory memory capacity) to be very child has some ability to produce spoken words, specifically
strongly related to all outcomes, from phonological processing the numbers one to nine, the results of this study suggest that
skills through to reading of single words. In particular, auditory administering a standardized test of auditory digit span such as
memory span was a highly significant predictor of nonword rep- that from the Children’s Memory Scale or Wechsler Intelligence
etition ability, accounting for 62% of the variance after taking Scale for Children IV, will provide further useful information.
account of the age at which the child was implanted. Poor per- Clearly there are a number of other potential factors that con-
formance on nonword repetition tasks is thought to reflect weak tribute to the variance in outcomes, which would also need to be
or underspecified phonological representations of words in the explored in a clinical assessment, such as the presence of a fam-
mental lexicon (Dillon & Pisoni 2006), which could occur as ily history of dyslexia or specific language disorder, the degree of
the result of the degraded auditory representation of speech pro- parental involvement in the child’s rehabilitation, any brain abnor-
vided by a cochlear implant. Nonword repetition deficits have malities evidenced on magnetic resonance imaging scans (for
been associated with poorer vocabulary knowledge and reading example after meningitis), or the child’s exposure to more than
skills, and our results are consistent with this. one spoken language in the home. All of these are likely to have
The main purpose of this study was to explore the possibil- an impact on progress in developing speech, language and literacy
ity that nonverbal visual cognitive processing factors may be skills, but could not be quantified or accounted for in this study.
useful in accounting for the variability in phonological pro- Theoretically, as Pisoni et al. (2008, p. 61) note, the absence
cesses, language and literacy abilities in children with cochlear of auditory stimulation may disturb the development of neural
implants. The measures of visual memory and reasoning ability systems and circuits other than those primarily involved in audi-
were chosen based on a combination of theoretical and clinical tory processing (i.e., motor and visual systems), before implan-
rationale. In particular, the tests are clinically useful because tation takes place. Delays in language acquisition during early
they can be administered to very young deaf children who have development is likely to produce effects on processes not neces-
not yet developed the spoken language skills needed to under- sarily related to the early sensory processes of audition, for exam-
take the tests previously identified as associated with outcome ple, executive functions such as problem solving, attention and
(e.g., digit span, nonword repetition, or phonological process- memory, particularly, memory for sequences and temporal order
ing). The results of this study suggest that these visual, nonver- information. Therefore, in terms of our findings it is plausible that
bal tests may indeed provide useful information for identifying nonverbal, visual cognitive processes such as visual sequential
children at risk of poor progress after cochlear implantation. memory, and visual sequential reasoning are affected by the early
The combination of visual memory span and visual sequential distortion or absence of auditory stimulation of the brain.
reasoning measures increased the amount of variation explained As with much research in this area, one limitation of this
in the speech and language outcomes by around 20%, over study is the sample size, which while respectable in comparison
that accounted for by age at implantation. For the majority of with other published studies on pediatric cochlear implantation
the outcome measures, visual memory span made the signifi- of this nature, is modest in view of the statistical analyses used.
cant contribution to the variance accounted for by the regres- A second possible limitation is the range of measures used as
sion models. It is interesting to note that the exception was the either explanatory and outcome variables. In particular there are
Phonological Processing subtest from the NESPY II where a relatively limited number of standardized visual tests available
the visual sequential reasoning variable (the Fluid Reasoning assessing reasoning or memory abilities, and even fewer that can
composite from the Leiter-R) accounted for the significant pro- be administered to young children. In contrast there are many
portion of variance. The NEPSY II Phonological Processing potential tests of language and literacy and it was necessary to
subtest may be considered to place greater processing demands choose a selection that would hopefully cover a range of speech,
on sequential processing skills, as the more difficult items in phonological processing, language and literacy outcomes.
this test require the child to manipulate phonemes within words In this study we examined the association between nonver-
and produce them in the correct order. bal cognitive abilities and outcomes in children aged between
The relationship between visual memory span and outcomes 5 and 12 years, some of whom had been using their cochlear
is particularly interesting given that overall, as a group, these implant for more than 8 years. Therefore for these children,
children with cochlear implants were performing well within the valuable time has elapsed when specific information-processing
normal average range on the test of visual memory span, (only deficits may have been identified and remedial intervention pro-
7% of the sample scored 1 SD or more below the mean for the vided, for them to maximize the benefit to be derived from the
normalization sample). This suggests that clinically, if a child implant(s). The advantage of the tests of visual sequential rea-
achieves a poor score on this test, further investigation of their soning and memory used in this study is that they can be admin-
information-processing abilities should be undertaken to assess istered to deaf children as young as 2 years of age. For some
specific areas of difficulty. In a clinical setting therefore, before children, this could be before they have actually received their
the child has well-established spoken language capabilities, cochlear implant(s). Therefore, for the first time, we have been

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
EDWARDS AND ANDERSON / EAR & HEARING, VOL. XX, NO. X, XXX–XXX 9

able to identify potential early information-processing indica- Gottardo, A., Stanovich, K. E., Siegel, L. S. (1996). The relationships
tors of children at risk of difficulties in developing speech, lan- between phonological sensitivity, syntactic processing, and verbal work-
ing memory in the reading performance of third-grade children. J Exp
guage and literacy skills after cochlear implantation. However, Child Psychol, 63, 563–582.
it is recognized that observing an association between nonver- Harris, M. & Moreno, C. (2004). Deaf children’s use of phonological cod-
bal cognitive abilities and outcomes at one time point postim- ing: Evidence from reading, spelling, and working memory. J Deaf Stud
plantation does not necessarily mean that these same cognitive Deaf Educ, 9, 253–268.
Helland, T., & Asbjørnsen, A. (2004). Digit span in dyslexia: Variations
abilities, if measured in younger deaf children, are predictive of according to language comprehension and mathematics skills. J Clin Exp
future difficulties: this needs to be the focus of further research. Neuropsychol, 26, 31–42.
Jeffries, S., & Everatt, J. (2004). Working memory: Its role in dyslexia and
other specific learning disabilities. Dyslexia, 10(3), 196–214.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Johnson, C., & Goswami, U. (2010). Phonological awareness, vocabulary,
and reading in deaf children with cochlear implants. J Speech Lang Hear
The authors declare no conflict of interest. Res, 53, 237–261.
Address for correspondence: Lindsey Edwards, Great Ormond Street Khan, S., Edwards, L., Langdon, D. (2005). The cognition and behaviour
Hospital for Children, Cochlear Implant Programme, Great Ormond of children with cochlear implants, children with hearing aids and their
Street, London WC1N 3JH, England, United Kingdom. E-mail: Lindsey. hearing peers: A comparison. Audiol Neurootol, 10, 117–126.
Edwards@gosh.nhs.uk Kibby, M. Y., Marks, W., Morgan, S., et  al. (2004). Specific impairment
in developmental reading disabilities: a working memory approach.
Received 13 September, 2012; accepted 13 October, 2013. J Learn Disabil, 37, 349–363.
Korkman, M., Kemp, S., Kirk, U. (2007). NEPSY—Second Edition. Oxford,
United Kingdom: Pearson.
REFERENCES Kyle, F.E. & Harris, M. (2006). Concurrent correlates and predictors of
reading and spelling achievement in deaf and hearing school children.
Archbold, S., Harris, M., O’Donoghue, G., et al. (2008). Reading abilities J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ, 11, 273–288.
after cochlear implantation: The effect of age at implantation on out- Lyxell, B., Wass, M., Sahlén, B., et al. (2009). Cognitive development, read-
comes at 5 and 7 years after implantation. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryn- ing and prosodic skills in children with cochlear implants. Scand J Psy-
gol, 72, 1471–1478. chol, 50, 463–474.
Bishop, D. V., North, T., Donlan, C. (1996). Nonword repetition as a behav- Marschark, M., Rhoten, C., Fabich, M. (2007). Effects of cochlear implants
ioural marker for inherited language impairment: Evidence from a twin on children’s reading and academic achievement. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ,
study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 37, 391–403. 12, 269–282.
Carter, A. K., Dillon, C. M., Pisoni, D. B. (2002). Imitation of nonwords Meinzen-Derr, J., Wiley, S., Grether, S., et  al. (2010). Language perfor-
by hearing impaired children with cochlear implants: Suprasegmental mance in children with cochlear implants and additional disabilities.
analyses. Clin Linguist Phon, 16, 619–638. Laryngoscope, 120, 405–413.
Casserly, E. D., & Pisoni, D. B. (2013). Nonword repetition as a predictor of Menghini, D., Finzi, A., Carlesimo, G. A., et al. (2011). Working memory
long-term speech and language skills in children with cochlear implants. impairment in children with developmental dyslexia: Is it just a phono-
Otol Neurotol, 34, 460–470. logical deficity? Dev Neuropsychol, 36, 199–213.
Catts, H. W., Adlof, S. M., Hogan, T. P., et al. (2005). Are specific language Olson, R., & Datta, H. (2002). Visual-temporal processing in
impairment and dyslexia distinct disorders? J Speech Lang Hear Res, ­reading-disabled and normal twins. Read Writ, 15, 127–149.
48, 1378–1396. Pisoni, D. B., & Cleary, M. (2004). Learning, memory, and cognitive pro-
Cleary, M., Pisoni, D. B., Geers, A. E. (2001). Some measures of verbal and cesses in deaf children following cochlear implantation. In F. G. Zeng, A.
spatial working memory in eight- and nine-year-old hearing-impaired N. Popper, R. R. Fay (Eds.), Springer Handbook of Auditory Research:
children with cochlear implants. Ear Hear, 22, 395–411. Auditory Prosthesis, SHAR Volume X (pp. 377–426). New York, NY:
Cleary, M., Pisoni, D. B., Kirk, K. I. (2000). Working memory spans as pre- Springer.
dictors of spoken word recognition and receptive vocabulary in children Pisoni, D.B., Conway, C.M., Kronenberger, W., et  al. (2010). Executive
with cochlear implants. Volta Rev, 102, 259–280. function, cognitive control, and sequence learning in deaf children
Cohen, M. (1997). Children’s Memory Scale. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt with cochlear implants. In M. Marschark & P.E. Spencer (Eds.), The
Brace & Company. Oxford Handbook of Deaf Studies, Language and Education, Volume 2
Conti-Ramsden, G., & Durkin, K. (2007). Phonological short-term memory, (pp. 439–457). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
language and literacy: Developmental relationships in early adolescence Pisoni, D.B., Conway, C.M., Kronenberger, W.G., et  al. (2008). Efficacy
in young people with SLI. J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 48, 147–156. and effectiveness of cohlear implants in deaf children. In M. Marschark
de Jong, P. F. (1998). Working memory deficits of reading disabled children. & P.C. Hauser (Eds.), Deaf Cognition: Foundations and Outcomes
J Exp Child Psychol, 70, 75–96. (pp. 52–101). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Dillon, C. M., & Pisoni, D. B. (2006). Non word repetition and reading Pisoni, D. B., Kronenberger, W. G., Roman, A. S., et al. (2011). Measures of
skills in children who are deaf and have cochlear implants. Volta Rev, digit span and verbal rehearsal speed in deaf children after more than 10
106, 121–145. years of cochlear implantation. Ear Hear, 32(1), 60S–74S.
Dyer, A., MacSweeney, M., Szczerbinski, M., et al. (2003). Predictors of Rispens, J. & Parigger, E. (2010). Non-word repetition in Dutch-speaking
reading delay in deaf adolescents: The relative contributions of rapid children with specific language impairment with and without reading
automatized naming speed and phonological awareness and decoding. problems. Br J Dev Psychol, 28, 177–188.
J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ, 8, 215–229. Roid, G. H., & Miller, L. J. (1997). Leiter International Performance
Fagan, M. K., Pisoni, D. B., Horn, D. L., et al. (2007). Neuropsychological ­Scale-Revised. Wood Dale, IL: Stoelting Co.
correlates of vocabulary, reading, and working memory in deaf children Smith-Spark, J. H., & Fisk, J. E. (2007). Working memory functioning in
with cochlear implants. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ, 12, 461–471. developmental dyslexia. Memory, 15, 34–56.
Geers, A. E., & Hayes, H. (2010). Reading, writing, and phonological pro- Wechsler, D. (2005). Wechsler Individual Achievement Test—Second UK
cessing skills of adolescents with 10 or more years of cochlear implant Edition. Oxford, United Kingdom: Pearson.
experience. Ear Hear, 32(1), 49S–59S. Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. (1992). Test of Word Knowledge. Oxford, United
Geers, A., Tobey, E., Moog, J., et  al. (2008). Long-term outcomes of Kingdom: Pearson.
cochlear implantation in the preschool years: From elementary grades to Wilcox, K., & Morris, S. (1999). Children’s Speech Intelligibility Measure.
high school. Int J Audiol, 47 Suppl 2, S21–S30. Oxford, United Kingdom: Pearson.

Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

Вам также может понравиться