Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 15

Sex Roles (2018) 78:394–408

DOI 10.1007/s11199-017-0800-9

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

The Academic Conference as a Chilly Climate for Women: Effects


of Gender Representation on Experiences of Sexism, Coping
Responses, and Career Intentions
Jacklyn Biggs 1,2 & Patricia H. Hawley 1,3 & Monica Biernat 1

Published online: 20 June 2017


# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2017

Abstract Across many disciplines, women are underrepre- female academics, they may signal devalued status and lack
sented in faculty positions relative to men. The present re- of fit and as such play an inadvertent role in the Bleaky
search focuses on the academic conference as a setting be- pipeline.^ We discuss strategies that conference organizers
cause it is a gateway to an academic career and a context in could implement to mitigate sexist climates, including broader
which women might experience sexism. We surveyed 329 inclusion of women in speaking and leadership roles and ex-
presenters (63% women) from three U.S. national academic plicit attention to cues that women belong.
conferences, which differed in women-to-men ratios, about
their perceptions of the conference climate, their coping tactics
(e.g., gender performance, silence, or voice), and their inten- Keywords Sexism . Academic settings . Gender equality .
tions to exit the conference or academia. The greater the rep- Organizational climate . Career commitment
resentation of women at the conference relative to men, the
less likely were women to perceive sexism and to feel they had
to behave in a masculine manner in that setting. In contrast, Women have made considerable progress in terms of their
women who perceived the conference as sexist and felt si- representation in academia, yet men still outnumber women
lenced also expressed increased intentions to exit from aca- in faculty positions. In the social sciences, only 33.7% of the
demic careers. Men’s perceptions of sexism predicted in- faculty were women in 2010 (National Science Foundation
creased intentions to exit from that particular conference, but 2010). Data from the American Association of University
not from academia. Because conferences signal the norms of a Professors indicate that fewer women attain faculty positions
discipline, it is important to explore their climates as they than would be expected based on the number of female doc-
relate to gender. Perhaps especially for new and aspiring torates entering the job market (Monroe and Chiu 2010).
Moreover, women tend to occupy lower status positions in
academia and earn less money than men do (Ceci and
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article
(doi:10.1007/s11199-017-0800-9) contains supplementary material,
Williams 2011; Monroe and Chiu 2010). Although some of
which is available to authorized users. these gendered salary gaps in academia have decreased over
time, others have increased (Ceci et al. 2014).
* Jacklyn Biggs Women’s paths to success in academia continue to be com-
jacklynbiggs@ku.edu plicated by sexist attitudes toward women that put them at a
disadvantage (Katila and Meriläinen 1999; Monroe and Chiu
1
Department of Psychology, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS,
2010). Recent treatments of women’s underrepresentation
USA have focused on gender differences in skills and interests,
2
Present address: Center for Public Partnerships and Research,
family-related obstacles, hindrances at the level of early prep-
University of Kansas, 1617 St. Andrews Dr., Lawrence, KS 66047, aration, lack of role models, and gender bias (Monroe and
USA Chiu 2010; Shaw and Stanton 2012; van Anders 2004). Is it
3
Present address: College of Education, Texas Tech University, possible that the contexts in which scientists socialize create
Lubbock, TX, USA obstacles in and of themselves? The present study explores
Sex Roles (2018) 78:394–408 395

perceptions of sexism in one of these contexts that before now expressions and practices that are inadvertently stacked
has largely gone unexplored: the scientific conference. against women.
Two main questions guided this research. First, we ex-
plored the extent to which women’s representation within an
academic conference was related to both women’s and men’s Expressions of Sexism at Conferences
perceptions of conference climate pertaining to sexism, cop-
ing responses employed while at the conference (e.g., silence Sexism consists of the attitudes, behaviors, and practices that
and voice, gender performance), and desires to leave academia Breflect negative evaluations based on gender or support un-
or discontinue conference attendance as a consequence to equal status of women and men^ (Swim and Hyers 2009, p.
these perceptions (i.e., exit intentions). Second, for both men 407) and ranges from highly visible and blatant to less obvious
and women, we examined the extent to which perceptions of and subtle expressions. An organization’s climate is defined as
sexism were related to coping responses and intentions to exit the collective of individuals’ subjective perceptions of the
the conference and academic contexts, independent of gender values of the organization as derived through interactions with
representation at the conference. We examined these questions others in the environment and aspects of the physical space
in three social science conferences using an anonymous, self- (Settles and O’Connor 2014). Regardless of the visibility of
report online survey. The conferences were chosen because sexist behaviors and practices, a climate that is perceived to
they reflect three key varying proportions of female-to-male devalue women relative to men might serve as another hurdle
presenters: a higher proportion of women, a higher proportion in women’s academic career paths. Normative practices and
of men, and a relatively equal representation. Although ours is behaviors at the conference that privilege men and devalue
not the first study to examine conference climate (e.g., Settles women create a sexist climate (Hall and Sandler 1982; Janz
and O’Connor 2014, discussed in detail below), our research and Pyke 2000; Settles et al. 2006, 2007). Specifically, a sexist
uniquely compares men’s and women’s experiences across climate at the conference may be evident in shared personal
conference type (i.e., gender representation), and examines stories, reputed reports and anecdotes (i.e., reputational
how perceptions of sexism are related to attendees’ coping climate; Janz and Pyke 2000), the attitudes and behaviors that
responses as well as their intentions to remain in or exit from attendees witness or personally experience (i.e., sexist
academia. Our focus on conference experiences in these ways attitudes and treatment; Riger et al. 1997; Swim et al. 1995),
provides a unique empirical examination of how the range of and the materials and content presented (Janz and Pyke 2000;
subjective experiences at the conference are associated with Riger et al. 1997). Sexist expressions may include
academic exit intentions for women and men. condescending remarks, patronizing comments, off-colored
Organizational life can be understood as a reflection of jokes, power-differentiated flirtatiousness, or exclusion from
cultural attitudes (Gherardi 1994; Priola 2007; Riley et al. discussions and activities (Burgess and Borgida 1999; Caza
2006). Along these lines, Riley et al. (2006) contend that the and Cortina 2007; Cortina 2008; Swim et al. 2004). Content or
normative culture of academia in the United States is mascu- materials presented may be sexually suggestive, or exclude
line because it is reflective of a masculine-dominant society the contributions of women or feminist perspectives, all of
(i.e., one in which men are ascribed more power than women which can signal a devalued status of women within the con-
are; see also Eagly and Karau 2002; Wilson et al. 2010). A text (Janz and Pyke 2000).
masculine-normative culture is characterized by the mainte- Furthermore, perceptions of climate—although subjective
nance of men’s dominant status through practices and dis- by definition—matter. Women who perceive a sexist climate
course, including tenure standards, role assignments, and ex- in their department reported experiencing more sexual harass-
pectations of independence (Knights and Richards 2003). ment and scholarly alienation as well as less job satisfaction
Professional conferences are an integral part of academia, and influence in their departments (Settles et al. 2006, 2013).
and as such they too are likely to be situated within and there- Moreover, a recent study of men’s and women’s perceptions
fore reflective of the masculine normative culture. For exam- of the climate at an academic conference found that women
ple, a competitive ethos is inherent to the conference atmo- perceived the climate as more sexist and exclusionary than
sphere: Prospective presenters compete for limited slots to men did and that women reported experiencing more incivility
give talks, some of which are more desirable than others. (Settles and O’Connor 2014). In this same study, perceptions
Participation on panels is competitive as well, as are network- of climate sexism and positive climate mediated the relation-
ing opportunities especially with high status others (who are ship between perceived incivility at the conference and men’s
more likely to be men than they are to be women). In short, the and women’s personal feelings of intellectual and social ex-
academic conference is a publically visible context where sta- clusion from the conference. That is, sexist and negative cli-
tus and prestige may be attained. The lack of childcare may mate perceptions drove the relationship between incivility and
additionally limit young parents’ (particularly women’s) abil- feelings of conference exclusion; incivility alone was not di-
ity to attend and participate. This backdrop may lend itself to rectly predictive of feeling conference exclusion. Taken
396 Sex Roles (2018) 78:394–408

together, these results demonstrate that sexist climates are re- not cover the full breadth of coping responses employed at
lated to important professional outcomes, including satisfac- the conference setting.
tion, influence, and exclusion. Conference attendees who experience or witness sexism can
choose to express their discontent about sexism or to ignore it.
Voice is conceptualized as speaking out against sexist treatment
Women’s Numerical Representation and is considered an agentic, active response to sexism (Barreto
and Conferences and Ellemers 2005; Garcia et al. 2005). Expression of voice is
argued to be empowering for women (Gilligan 1982) because it
Women’s numerical representation within a specific academic has the potential to alter group norms and (Settles et al. 2007)
context may also influence perceptions of the climate. Low found that female academics who expressed voice experienced
representation of women in light of the existing power structure more job satisfaction than women who did not.
intensifies negative outcomes for professional women (Yoder Engaging in voice, however, is not without potential nega-
1991, 1994). Kanter (1977) proposed that group culture chang- tive consequence (Garcia et al. 2005; Major et al. 2002).
es as the proportion of women relative to men changes. People are motivated to maintain ingroup solidarity (see
According to this line of reasoning, because academia has tra- Levine and Moreland 2002), and vocal expression challeng-
ditionally been and continues to be predominantly occupied by ing prevailing norms threatens to undermine said solidarity.
men, the culture is masculine (Toren 1990). Increasing Thus, perceived costs to voice include hostility, dislike, and
women’s representation in principle should influence percep- even ostracism (Kaiser and Miller 2001; Stangor et al. 2002).
tions of the climate in a more favorable direction for women To avoid such consequences, self-silencing may occur. An al-
(Kanter 1977; Yoder 1991). Recent research on the stereotype ternate more common response to sexist treatment is to ignore
inoculation model (Dasgupta 2011) also points to the role of the transgression (Barreto and Ellemers 2005), even despite a
numerical representation in increasing positive outcomes for private desire to say something (Swim and Hyers 1999).
members of devalued groups. This latter model suggests that Additionally, women may resist publicly commenting during
the presence of both Bingroup experts^ and Bingroup peers^ in meetings or refrain from asking questions after a symposium
a given context increases identification, self-efficacy, and mo- because they think they will not be taken seriously or that they
tivation in achievement domains (Stout et al. 2011). Following are questioning the status quo (Kriwy et al. 2013; Rowe 1990).
this logic, increasing women’s numerical representation may The advantages of silence include a reduced chance of hostility
serve as an antidote to sexist climates. Likewise, when profes- and dislike; however, silence is neither an effective mechanism
sional women were underrepresented relative to men, women of cultural change nor a path to personal agency.
were more likely to express exit intentions and less job satis- Gender is Bsomething we think and something we do^
faction than when women were equally or predominantly rep- (Gherardi 1994, p. 592) and may be performed in stereotypi-
resented (Burke and McKeen 1996). cally masculine or feminine ways as a means of coping with
Academic social science conferences, the focus of the pres- sexist climates. One such coping response is to assimilate
ent research, certainly include women. However, the confer- oneself to masculine norms by enacting a masculine gendered
ences are likely situated within masculine norms, particularly performance (i.e., agentic, independent, and aggressive; Katila
when the numerical presence of women is low. A sexist climate and Meriläinen 1999). Doing so may reduce evaluation based
may accordingly and disproportionately influence women’s be- on gender (Schmitt et al. 2003) and increase evaluations of
haviors at the conference and lead to increased exit intentions Bfit^ because the norms of the group are not subverted (Deaux
for the conference itself and the academic career path. and Major 1987; Heilman et al. 2004; Katila and Meriläinen
1999; Powell et al. 2009).
Alternatively, women may cope with a sexist climate
Coping with Perceived Sexism in a Conference through feminine gendered performance. These behaviors
Context and attitudes include communal actions such as helping and
nurturance or passivity (i.e., low dominance; Lun et al. 2009).
A sexist climate may elicit several behavioral coping re- Women may engage in stereotypical feminine performance to
sponses from women (and men) while at the conference. For fit with what is expected of women (i.e., prescriptive
instance, attendees may choose to speak out against sexism at stereotypes; Burgess and Borgida 1999) and to avoid backlash
the conference (i.e., express voice) or they may stay silent and for not behaving in accordance with gender stereotypic behav-
say nothing (i.e., silence). Moreover, women and men may ior (Heilman 2001; Rudman 1998). Of course, behaving in
behave in gender stereotypic ways in order to fit in with norms ways that are consistent with feminine stereotypes may in-
of the conference context (i.e., gender performance). We crease the perceived Black of fit^ with masculine norms and
chose to focus on these coping responses (i.e., voice, silence, expectations (Eagly and Karau 2002; Heilman 2001, 2004).
gendered performance), recognizing though that these may Masculine conference settings may therefore shift women’s
Sex Roles (2018) 78:394–408 397

gendered performance coping responses toward masculine these attitudes and that women do not belong (Cheryan et al.
and/or feminine stereotypic behavior (i.e., self-stereotyping; 2009; Settles and O’Connor 2014).
Chiu et al. 1998; Hogg and Turner 1987; Lun et al. 2009). Our initial hypotheses focus on expected mean differences
Indeed, they may engage in both of these coping responses at in perceived conference climate based on gender, conference
different times during the same conference. Both masculine type (relative representation of women), and their interaction.
and feminine gendered performance might help women fit in Because sexism generally targets women (Benokraitis 1997),
with the varying gendered behavioral demands of numerous we hypothesized that women would perceive more sexism
conference contexts, but they risk contributing to a sense of than men would (Hypothesis 1a: main effect of participant
inauthenticity (Ryan and Deci 2004). gender), that the conference numerically dominated by wom-
en would be perceived as the least sexist (Hypothesis 1b: main
effect of conference), and that the gender difference would be
strongest in the conference setting where men are numerically
Conference and Academia Exit Intentions
dominant and eliminated in the context where women are
numerically dominant (Hypothesis 1c: interaction be-
One response to perceived inequity is to simply leave the
tween participant gender and conference type). As wom-
group (Tajfel 1975), particularly when it seems as though
en become more highly represented across conferences,
the situation will not improve (Farrell 1983; Hirschman
the group culture may shift in the direction of a reduc-
1970). In social identity theory terms, leaving the group (here,
tion of sexist attitudes and behavior, and, accordingly, a
the conference and/or academia), and joining another is a so-
reduction of gender differences in perceptions of sexism
cial mobility strategy (Tajfel and Turner 1986). Leaving may
should emerge (Burke and McKeen 1996).
be motivated by low perceived similarity or low feelings of
Silence, saying nothing, and voice, confronting, are two
belongingness (i.e., a lack of Bambient belonging^; Cheryan
complementary coping responses. Because both should be
et al. 2009), which can be communicated via subtle stereotyp-
heightened in those who perceive sexism, we predicted that
ic cues in the context (Cheryan et al. 2009; Cheryan and Plaut
women would be more likely to use both strategies than men
2010). Indeed, perceived dissimilarity with people in a do-
would (Hypothesis 2a), that the conference where women
main, even when controlling for gender proportions, can lead
were underrepresented would produce the highest levels of
to a lack of interest in that domain (Cheryan and Plaut 2010).
both silence and voice (Hypothesis 2b), and that the gender
In the conference context, climate cues communicating sex-
difference would be most evident in the context where men
ism (i.e., reputation, actions, beliefs, and materials) may signal
are numerically dominant and least evident when women are
to women that they do not belong and therefore contribute to
numerically dominant (Hypothesis 2c).
their exit intentions. Moreover, men who hold nonsexist atti-
We conceptualized engagement in gendered behavior—
tudes may also perceive a sexist climate as unwelcoming and
both feminine and masculine—as additional coping re-
express exit intentions as well. Exiting, like silence, does noth-
sponses. Masculine gendered performance reflects assimila-
ing to change the status quo, and the loss of human capital
tion to valued conference norms (Riley et al. 2006) and is
when well-trained individuals leave the field is a major
gender-normative behavior for men, whereas feminine gen-
concern. Understanding the role of sexism perceptions
dered performance is generally counter-normative for aca-
in exit intentions for women and men is a central goal
demic conferences and for men, but gender-normative for
of our research.
women. For these reasons, we expected a gender difference
in feminine gender performance (Hypothesis 3a), but no gen-
der difference in masculine gender performance (Hypothesis
The Present Study 3b). We also predicted a Gender x Conference interaction such
that men’s gender performance would not vary across confer-
We examined female and male conference-goers’ perceptions ence contexts but that women would be particularly likely to
of sexist conference climate, whether these perceptions dif- engage in masculine and feminine gender performance in the
fered based on women’s numerical representation at the con- most masculinized conference setting, when men are numer-
ference, and how these perceptions predicted coping re- ically dominant (Hypothesis 3c).
sponses and intentions to attend future conference meetings With regard to exit intentions, we had somewhat different
and pursue or maintain careers in academia. Conferences predictions regarding exiting the conference versus exiting
serve to socialize aspiring academics on practices and norms academia more broadly. With regard to exiting the conference,
of the field (Egri 1992); they also provide a medium through we predicted that women would be more likely than men
which aspiring academics get their first experiences with col- would be to seek to exit the conference dominated by men,
leagues from other institutions. If a conference climate is sex- but not the other conferences (a Gender x Conference interac-
ist, the young academic may conclude that the field shares tion, Hypothesis 4a). We thought it less likely that conference
398 Sex Roles (2018) 78:394–408

experiences would directly impact women’s broader career conferences: 205 women (62.3%); 275 (83.7%) Caucasian, 11
intentions, but expected a gender main effect (women more (3.3%) East Asian, 6 (1.8%) African American, 6 (1.8%)
likely to report intentions to leave academia than men do; South Asian, 22 (6.6%) other not listed/decline to answer;
Hypothesis 4b), and viewed the question of conference impact Mage = 35.22, SD = 10.41, range = 21–69 who responded to
as exploratory. an online survey. The sample consisted of academics at dif-
Our study also addresses the question of how perceptions ferent stages in their career: Undergraduate students (7, 2.1%),
of conference climate are related to coping responses and exit graduate students (111, 33.5%), post docs (23, 6.9%), assistant
intentions for women and men. We expected that perceptions professors (69, 20.8%), associate professors (36, 10.9%), full
of a sexist climate would positively predict engagement in professors (39, 11.8%), as well as non-tenure track profession-
silence, voice, and masculine and feminine gender perfor- al Ph.D.s (15, 4.5%). A power analysis indicated that a sample
mance, as well as academic and conference exit intentions size of 152 was needed to detect significance (p < .05), which
for women (Hypothesis 5). Perceived sexism and feelings of was achieved.
devaluation may prompt disengagement (silence, exit
intentions; Tsui and Gutek 1999), but may also encourage
voice as a means to change the situation (Farrell 1983; Procedure and Measures
Hirschman 1970). Perceptions of sexism may also prompt
women to engage in both masculine- and feminine-gendered The sample consisted of presenters who attended one of three
performance: masculine in order to meet expectations for suc- U.S. national academic, scientific conferences held in 2011.
cess and feminine to avoid backlash (Rudman 1998). Two of the conferences were multidisciplinary social science
We additionally hypothesized that silence and gendered meetings studying human development and behavior
performance (both masculine and feminine) would positively (Conference LW, for lowest representation of women to
predict conference and academic exit intentions (Hypothesis men, and Conference MW, more women than men), whereas
6a), whereas voice would negatively predict these intentions the other was a psychology-dominant social science meeting
(Hypothesis 6b). Silence may be reflective of experiencing studying human social behavior (Conference EW, equal rep-
detachment and a lack of belonging (Tsui and Gutek 1999), resentation of women and men). Specific names of the con-
and having to perform gender in order to fit in with others’ ferences are kept anonymous per the IRB protocol.
gendered expectations may also contribute to a lack of ambi- Conferences were chosen based on their general topic of study
ent belonging (Cheryan et al. 2009; Good et al. 2012), inau- (i.e., human behavior) and varied gender numerical
thenticity, and ill-being (Ryan and Deci 2004). Expressing representations.
voice, on the other hand, is an active attempt to change, rather Names and affiliations of participants identified for the
than to escape, the situation (Hirschman 1970; Farrell 1983). sample were found in publicly available conference programs,
Therefore, women who express voice may do so despite social and we used internet search engines to find publicly available
risks because they wish to change the normative culture of a email addresses for all presenters (regardless of authorship
conference (Crosby et al. 2003). Finally, we predicted that order). Some email addresses could not be found, leaving a
silence, voice, and gender performance would serve as medi- total potential presenter pool of 853 from Conference LW.
ators of the relationship between climate perceptions and exit Because LW was the smallest conference, we oversampled
intentions for women (Hypothesis 7). by issuing email invitations to all 853 identified presenters,
Our predictions mainly target women, given the greater then we used that number as a basis for sampling from
relevance of sexism for their lives. But men may find sexist Conference EW (where n = 853 reflects 27.72% of total pre-
climates unappealing as well, and some of the relationships senters) and Conference MW (12.8% of total presenters) using
found for women may hold for men. Still, we had less clear Microsoft Excel’s random selection tool.
predictions regarding the effects of conference climate percep- Research assistants recorded participants’ gender when
tions on coping responses and exit intentions in men. possible using all available public information, such as pho-
tographs and personal and professional bios. These proce-
dures are biased and may not depict the exact representation
Method of women at these conferences; however, they capture a sub-
jective and general numerical representation of women of the
Participants conference. Categorization of conferences was based on the
collective gender representation of the presenters invited to
Online conference programs were consulted to derive a list of participate in the study. Conference MW had the greatest pro-
potential participants to the present study, as described in more portion of women presenters/co-presenters in 2011 (71.6%
detail in the following. The final sample included 329 present- women), Conference LW had the smallest proportion (38%
ing conference participants from three national social science women), and Conference EW was roughly at parity (53.6%
Sex Roles (2018) 78:394–408 399

women). The final sample of 329 reflects these gender distri- 7 (strongly agree) response scales. After reverse scoring
butions as well. such that high numbers indicate greater perception of
Participants were recruited through an email invitation sent sexist attitudes, an average across the seven items was
directly to them, approximately 1–3 months after the confer- computed (α = .62).
ences took place. Response rates from each conference were Sexist perceptions of the conference material (i.e., sexism
relatively low (10.8–17.6%), but this was expected given the present in research presented via talks, posters, etc.) were
survey conditions (i.e., electronic survey, academic measured using (a) one item adapted from the PCCS for the
participants; Cook et al. 2000). The survey was conducted conference context (Janz and Pyke 2000): BResearch is pre-
using Qualtrics software. Demographic items were sented from a broad range of perspectives throughout the con-
placed at the beginning of every survey, and each scale ference (i.e., includes the perspectives of women, feminists,
appeared on a separate page. The order of presentation minorities, etc)^ reverse scored; (b) one item from the
of the following measures was randomized, as was the AWESW (Riger et al. 1997): BDuring the conference, infor-
order of items within each scale. A full list of items can mal gatherings and conversations about conference material
be found in an online supplement. tend to include female students or colleagues^ (reverse
scored); and (c) three other items created by the research team
Sexist Climate to assess perceptions of sexist content in talks and/or posters
and audience reactions: BTalks or posters at this conference
Conference climate was measured with three subscales: repu- have been presented from a gender stereotypical perspective^;
tational climate, perceptions of sexist attitudes and treatment, BAudience reactions to talks or posters at this conference have
and impressions of the conference material. Reputational been uncomfortable (e.g., cat calls, whistles, snickers)^; and
climate refers to the climate that attendees Bhave heard about^ BTalks or posters given at the conference have contained gra-
and was operationalized using seven items, five from an tuitous sexual content through verbal and/or visual
adapted version of Janz and Pyke’s (2000) Perceptions of presentations.^ Responses were made on 1 (strongly disagree)
Chilly Climate Survey (PCCS). The PCCS was originally to 7 (strongly agree) scales. After reverse scoring where nec-
developed for a classroom context; we changed wording essary such that high numbers reflect greater perceptions of
slightly to reflect the conference context. For example, we sexism, an average was computed (α = .61). (We also mea-
changed Bteaching staff^ to Ba member at this conference^ sured participants’ personal experiences of microinequities
and added references to colleagues (e.g., BI have heard of during the conference. Due to overlap with the perceived sex-
one or more instances where a member at this conference ism items and multicollinearity issues, microinequities were
engaged in inappropriate physical contact toward a female not included in the present analyses.)
student or colleague^). Two additional items assessing the
reputational climate at conference after-parties and social Voice and Silence
events were included: BThe atmosphere at conference after-
parties/events is sexualized or otherwise inappropriate^ and BI The extent to which participants engaged in silence (α = .66)
have heard conference after-parties/events are centered around and expressed voice (α = .68) were measured with three and
alcohol.^ All items were rated on 1 (never) to 7 (many times) two items each, respectively. The silence items were devel-
scales. Items were averaged to create a reputational climate oped based on work by Powell et al. (2009), Swim and Hyers
index, such that higher numbers reflect more perceived sexism (1999), and Swim et al. (2010), designed to reflect outwardly
(α = .75). saying nothing in response to sexism and refraining from
Seven additional items were included to assess sexist atti- speaking up during meetings, discussions, and presentations:
tudes and treatment that attendees perceived and observed BWhile at the conference, to what extent do you . . . refrain
within the conference context. Items were selected from three from speaking up during meetings or discussions? . . . resist
different existing scales and minimally adapted to reference commenting during and/or after talks/presentations? . . . say
the conference context: The PCCS (Janz and Pyke 2000; e.g., nothing in response to sexist comments or behaviors?^ Voice
BA woman must out-perform a male colleague to be taken items were based on Gilligan (1982), Settles et al. (2007), and
seriously at this conference^), the Modern Sexism Scale Garcia et al. (2005): BWhile at the conference to what extent
(MSS; Swim et al. 1995; e.g., BMany members at this confer- do you . . . confront someone who made a sexist comment or
ence believe that society has reached the point where women behaved in a sexist way? . . . express concerns to others re-
and men have equal opportunities for achievement,^ reverse garding sexist comments or behaviors?^. Items were mea-
scored) and the Academic Work Environment Scale for sured on 1 (never) to 7 (many times) scales. The voice items
Women (AWESW; Riger et al. 1997; e.g., BMembers at this and the third silence item included the following option: BN/A
conference respect both male and female colleagues equally,^ because I’ve not encountered sexist behaviors or comments.^
reverse scored). All were measured on 1 (strongly disagree) to These N/A responses were treated as missing data in the
400 Sex Roles (2018) 78:394–408

ANOVAs we report here. For the SEM analysis, in which all factorial invariance (i.e., configural invariance, weak invari-
variables were considered simultaneously, missing values ance, and strong invariance) indicates that we measured the
were imputed using full information maximum likelihood same construct across groups for cross-group comparisons
(FIML; Enders 2010). Item responses for each measure were (Brown 2006). All these criteria were met.
averaged such that higher scores indicated more engagement We also examined whether participants from the three con-
in either silence or giving voice. ferences differed on demographic variables. Race/ethnicity
did not differ across conferences nor did academic status,
Gendered Performance but participants from the EW conference (M = 33.57,
SD = 9.53) were significantly younger than those from the
Gendered performance was operationalized as personal en- MW (M = 36.51, SD = 11.02) and LW conferences
gagement in stereotypically feminine or masculine behaviors. (M = 36.93, SD = 10.33), F(2320) = 3.88, p < .03.
Two items measuring feminine gendered performance (α = Therefore, all mean level analyses include controls for age.
.65) were developed from the theoretical work of
Walkerdine (1989): BWhile at the conference, to what extent
do you… behave in typically feminine ways, such as being Results
soft-spoken and yielding? . . . try to act like Bone of the
girls^?^ Two items measuring masculine gender performance Overview
(α = .67) were developed based on work on assimilation (Sam
and Berry 2010) and gendered behavior in masculine domains To address Hypotheses 1–4, which concern mean differences
(Deaux and Major 1987; Katila and Meriläinen 1999): BWhile in climate perceptions, coping responses, and exit intentions
at the conference, to what extent do you . . . behave in stereo- by gender and conference type, a series of Conference x
typically masculine ways, such as being competitive . . . Participant Gender MANOVAs was conducted, using GLM
try to act like ‘one of the guys’?^ Responses were made procedures to account for unequal cell sizes. Structural equa-
on 1 (never) to 7 (many times) scales and were aver- tion modeling (SEM) was then used to examine Hypotheses
aged such that higher scores indicated higher levels of 5–7, which concern the extent to which climate perceptions
masculine or feminine gender performance. affected coping responses and exit intentions for men and
women, independently. In the SEM model, the BClimate^ la-
Exit Intentions tent construct was based on three parcels reflecting the repu-
tational climate, sexist attitudes and treatment, and conference
Three items focused on intent to exit tenure-track academia as material subscales. All other latent constructs are based on
a profession (α = .69; BI intend to pursue or continue a tenure- individual scale items as outlined in the measure descriptions.
track academic career,^ BI feel well-suited to the academic An overview of the data can be seen in Table 1, which
atmosphere of my chosen discipline,^ and BI am confident reports means on the climate, coping, and exit intentions in-
that I can succeed in a tenure-track academic career,^ all re- dexes, along with correlations among all indexes, separately
verse scored). Three additional items measured intent to exit for women and men. The correlational findings indicate that
from the conference (α = .69; e.g., BI plan to attend this con- the sexist climate scales are modestly intercorrelated for wom-
ference in the future,^ BI intend to continue my membership in en and men (rs = .26–.43), justifying their joint consideration
the academic society associated with this conference,^ and BI in a MANOVA. The four coping responses showed a mixed
like attending this conference,^ all reverse scored). These pattern of intercorrelations. Silence and voice were not corre-
items were informed by the work of Cortina et al. (2001) lated for women (r = .05) and only weakly correlated for men
and Lim et al. (2008). All items were measured on 1 (strongly (r = .21), but because we predicted similar patterns for these
disagree) to 7 (strongly agree) scales and were averaged such two variables, both are considered in a single MANOVA.
that higher values capture stronger intentions to exit. The modest correlations between feminine and masculine
gendered performance (rs = .32 and .35) warranted their
Analytic Procedure joint consideration in a separate MANOVA, as did the
small but significant correlations between the two exit
Scale development was theory-driven, as discussed in the pri- variables (rs = .18 and .19).
or measures. Before mean differences or predictive paths were
investigated, a multiple-group confirmatory factor analysis Sexist Climate
(CFA) was conducted to specify the theoretically derived mea-
surement model and to ensure the data fit the theoretical model Hypothesis 1a was that women would perceive the conference
(Brown 2006). Measurement equivalence (i.e., factorial in- climate as more sexist than men would. The MANOVA in-
variance) across men and women was tested. Establishing cluding the three climate indexes revealed a reliable omnibus
Sex Roles (2018) 78:394–408 401

Table 1 Descriptive statistics and correlations among measures by gender

Measure Women Men Correlations

M (SD) M (SD) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

1. Climate-REP 2.23a (1.05) 2.10b (.80) – .37** .41** .05 .33** .34** .15* -.05 -.003
2. Climate-SAT 3.08a (.85) 2.83b (.63) .43** – .33** .23** .14* .18* .08* .20 .13
3. Climate-CM 2.62a (.62) 2.35b (.59) .26** .40** – .16* .23* .20** .18** .11 .10
4. Silence 3.55 a (1.57) 3.10b (1.46) .11 .14 .16 – .05 .11 .37** .04 .22**
5. Voice 2.54 a (1.53) 2.09 b (1.37) .25 .02 .18 .21* – .17 .004 -.06 -.07
6. MasGP 2.59 a (1.37) 2.63 a (1.39) .18 .05 .13 .08 .22 – .32** -.07 .004
7. FemGP 2.31 a (1.27) 1.57 b (.84) .11 -.03 .21* .35** .37** .35** – -.05 .10
8. ExitConf 1.95 a (1.26) 1.81 a (1.24) .16 .27** .35** .16 .16 .08 .06 – .19**
9. ExitAcad 2.49 a (1.41) 1.19 b (1.16) -.03 .001 .13 .15 -.03 .02 .12 .18* –

Correlations for women (n = 206) are presented above the diagonal, and for men (n = 123), below, with the exception of correlations with Bvoice^, where
n = 90 for women and n = 48 for men. Different subscripts for the means for women and men indicate signficiant gender differences (p < .05)
Climate- REP Reputational Climate, Climate-SAT Sexist Attitudes and Treatment, Climate – CM Conference Material, Mas GP Masculine Gendered
Performance, FemGP Feminine Gendered Performance, ExitConf Exit Conference, ExitAcad Exit Academia
*p < .05. **p < .01

main effect of gender, F(3301) = 7.22, p < .001 (see means in omnibus main effect of gender, F(2128) = 4.80, p = .010,
Table 1), which held for each of the three individual climate which held for both silence, F(1129) = 5.13, p = .025, ηp2 =
indexes, Fs(1303) = 6.77, 7.92, and 19.52, for reputational .04, and voice, F(1129) = 5.54, p = .020, ηp2 = .04. As can be
climate, sexist attitudes and treatment, and conference mate- seen in Table 1, women reported both more silence and voice
rials, respectively, ps < .01, ηp2 = .02, .03, .06. Also consistent than men did. Hypothesis 2b was not supported in that there
with Hypothesis 1b, the main effect of conference was signif- was no main effect of conference type, F(4256) = 1.17,
icant, F(6602) = 7.53, p < .001, again on all three climate p = .32, but the interaction predicted in Hypothesis 2c was
indexes, Fs(2303) = 12.23, 9.36, and 3.95, respectively, p- significant at the omnibus level, F(4256) = 2.66, p = .033, and
s < .03, ηp2 = .07, .06, .03. The lowest levels of sexism were for voice in particular, F(2129) = 4.10, p = .019, ηp2 = .06, but
perceived in the conference dominated by women not silence (F(2129) = 1.45, p = .239). Women (M = 3.48,
(MMW = 1.68, 2.79, and 2.45, SDs = .73, .74, .48 for reputa- SD = 1.58) reported more voice than men (M = 1.91,
tional, sexist attitudes, and conference materials, respectively), SD = 1.07) only in the conference where they were underrep-
compared to the EW (MEW = 2.41, 3.22, and 2.53, SDs = .93, resented (LW, p < .001, d = 1.16), but not in the EW
.79, .56) and LW conferences (MLW = 2.40, 2.83, and 2.62, (Mwomen = 2.47, SD = 1.55, Mmen = 2.06, SD = 1.47) or
SDs = 1.05, .74, .85). These effects were qualified by the MW conferences (Mwomen = 1.94, SD = 1.08, Mmen = 2.42,
predicted Conference × Gender interaction, F(6602) = 3.34, SD = 1.63; ps > .08). Additionally, women at the LW confer-
p = .003. Univariate analyses indicated that the interaction was ence expressed more voice than did women at either the EW
significant for the reputational climate, F(2303) = 5.39, (d = .63) or MW (d = 1.12) conference, ps < .01 (who did not
p = .005, ηp2 = .03, and conference materials, differ from each other).
F(2303) = 3.21, p = .042, ηp2 = .02, indexes, although not Gendered performance—both masculine and feminine—
for the sexist attitudes and treatment index, F(2303) = .30, reflect additional coping responses to perceived sexism. In
p = .73. These interactions (with their means) are presented Hypotheses 3a and 3b, we predicted a main effect of gender
in Fig. 1. As predicted in Hypothesis 1c, the gender difference on feminine, but not masculine performance. The omnibus
in perceived sexism was strongest in the LW conference gender effect was significant, F(2305) = 9.84, p < .001, and
(d = .89 for reputational climate, d = .78 for conference ma- as predicted, univariate analyses indicated that women en-
terials, ps < .0001), and it was nonsignificant in both the EW gaged in more feminine gendered performance than men did
and LW conferences (ps > .06). (see Table 1), F(1306) = 17.30, p < .001, ηp2 = .05, but men
and women were equally likely to engage in masculine gen-
Coping Responses dered performance, F(1306) = .06, p > .81. We additionally
predicted a Gender x Conference interaction (Hypothesis 3c),
Coping responses to sexism included silence and voice. but the omnibus test was not significant, F(4610) = 1.87,
Consistent with Hypothesis 2a, the MANOVA indicated an p = .11, and therefore we did not support the hypothesis that
402 Sex Roles (2018) 78:394–408

Fig. 1 Conference x Gender


interactions on sexist climate
indexes: (a) Reputational Climate Women Men
2.89
and (b) Conference Materials,
where bars represent standard

Mean
2.47
errors 2.31
2.01
1.66 1.74

LW - Low representation of women EW - Equal representation of MW - More women then men


women
Conference type
(a) Sexist Reputational Climate

3.5 Women Men


2.97
3
2.60
Mean

2.51
2.5 2.34 2.41
2.17
2

1.5

1
LW - Low representation of women EW - Equal representation of MW - More women then men
women
Conference type
(b) Sexist Conference Materials

women’s gendered performance would vary by conference sample size limitations we were unable to further delineate
context. by conference. Fit to our theorized two-group model was ac-
ceptable, χ2 (254, n = 329) = 365.82, p < .01, RMSEA = .05,
Exit Intentions 95% CI [.04, .06], NNFI = .91, CFI = .93. All reported effects
are standardized; see Fig. 2 for final model and Table 1 for
The MANOVA including intentions to exit the conference and correlations among constructs by gender.
academia indicated only a main effect of gender, For women, as predicted in Hypothesis 5, perceptions of
F(2297) = 3.12, p = .046 which held at the univariate level the climate as sexist significantly predicted silence (β = .21,
only for academia exit intentions, F(1298) = 5.94, p = .015, SE = .09, p = .013), voice (β = .40, SE = .14, p = .003),
ηp2 = .02, but not conference exit intentions, F(1298) = 1.10, feminine performance (β = .34, SE = .10, p = .001), masculine
p = .25. Thus, we found no support for Hypothesis 4a, which performance (β = .40, SE = .10, p < .001), and academic exit
predicted a Gender x Conference interaction on conference intentions (β = .16, SE = .08, p = .044), but not conference exit
exit intentions (interaction F < 1), but did support intentions (β = .10, SE = .09, p > .05, p = .241). Support was
Hypothesis 4b, with women more likely to intend to leave less strong for Hypothesis 6 in that only silence significantly
academics than men were (see Table 1). predicted academic exit intentions (β = .20, SE = .06,
p = .001); the other coping responses did not (ps > .20).
Effects of Sexist Conference Climate Perceptions Silence also mediated the path between sexist climate and exit
on Coping and Exit Intentions intentions (Hypothesis 7): Those women who responded to
perceived climate sexism with silence during the conference
Structural equation modeling using MPlus (Version 7, 2012) were the most likely to intend to exit the academy. Again
was used to test whether conference climate predicted however, only silence played this mediating role for women.
women’s and men’s coping responses and exit intentions. Predictions for men were less certain, but as can be seen in
The measurement model was established using CFA. We test- Fig. 2, results for men were fairly comparable to those for
ed the model separately for women and men, but due to women. Perceptions of the climate as sexist significantly
Sex Roles (2018) 78:394–408 403

Representation, Climate, Coping, and Exit Intentions


Silence Exit:
Academia
Our first set of research questions (Hypotheses 1–4) addressed
the extent to which perceptions of sexism, silence and voice,
gender performance, and exit intentions varied by gender of
Sexist
Voice the participant and conference type (i.e., numerical represen-
Climate tation). Previous research has suggested that gender propor-
tions in professional contexts influence professional out-
Exit:
Masculine Conference comes, including the culture of the organization (i.e., the cli-
Gendered
Performance mate) and exit intentions for women (Burke and McKeen
1996). Twenty years on, our work suggests that this may still
be the case.
Feminine
Gendered Women overall were more likely to report perceiving sex-
Performance
ism than men did across all conferences, as we expected
(Hypothesis 1). In general, women are more likely the target
Fig. 2 Final structural equation model of the significant relationships
among the latent constructs for women and men. Model fit was good,
of sexist attitudes and behaviors than are men (Benokraitis
χ2 (254, n = 329) = 365.82, p < .01, RMSEA = .05, 90% CI [.04, .06], 1997). Regarding numerical representation, we found that
NNFI = .91, CFI = .93. Standardized coefficients (β) are reported, with both women and men perceived the conference climate as
coefficients for women listed first; for men, second.*p < .05. **p < .01. least sexist when there were more women than men present.
***p < .001
At the same time, men and women differed in their percep-
tions of the conference with lowest female representation:
predicted engagement in silence (β = .26, SE = .12, p = .025),
Women perceived this climate as the most sexist whereas
feminine performance (β = .45, SE = .18, p = .013), and
men did not. Men’s perceptions in the LW condition in fact
masculine performance (β = .25, SE = .12, p = .029), but not
did not differ from the equal representation nor the female
voice (β = .27, SE = .17, p = .098). One noteworthy difference
majority representation conditions. Furthermore, although
in findings for men was that sexist climate predicted intention
women generally perceived more sexism than men do overall,
to exit the conference (β = .63, SE = .10, p < .001), but not
this gender difference in perceptions was only reliable when
academia (β = .15, SE = .08, p = .059). There were no indirect
women were in the minority in the conference setting. Thus, if
paths from sexist climate perceptions to exit intentions for
low representation of women contributes to a climate problem
men (ps > .20).
in the eyes of women, male attendees appear to be unaware of
The structural models for men and for women were com-
this perception.
pared to see if they statistically differed from one another.
We also examined various coping responses to sexism by
Examination of both Chi-square difference tests (ps < .05)
men and women, including silence and voice, and gendered
and Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC) indicated that sep-
performance (Hypotheses 2 and 3). As predicted, women en-
arate models for men and women were appropriate
gaged in more silence and voice than did men, regardless of
(AIC f r e e = 16,638.17; AIC m a l e = f e m a l e = 16,643.03;
numerical representation. Contrary to our expectation, how-
AICfemale=male = 16,647.81).
ever, there was no main effect of conference type for silence or
voice. The predicted interaction (Hypothesis 2c) did not
emerge for silence, but did for voice. Thus, the conference
Discussion where women perceived the most sexism, they also expressed
the most voice. Also as predicted, women were more likely to
Women’s underrepresentation in academia compared to men’s engage in feminine performance than men were, regardless of
compels research on understanding obstacles to women’s suc- conference context (Hypothesis 3a). With regard to masculine
cess and the reasons women exit academic careers, including gender performance, we expected and found no gender differ-
the role of the conference context (see also Settles and ences (Hypothesis 3b) because these behaviors are normative
O’Connor 2014). To our knowledge, our research is the first in the competitive atmosphere of academia (Katila and
to specifically examine women’s and men’s numerical repre- Meriläinen 1999; Schmitt et al. 2003). However, we did not
sentation at conferences, sexist climate perceptions, coping support Hypothesis 3c, that women’s gendered performance
responses, and exit intentions. Moreover, the present would vary across conference contexts. Instead, the predicted
study documents systematic differences by gender and interacton was nonsignificant; neither men nor women’s gen-
type of conference (i.e., gender representation) as well dered performance varied across conferences.
as connections among sexist climate, silencing, and ac- Regarding exit intentions, overall intentions to exit the con-
ademic exit intentions for women. ference were very low and did not vary by gender or
404 Sex Roles (2018) 78:394–408

conference type. All of the conferences seemed to provide We were uncertain what to expect among men. We found
enough interest and value that participants (at least those that for men, perceptions of a sexist climate predicted all of the
who were inclined to respond to our survey) intended to attend coping strategies except voice; even when men perceived sex-
again. Intentions to exit academic careers also did not vary by ism, they were not likely to speak out about it. The effect on
conference, and therefore we did not support our prediction feminine gender performance is particularly surprising; men
that women in particular would flee from the setting in which who behaved in stereotypically feminine ways were behaving
few women were represented (Hypothesis 4a). However, outside of the gender expectations for men as well as for the
women expressed greater intent to exit academia than men academic conference expectations for success (Burgess and
did. This finding is consistent with Hypothesis 4b and with Borgida 1999; Knights and Richards 2003; Moss-Racusin
other research pointing to the loss of women in the professo- et al. 2010). Moreover, men’s perceptions of sexism were
riate in many social and biological science fields. This loss of positively associated with their desire to exit from the confer-
women generally happens in the transition from Ph.D. to as- ence, but not from academia. Thus, men appear more likely
sistant professorship (Ceci et al. 2014). Many reasons under- than women to confine their perceptions of sexism to the
lying this Bleaky pipeline^ have been discussed in the litera- conference itself rather than to the field as a whole.
ture, including gender stereotypes surrounding STEM topics Because the loss of women in social science fields occurs at
(Steffens et al. 2010), lack of role models for women (Drury the Ph.D. to assistant professor level, some have argued that
et al. 2011; Stout et al. 2011), gendered hiring practices Bchilly climate^ cannot account for women’s lesser tendency to
(Sheltzer and Smith 2014), and work-family balance pressures remain in academe: BThe deleterious effect of chilly climate
(Goulden et al. 2011; van Anders 2004). The present study accrues over time as women feel isolated and disrespected by
focused on sexist climate, but regrettably cannot differentiate male colleagues...graduate students and postdocs are hardly
among these other possible reasons behind exit intentions. aware of chilly climate; thus although it is possible that chilly
climate may cause leakage in the post-hiring stage, it is highly
unlikely that it does so before one even applies for a job^ (Ceci
Sexist Climate, Coping Responses, and Exit Intentions et al. 2011, pp. 256–257). In addition to our doubting the as-
sumption that chilly departmental or disciplinary climates have
In addition to the mean difference results, we also explored no effect on students, our findings additionally suggest that the
linear relationships in a structural equations modeling frame- climate at professional conferences may be one mechanism by
work. Consistent with Hypothesis 5, perceptions of a sexist which graduate students and postdocs receive messages that
climate increased the likelihood that during the conference deter their commitment to academic careers.
women reported engaging in silence, expressing voice, and
engaging in both feminine and masculine performance. Practice Implications
Interestingly, silence and voice were uncorrelated for women,
but feminine and masculine performance were positively re- That women perceive more sexism at conferences than men
lated, as were silence and feminine performance. These find- do should come as no surprise. Women’s perceptions are in
ings suggest that the coping responses were not mutually ex- part a consequence of experiences of microaggressions and
clusive, and instead may have been engaged strategically, thwarted efforts, and perhaps shared experiences with other
throughout the conference. Future research would benefit by women. In the conference with few women, however, despite
considering moment-to-moment responses to a sexist climate, women’s high perceptions of sexism and reports of voice, men
and to whom voice was expressed and under what conditions. did not perceive the sexism in the same way. This lack of
Also as hypothesized, women’s academic exit intentions perception by men—despite voice expressed by women at
were predicted by sexist climate and by silencing responses. that conference—makes it unlikely that the climate will posi-
Indeed, one woman conferee commented, B…covert sexism tively change. Men may doubt women’s experiences and may
persists—men do all the talking at most meetings at the uni- react in ways that marginalize women further. This risk is
versity as well as conferences...^ Perceptions of a sexist con- heightened if men in that context worry that gains for women
ference climate can signal to women the extent to which the imply losses for men (i.e., a zero-sum perspective; Kehn and
field values women’s contributions relative to men’s, as well Ruthig 2013; Ruthig et al. 2017). This worry may be even
as may contribute to a lack of belongingness (Cheryan et al. greater in men if they themselves have had a personal experi-
2009; Cheryan and Plaut 2010). Although we predicted a ence that they believe to be related to discrimination in which
broader role for coping responses in mediating the effects of a woman was favored. In the words of one young male par-
climate sexism on exit intentions, we found that only silence ticipant from the LW conference:
played this role (Hypothesis 7). Those women who responded
with silence expressed greater intention to exit from academia, I just finished ...graduate school and I have a great pub-
and silence mediated the climate-exit relationship. lication record... I sometimes feel frustrated when I see a
Sex Roles (2018) 78:394–408 405

female and/or minority secure a good assistant professor of women speakers on panels and could take steps signifying
position when their publication record is clearly not as that women are welcome (e.g., on-site childcare, implementa-
good as a white male's... I believe this is an issue that is tion of anti-discrimination policy). Additionally, societies
worth discussing publicly. hosting conferences should strive to make visible women’s
influence and input at the conference (Katila and Meriläinen
Alternatively, if men (and women) in a given context hold 1999). To this end, conferences should have women involved
beliefs that sexism no longer exists (Swim et al. 1995), the in all levels of conference leadership (Eagly and Carli 2007)
status quo favoring men may be sustained (e.g., Becker and and include women’s perspectives on research topics during
Swim 2012). One woman observed: the conference as well on their conference experiences.
Finally, inappropriate advertising material, sexualized social
I think the biggest problem in our field regarding gender gatherings, blatantly bad behavior, inappropriate images in
issues is... that there are still some offenders... enough posters and slides could be explicitly censured (see Biernat
that many women I know have had the experience... of a and Hawley 2017). One woman commented:
man in the field behaving inappropriately towards
them... but I suspect many men in my field think it is Most of the gender-based harassment I see is directed
still more rare than it actually is. toward the female PhD students... It’s generally sexual-
ized situations (flirtatiousness, efforts to create compro-
The present research suggests that conference climate may mised situations, etc).... It’s almost all at the parties
affect not only behavior at the conference itself but also aca- where the women stop attending when they Bgrow up^
demic decisions downstream. The finding that women who and become faculty members, but a subset of men reg-
engaged in silencing during the conference expressed greater ularly attend... the students are taken by their Bfame^
desire to exit from academic careers is concerning. The con- and enjoy the attention until they realize... sexualized
ference is a reflection of the academic discipline, and for many motives.
new and aspiring academics the conference might have pro-
vided early socialization into the norms of the discipline. For
women in particular, the conference may have signaled their Limitations and Future Directions
devalued status and lack of fit (Tsui and Gutek 1999), which in
turn triggered silence that predicted academic exit intentions. Academic conferences, as the powerful socializing contexts
Men expressed intention to exit the conference to the extent they are, have not been the target of much empirical research
they perceived it as sexist, but sexist conference climate (but see Settles and O’Connor 2014); we found no published
served as more of an obstacle to women’s than men’s careers. and validated measures of conference perceptions, experi-
Participants also responded to perceived sexism with voice, ences, coping responses, or exit intentions. Thus, we
but voice frequency was low overall and voice was unrelated employed and adapted measures from those validated in other
to conference and academic exit intentions. However, expres- contexts or were created based on social psychological and
sion of voice is agentic and empowering (Gilligan 1982); it feminist literature. Doing so unavoidably leads to some degree
can also bring attention to objectionable situations (Crosby of measurement error. At the same time, however, measure-
et al. 2003) and change the norms of the context. It is some- ment error tends to lead to Type II rather than Type I errors.
what encouraging that women at the LW conference—who That we found significant patterns in the expected directions
perceived the most sexism overall—also engaged in the most indicates to us that our efforts were not entirely fruitless.
voice and expressed no greater intent to exit academia than Nonetheless, future research on conference contexts should
their female counterparts at other conferences did. (a) be undertaken energetically because of its importance
Given these considerations regarding men’s perceptions and (b) validate measures relevant to conferences and conduct
and beliefs as well as women’s silence and voice, perhaps a longitudinal studies to speak to causality.
more explicit challenging of sexism may be necessary to On this latter point of causality stands another limitation of
broadly change conference norms and increase feelings of the present study: Namely, the patterns revealed associated
inclusion for women. Such a challenge may begin, for exam- with gender representation may not have been due to repre-
ple, with a grassroots establishment of a women’s group with- sentation per se, but rather to fundamental differences in con-
in the broader society. Doing so not only provides space for ference content. The degree to which content topics were
women to express their views and experiences, but also sig- causally or spuriously associated with gender representation
nals to the rest of the society that such a group was necessary cannot be discerned by the present study alone. The three
in the first place. Moreover, conference organizers could ac- conferences differed not only in numerical representation of
tively implement strategies to mitigate sexist climates. For women, but also in overall size and disciplinary focus.
instance, they could do more to increase the representation Though confounded in the real world, future research should
406 Sex Roles (2018) 78:394–408

explore ways to tease apart the effects of gender representation versus pervasiveness of benevolent sexism. Social Psychology,
43(3), 127–137. doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000091.
and disciplinary content at conferences on outcomes. In
Benokraitis, N. V. (1997). Sex discrimination in the 21st century. In N. V.
the end, we believe our focus on gender representation Benokraitis (Ed.), Subtle sexism: Current practice and prospects for
is a strength of the present study that opens the doors to change (pp. 5–33). Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications, Inc.
future work. Biernat, M., & Hawley, P. (2017). Sexualized images in professional
contexts: Effects on anticipated experiences and perceived climate
Additionally, the overall survey response rates were low,
for women and men. Manuscript under review.
especially by men in the majority women condition. Those Brown, T. A. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research.
who chose to respond may not have represented men’s and New York: Guilford Press.
women’s perceptions as a whole because participating in our Burgess, D., & Borgida, E. (1999). Who women are, who women should
be: Descriptive and prescriptive gender stereotyping in sex discrim-
survey represents some degree of expression of voice. It is
ination. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 5, 665–692. doi:10.
also possible the participants may be among the most commit- 1037/1076-8971.5.3.665.
ted to staying at the conferences. Those who have left, or are Burke, R. J., & McKeen, C. A. (1996). Do women at the top make a
considering leaving, may prefer silence and may have been difference? Gender proportions and the experiences of managerial
and professional women. Human Relations, 49, 1093–1104. doi:10.
underrepresented in our sample.
1177/001872679604900804.
An important conceptual limitation of our research project Caza, B. B., & Cortina, L. M. (2007). From insult to injury: Explaining
was that it did not address race or racism perceptions, and the impact of incivility. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 29,
therefore the study privileged Western and White cultural no- 335–350. doi:10.1080/01973530701665108.
tions of gender. We recognize that race meaningfully inter- Ceci, S. J., & Williams, W. M. (2011). Understanding current causes of
women’s underrepresentation in science. Proceedings of the
sects with gender (i.e., gendered races; Galinsky et al. 2013) National Academy of Sciences, 108, 3157–3162. doi:10.1073/
and that by considering gender differences in the absence of pnas.1014871108.
race/ethnicity, experiences of Women of Color are min- Ceci, S. J., Williams, W. M., Sumner, R. A., & DeFraine, W. C. (2011).
imized. We also recognize that racism (and heterosex- Do subtle cues about belongingness constrain women’s career
choices? Psychological Inquiry, 22(4), 255–258. doi:10.1080/
ism) can be important additional components of climate. 1047840X.2011.619112.
Future work on conference climate should examine oth- Ceci, S. J., Ginther, D. K., Kahn, S., & Williams, W. M. (2014). Women
er facets of climate and incorporate racial and sexual in academic science: A changing landscape. Psychological Science
minority participants’ perspectives. i n t h e P u b l i c I n t e re s t , 1 5 ( 3 ) , 7 5 – 1 4 1 . d o i : 1 0 . 11 7 7 /
1529100614541236.
Cheryan, S., & Plaut, V. C. (2010). Explaining underrepresentation: A
Conclusions theory of precluded interest. Sex Roles, 63, 475–488. doi:10.1007/
s11199-010-9835-x.
Our results illuminate the ways in which gender representa- Cheryan, S., Plaut, V. C., Davies, P. G., & Steele, C. M. (2009). Ambient
belonging: How stereotypical cues impact gender participation in
tions at conferences stand to impact the conference climate computer science. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
vis-à-vis sexism and the ways sexist climates are in turn relat- 97, 1045–1060. doi:10.1037/a0016239.
ed to women’s and men’s coping responses and academic Chiu, C., Hong, Y., Lam, I. C., Fu, J. H., Tong, J. Y., & Lee, V. S. (1998).
career intentions. Sexism perceptions varied by gender and Stereotyping and self-presentation: Effects of gender stereotype ac-
tivation. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 1, 81–96. doi:10.
conference type, and perceived sexism uniquely contributed 1177/1368430298011007.
to women’s career exit intentions and to men’s conference exit Cook, C., Heath, F., & Thompson, R. L. (2000). A meta-analysis of
intentions. Sexist conference climates, especially to the extent response rates in web- or internet-based surveys. Educational and
they trigger silencing in women, may play an important role in Psychological Measurement, 60, 821–836. doi:10.1177/
00131640021970934.
women’s intention to leave academic careers. Further work is Cortina, L. M. (2008). Unseen injustice: Incivility as modern discrimina-
needed to delineate this effect and to develop effective mech- tion in organizations. The Academy of Management Review: Special
anisms for change. Topic Forum on Stigma and Stigmatization, 33, 55–75. doi:10.2307/
20159376.
Cortina, L. M., Magley, V. J., Williams, J. H., & Langhout, R. D. (2001).
Acknowledgements The present manuscript is based on data from the Incivility in the workplace: Incidence and impact. Journal of
first author’s doctoral dissertation. Occupational Health Psychology, 6, 64–80. doi:10.1037/1076-
8998.6.1.64.
Crosby, F. J., Iyer, A., Clayton, S., & Downing, R. A. (2003). Affirmative
References action: Psychological data and the policy debates. American
Psychologist, 58, 93–115. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.58.2.93.
Dasgupta, N. (2011). Ingroup experts and peers as social vaccines who
Barreto, M., & Ellemers, N. (2005). The perils of political correctness: inoculate the self-concept: The stereotype inoculation model.
Men’s and women’s responses to old-fashioned and modern sexist Psychological Inquiry, 22(4), 231–246. doi:10.1080/1047840X.
views. Social Psychology Quarterly, 68, 75–88. doi:10.1177/ 2011.607313.
019027250506800106. Deaux, K., & Major, B. (1987). Putting gender into context: An integra-
Becker, J. C., & Swim, J. K. (2012). Reducing endorsement of benevolent tive model of gender-related behavior. Psychological Review, 94,
and modern sexist beliefs: Differential effects of addressing harm 369–398. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.94.3.369.
Sex Roles (2018) 78:394–408 407

Drury, B. J., Siy, J. O., & Cheryan, S. (2011). When do female role Kanter, R. M. (1977). Men and women of the corporation. New York:
models benefit women? The importance of differentiating recruit- Basic Books.
ment from retention in STEM. Psychological Inquiry, 22(4), 265– Katila, S., & Meriläinen, S. (1999). A serious researcher or just another
269. doi:10.1080/1047840X.2011.620935. nice girl? Doing gender in a male-dominated scientific community.
Eagly, A. H., & Carli, L. L. (2007). Through the labyrinth: The truth Gender, Work and Organization, 6, 163–173. doi:10.1111/1468-
about how women become leaders. Boston: Harvard Business 0432.00079.
School Press. Kehn, A., & Ruthig, J. C. (2013). Perceptions of gender discrimination
Eagly, A. H., & Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice across six decades: The moderating roles of gender and age. Sex
toward female leaders. Psychological Review, 109, 573–598. doi:10. Roles, 69(5–6), 289–296. doi:10.1007/s11199-013-0303-2.
1037/0033-295X.109.3.573. Knights, D., & Richards, W. (2003). Sex discrimination in UK academia.
Egri, C. P. (1992). Academic conferences as ceremonials: Opportunities Gender, Work & Organization, 10, 213–238. doi:10.1111/1468-
for organizational integration and socialization. Journal of 0432.t01-1-00012.
M a n a g e m e n t E d u c a t i o n , 1 6 , 9 0 – 11 5 . d o i : 1 0 . 11 7 7 / Kriwy, P., Gross, C., & Gottburgsen, A. (2013). Look who’s talking:
105256299201600107. Compositional effects of gender and status on verbal contributions
Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied missing data analysis. New York: Guilford at sociology conferences. Gender, Work and Organization, 20(5),
Press. 545–560. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0432.2012.00603.x.
Farrell, D. (1983). Exit, voice, loyalty, and neglect as responses to job Levine, J. M., & Moreland, R. L. (2002). Group reactions to loyalty and
dissatisfaction: A multidimensional scaling study. Academy of disloyalty. In S. Thye & E. Lawler (Eds.), Group cohesion, trust,
Management Journal, 26, 496–607. doi:10.2307/255909. and solidarity (pp. 203–228). New York: Elsevier Science.
Galinsky, A. D., Hall, E. V., & Cuddy, A. J. (2013). Gendered races: Lim, S., Cortina, L. M., & Magley, V. J. (2008). Personal and workgroup
Implications for interracial marriage, leadership selection, and ath- incivility: Impact on work and health outcomes. Journal of Applied
letic participation. Psychological Science, 24(4), 498–506. doi:10. Psychology, 93, 95–107. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.93.1.95.
1177/0956797612457783. Lun, J., Sinclair, S., & Cogburn, C. (2009). Cultural stereotypes and the
Garcia, D. M., Reser, A. H., Amo, R. B., Redersdorff, S., & Branscombe, self: A closer examination of implicit self-stereotyping. Basic and
N. R. (2005). Perceivers’ responses to in-group and out-group mem- Applied Social Psychology, 31, 117–127. doi:10.1080/
bers who blame a negative outcome on discrimination. Personality 01973530902880340.
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 769–780. doi:10.1177/ Major, B., Gramzow, R. H., McCoy, S. K., Levin, S., Schmader, T., &
0146167204271584. Sidanius, J. (2002). Perceiving personal discrimination: The role of
group status and legitimizing ideology. Journal of Personality and
Gherardi, S. (1994). The gender we think, the gender we do in our ev-
Social Psychology, 82, 269–282. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.3.269.
eryday organizational lives. Human Relations, 47, 591–611. doi:10.
Monroe, K. R., & Chiu, W. F. (2010). Gender equality in the academy:
1177/001872679404700602.
The pipeline problem. Political Science & Politics, 43, 303–308.
Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and
doi:10.1017/S104909651000017X.
women's development. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Moss-Racusin, C. A., Phelan, J. E., & Rudman, L. A. (2010). When men
Good, C. D., Rattan, A., & Dweck, C. S. (2012). Why do women opt out?
break the gender rules: Status incongruity and backlash against mod-
Sense of belonging and women’s representation in mathematics.
est men. Psychology of Men and Masculinity, 11, 140–151. doi:10.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 700–717. doi:
1037/a0018093.
10.1037/a002665.
National Science Foundation, Division of Science Resources Statistics.
Goulden, M., Mason, M. A., & Frasch, K. (2011). Keeping women in the (2010). Survey of doctorate recipients. Retrieved from http://www.
science pipeline. Annals of the American Academy of Political and nsf.gov/statistics.
Social Science, 638(1), 141–162. doi:10.1177/0002716211416925. Powell, A., Bagilhole, B., & Dainty, A. (2009). How women engineers do
Hall, R. M., & Sandler, B. R. (1982). The classroom climate: A chilly one and undo gender: Consequences for gender equality. Gender, Work
for women? The project on the status and education of women. and Organization, 16, 411–428. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0432.2008.
Washington, DC: The Association of American Colleges http:// 00406.x.
eric.ed.gov/PDFS/ED215628.pdf. Priola, V. (2007). Being female and doing gender: Narratives of women in
Heilman, M. E. (2001). Description and prescription: How gender stereo- education management. Gender and Education, 19, 21–40. doi:10.
types prevent women’s ascent up the organizational ladder. Journal 1080/09540250601087728.
of Social Issues, 57, 657–674. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.00234. Riger, S., Stokes, J., Raja, S., & Sullivan, M. (1997). Measuring percep-
Heilman, M. E., Wallen, A. S., Fuchs, D., & Tamkins, M. M. (2004). tions of the work environment for female faculty. The Review of
Penalties for success: Reactions to women who succeed at male Higher Education, 21, 63–78. doi:10.1353/rhe.1997.0015.
gender-typed tasks. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 416–427. Riley, S., Frith, H., Archer, L., & Veseley, L. (2006). Institutional sexism
doi:10.1037/0021-9010.89.3.416. in academia. The Psychologist, 19, 94–100.
Hirschman, A. O. (1970). Exit, voice, and loyalty: Responses to decline in Rowe, M. P. (1990). Barriers to equality: The power of subtle discrimi-
firms, organizations, and states. Cambridge: Harvard University nation to maintain unequal opportunity. Employee Responsibilities
Press. and Rights Journal, 3, 153–163. doi:10.1007/BF01388340.
Hogg, M. A., & Turner, J. C. (1987). Intergroup behaviour, self- Rudman, L. A. (1998). Self-promotion as a risk factor for women: The
stereotyping and the salience of social categories. British Journal costs and benefits of counterstereotypical impression management.
of Social Psychology, 26, 325–340. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8309.1987. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 629–645. doi:10.
tb00795.x. 1037/0022-3514.74.3.629.
Janz, T. A., & Pyke, S. W. (2000). A scale to assess student perceptions of Ruthig, J. C., Kehn, A., Gamblin, B. W., Vanderzanden, K., & Jones, K.
academic climates. Canadian Journal of Higher Education, 30, 89– (2017). When women’s gains equal men’s losses: Predicting a zero-
122. sum perspective of gender status. Sex Roles, 76, 17–26. doi:10.1007/
Kaiser, C. R., & Miller, C. T. (2001). Stop complaining! The social costs s11199-016-0651-9.
of making attributions to discrimination. Personality and Social Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2004). Autonomy is no illusion: Self-
P s y c h o l o g y B u l l e t i n , 2 7 ( 2 ) , 2 5 4 – 2 6 3 . d o i : 1 0 . 11 7 7 / determination theory and the empirical study of authenticity, aware-
0146167201272010. ness, and will. In J. Greenberg, S. L. Koole, & T. Pyszczynski (Eds.),
408 Sex Roles (2018) 78:394–408

Handbook of experimental existential psychology (pp. 449–479). Swim, J. K., & Hyers, L. L. (1999). Excuse me—What did you just say?
New York: Guilford Press. Women’s public and private responses to sexist remarks. Journal of
Sam, D. L., & Berry, J. W. (2010). Acculturation: When individuals and Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 68–88. doi:10.1006/jesp.1998.
groups of different cultural backgrounds meet. Perspectives on 1370.
P s y c h o l o g i c a l S c i e n c e , 5 , 4 7 2– 4 8 1. d oi :1 0 . 11 7 7/ Swim, J. K., & Hyers, L. L. (2009). Sexism. In T. Nelson (Ed.),
1745691610373075. Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination (pp. 407–
Schmitt, M. T., Ellemers, N., & Branscombe, N. R. (2003). Perceiving 430). New York: Psychology Press.
and responding to gender discrimination in organizations. In S. A. Swim, J. K., Aikin, K. J., Hall, W. S., & Hunter, B. A. (1995). Sexism and
Haslam, D. van Knippenberg, M. J. Platow, & N. Ellemers (Eds.), racism: Old-fashioned and modern prejudices. Journal of
Social identity at work: Developing theory for organizational Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 199–214. doi:10.1037/
practice (pp. 277–292). New York: Psychology Press. 0022-3514.68.2.199.
Settles, I. H., & O’Connor, R. C. (2014). Incivility at academic confer- Swim, J. K., Mallett, R., & Stangor, C. (2004). Understanding subtle
ences: Gender differences and the mediating role of climate. Sex sexism: Detection and use of sexist language. Sex Roles, 51(3–4),
Roles, 71, 71–82. doi:10.1007/s11199-014-0355-y. 117–128. doi:10.1023/B:SERS.0000037757.73192.06.
Settles, I. H., Cortina, L. M., Malley, J., & Stewart, A. J. (2006). The Swim, J. K., Eyssell, K. M., Murdoch, E. Q., & Ferguson, M. J. (2010).
climate for women in academic science: The good, the bad, and the Self-silencing to sexism. Journal of Social Issues, 66, 493–507. doi:
changeable. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 30, 47–58. doi:10. 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2010.01658.x.
1111/j.1471-6402.2006.00261.x. Tajfel, H. (1975). The exit of social mobility and the voice of social
Settles, I. H., Cortina, L. M., Stewart, A. J., & Malley, J. (2007). Voice change: Notes on the social psychology of intergroup relations.
matters: Buffering the impact of a negative climate for women in Social Science Information, 14, 101–118. doi:10.1177/
science. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 31, 270–281. doi:10. 053901847501400204.
1111/j.1471-6402.2007.00370.x.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup
Settles, I. H., Cortina, L. M., Buchanan, N. T., & Miner, K. N. (2013).
behavior. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), The psychology of
Derogation, discrimination, and (dis)satisfaction with jobs in sci-
intergroup relations (pp. 7–24). Chicago: Nelson-Hall.
ence: A gendered analysis. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 37,
Toren, N. (1990). Would more women make a difference? In S. Stiver Lie
197–191. doi:10.1177/0361684312468727.
& V. E. O'Leary (Eds.), Storming the tower: Women in the academic
Shaw, A. K., & Stanton, D. E. (2012). Leaks in the pipeline: Separating
world (pp. 74–85). London: Kogan.
demographic inertia from ongoing gender differences in academia.
Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 279, 3736–3741. doi:10.1098/ Tsui, A. S., & Gutek, B. A. (1999). Demographic differences in organi-
rspb.2012.0822. zations: Current research and future directions. Lanham: Lexington
Sheltzer, J. M., & Smith, J. C. (2014). Elite male faculty in the life Books.
sciences employ fewer women. PNAS Proceedings of the National van Anders, S. M. (2004). Why the academic pipeline leaks: Fewer men
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(28), than women perceive barriers to becoming professors. Sex Roles, 5,
10107–10112. doi:10.1073/pnas.1403334111. 511–521. doi:10.1007/s11199-004-5461-9.
Stangor, C., Swim, J. K., Van Allen, K. L., & Sechrist, G. B. (2002). Walkerdine, V. (1989). Femininity as performance. Oxford Review of
Reporting discrimination in public and private contexts. Journal of Education, 15, 267–279. doi:10.1080/0305498890150307.
Personality and Social Psychology, 82(1), 69–74. doi:10.1037/ Wilson, J. Z., Marks, G., Noone, L., & Hamilton-McKenzie, J. (2010).
0022-3514.82.1.69. Retaining a foothold on the slippery paths of academia: University
Steffens, M. C., Jelenec, P., & Noack, P. (2010). On the leaky math women, indirect discrimination, and the academic marketplace.
pipeline: Comparing implicit math-gender stereotypes and math Gender and Education, 22, 535–545. doi:10.1080/
withdrawal in female and male children and adolescents. Journal 09540250903354404.
of Educational Psychology, 102(4), 947–963. doi:10.1037/ Yoder, J. D. (1991). Rethinking tokenism: Looking beyond numbers.
a0019920. G e n d e r a n d S o c i e t y, 5 , 1 7 8 – 1 9 2 . d o i : 1 0 . 11 7 7 /
Stout, J. G., Dasgupta, N., Hunsinger, M., & McManus, M. A. (2011). 089124391005002003.
STEMing the tide: Using ingroup experts to inoculate women's self- Yoder, J. D. (1994). Looking beyond numbers: The effects of gender
concept in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics status, job prestige, and occupational gender-typing on tokenism
(STEM). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(2), processes. Social Psychology Quarterly, 57, 150–159. Retrieved
255–270. doi:10.1037/a0021385. from http://www.jstor.org/stable/2786708 .

Вам также может понравиться