Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 21

Psychological Bulletin Copyright 1992 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1992, Vol. I I 2 , No. 1,3-23 0033-2909/92/S3.00

The Dependent Personality: Developmental,


Social, and Clinical Perspectives
Robert F. Bornstein
Gettysburg College

Developmental, social, and clinical studies of dependency have produced remarkably consistent
results. A review and integration of these findings allow strong conclusions to be drawn regarding
the etiology and dynamics of dependency. The etiology of dependency appears to lie in overprotec-
tive, authoritarian parenting. In social settings, dependency is associated with suggestibility, confor-
mity, compliance, interpersonal yielding, affiliative behavior, and sensitivity to interpersonal cues.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Dependency predicts the onset of certain psychological disorders and follows the onset of others. It
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

seems that the fundamental motivation of the dependent person, from which the behaviors that are
exhibited in different situations are derived, is a strong desire to obtain and maintain nurturant,
supportive relationships. Implications of these findings for different theoretical models of depen-
dency are discussed.

A few life experiences are so widely shared by people of dif- various physical disorders (e.g., ulcers, heart disease, cancer), I
ferent backgrounds that they transcend the boundaries of cul- do not discuss in detail findings regarding the dependency-
ture, gender, and ethnicity. Death and taxes are two such univer- disease link. In addition, I do not discuss studies of depen-
sal life experiences. Dependency is another. Early in life, every dency as a social cue, because these studies do not treat depen-
one of us was completely dependent on the primary caretaker dency as an individual-difference variable. Investigations in
for protection, nurturance, and support. Of course, our pri- this area typically examine the effects of the perceived depen-
mary caretaker was once completely dependent on his or her dency of a target person on actual or self-reported helping be-
primary caretaker, who was in turn dependent on his or her havior (Baker & Reitz, 1978; Berkowitz & Daniels, 1963,1964;
primary caretaker, and so on. Given the universality of the Harris & Ho, 1984; Taylor, Messick, Lehman, & Hirsch, 1982).
dependency experience across time and culture, it is not sur- The results of these investigations are discussed later, in the
prising that psychologists have long been interested in the dy- context of findings from studies of dependency and the depen-
namics, correlates, and consequences of dependency. dent personality.
Although developmental, social, and clinical psychologists The review is divided into six sections. I first discuss theoreti-
share an interest in dependency, there has been relatively little cal models of dependency, to place the ensuing review of the
integration of the findings that have emerged in different re- empirical literature in context. Second, I provide a brief over-
search areas. Furthermore, there are no reviews available that view of widely used objective and projective dependency mea-
summarize and integrate the findings obtained in these three sures. Third, I review studies that examine the etiology and
domains of dependency research. The purpose of this article is development of dependency. Fourth, I review research on the
to review the empirical literature on dependency and the de- interpersonal consequences of dependency. Fifth, I review re-
pendent personality, with the aim of linking different research search on dependency and psychopathology. Finally, I discuss
programs in this area and arriving at a more integrated perspec- and integrate findings from developmental, social, and clinical
tive on this topic. research in order to get as complete a picture as possible of the
In reviewing the vast literature on dependency, I must neces- traits and behaviors that characterize dependency and compose
sarily be selective in the topics that I can include. Thus, I review the dependent personality in adults.
only those studies that treat dependency as an individual-dif-
ference variable. Because Greenberg and Bornstein (1988a) and Theoretical Models of Dependency
Masling and Schwartz (1979) provided extensive reviews of the
literature examining the relationship of dependency to risk for Every theory of personality implicitly or explicitly includes a
conceptual model of dependency. However, some theoretical
perspectives have been more influential than others. Two mod-
I would like to thank Michelle L. Koons, Amy B. Krukonis, Kath- els in particular have had a pervasive influence on dependency
leen A. Manning, and Susan R. Roach for help in conducting the litera- theory and research: the psychodynamic approach and the so-
ture searches for this review. I would also like to thank several anony- cial learning view.
mous reviewers for helpful comments on an earlier version of this
article. The Psychodynamic Approach
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Rob-
ert F. Bornstein, Department of Psychology, Gettysburg College, Get- In the classical psychoanalytic model, dependency is inextri-
tysburg, Pennsylvania 17325-1486 cably linked to the events of the infantile, "oral" stage of devel-
ROBERT F. BORNSTEIN

opment (Fisher & Greenberg, 1985; Masling, 1986). In this ingly influenced by ethological theories, so that the relation-
model, frustration or overgratification during the oral stage is ship of infrahuman research and ethology has become one of
hypothesized to result in oral "fixation" and in an inability to mutual influence and integration (Bowlby, 1980).
resolve the developmental issues that characterize this stage Although there is some overlap between the concepts of de-
(i.e., conflicts regarding dependence-independence). Freud pendency and attachment, recent research makes clear that
(1938/1953) was explicit in linking later personality develop- these constructs are not one and the same (see, e.g., Livesley,
ment to the feeding experience during infancy, arguing that "a Schroeder, & Jackson, 1990). Clearly, dependency is an impor-
child sucking at his mother's breast becomes the prototype of tant component of certain forms of attachment behavior (e.g.,
every relation of love" (p. 222). Classical psychoanalytic theory insecure attachment), both in childhood (Waters & Deane,
postulates that the orally fixated (or "oral dependent") person 1985) and during adolescence and adulthood (Livesley et al.,
will (a) remain dependent on others for nurturance and support 1990; Sperling & Berman, 1991). However, individual differ-
and (b) continue to exhibit behaviors in adulthood that reflect ences in attachment and dependency in children have different
the oral stage of development (i.e., preoccupation with activities antecedents (Sroufe, Fox, & Pancake, 1983) and correlates
of the mouth, reliance on food and eating as a means of coping (Ainsworth, 1969; Maccoby, 1980) and predict different aspects
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

with anxiety). of adult behavior (Kaul, Mathur, & Murlidharan, 1982; Livesley
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Two theoretical models of dependency evolved from the clas- et al., 1990). Furthermore, the behaviors that characterize de-
sical psychoanalytic model: object relations theory and ethologi- pendency and insecure attachment show only moderate over-
cal (i.e., attachment) theory. Although object relations theory lap, both conceptually (Ainsworth, 1969, 1972; Waters &
and attachment theory differ in some important ways, these Deane, 1985) and empirically (Sears, Rau, & Alpert, 1965;
two approaches also have much in common. Both models Sroufe et al., 1983; West, Livesley, Reiffer, & Sheldon, 1986).
deemphasize the importance of actual oral activities (e.g., In this context, it is not surprising that the operational defini-
breastfeeding) in determining the development of oral depen- tions of attachment and dependency used by developmental
dent traits and behaviors (J. R. Greenberg & Mitchell, 1983). and clinical researchers differ substantially (see Sears, 1972;
Instead, these models hypothesize that the overall quality of the West et al., 1986). As Livesley et al. (1990) note, attachment
infant-caretaker relationship during infancy and early child- behavior typically refers to
hood is the primary determinant of dependent traits in adult-
any form of behavior that results in a person attaining or retaining
hood (Ainsworth, 1969). In object relations theory and attach- proximity to some preferred individual, who is usually conceived
ment theory, the infant's feeding and weaning experiences do as stronger and/or wiser. Dependency behaviors, in contrast, are
not play the central role in determining later personality devel- not directed toward a specific individual nor are they concerned
opment. Rather, early feeding experiences are viewed as one with promoting the feelings of security that arise from proximity
to attachment figures. Instead, they are more generalized behav-
aspect, albeit an important one, of the overall infant-caretaker iors designed to elicit assistance, guidance and approval [from
relationship. others], (p. 132)
The object relations model of dependency extends the classi-
cal psychoanalytic model primarily by emphasizing separa- Along similar lines, Ainsworth (1972) argued that
tion-individuation and the development of the self-concept as attachment is an affectional tie or bond that one individual (per-
critical developmental tasks that occur during infancy and early son or animal) forms between himself and another specific indi-
childhood. The most important implication of this conceptual vidual. In contrast, dependency is a generalized or nonfocused
shift is that the infant-caretaker relationship is no longer response characteristic (p. 100).
viewed as being rooted exclusively in biological gratification Thus, Ainsworth (1972), Livesley et al. (1990), and others
provided by the caretaker to the infant. Rather, object relations suggest that dependency and attachment differ in two impor-
theorists conceptualize the early infant-caretaker relationship tant respects. First, attachment behavior is manifested primar-
as a prototype for later interpersonal relationships. Thus, in the ily by proximity seeking, whereas dependent behavior is mani-
object relations model, self and object representations that are fested primarily by help seeking. Second, attachment behaviors
internalized (or introjected) during infancy and early childhood are object specific and are consistently directed toward the
are hypothesized to play a central role in personality develop- same person. In contrast, dependent behaviors may be directed
ment and dynamics (Bornstein, Galley, & Leone, 1986). toward any number of people who represent, in the eyes of the
The ethological approach differs from the object relations dependent person, potential nurturers, protectors, or care-
model primarily in its emphasis on the innate, biological un- takers.1
derpinnings of infant-mother bonding as a determinant of the
self-concept and subsequent interpersonal behavior (Bowlby, 1
1969, 1973, 1980). In contrast to object relations theory, the 1 do not mean to imply by this discussion that dependency and
ethological perspective combines concepts from psychoanaly- attachment are completely unrelated. Clearly, children who show an
sis and evolutionary theory to account for individual differ- insecure attachment style also tend to show exaggerated dependent
behaviors (Sroufe, Fox, & Pancake, 1983). Moreover, certain qualities
ences in the infant-caretaker relationship and to understand of the infant-caretaker relationship that result in the development of
the effects of these differences on later personality develop- an insecure attachment style also produce high levels of dependency
ment. Not surprisingly, infrahuman research on attachment during middle and late childhood (see Ainsworth, 1972). Without ques-
and bonding (e.g., Harlow, 1958) was initially an important im- tion, the relationship between dependency and attachment is complex
petus behind the development of the ethological approach. and warrants continued attention from developmental researchers.
However, infrahuman attachment research has been increas- Nonetheless, I believe that, consistent with the suggestions of Ains-
THE DEPENDENT PERSONALITY

The Social Learning View and support, and may lead to increased risk for certain forms of
depression (Abramson et al., 1978).
Social learning theorists initially regarded dependency as an
acquired drive, the impetus for which was the reduction of pri-
mary drives (e.g., hunger) through the provision of primary The Psychoanalytic and Social Learning Models of
reinforcers (e.g., food) within the context of the infantile depen- Dependency: A Comparison
dency relationship (Walters & Parke, 1964). Later, social learn- At first glance, the psychoanalytic and social learning mod-
ing theorists recognized the importance of social reinforce- els of dependency appear to have little in common. Nothing
ment provided by caretakers for the development of childhood could be further from the truth. To be sure, there is at least one
and adult dependency (Ainsworth, 1969; Maccoby, 1980). Thus, fundamental difference between these two theoretical frame-
the social learning model hypothesizes that as the primary care- works: Psychoanalytic theory hypothesizes that the dependent
taker provides biological and psychological gratification to the person will be preoccupied with oral activities (e.g., eating,
infant (through feeding, providing warmth and contact com- drinking, smoking), whereas the social learning model makes
fort, etc.), she comes to be associated with pleasurable experi- no prediction regarding a link between dependency and these
ences and in effect becomes a kind of secondary reinforcer. To
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

behaviors. However, aside from this difference, there are some


the extent that the infant's beliefs and expectations regarding
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

important similarities between the two perspectives. Although


the caretaker's nurturing behavior generalize to other potential psychoanalysis and social learning theory use different lan-
caretakers (e.g., teachers, supervisors, romantic partners), de- guage to describe certain ideas, careful examination of these
pendent behavior will continue to be exhibited in these rela- ideas reveals that they actually have much in common.
tionships as well. Ainsworth (1969) provided a succinct sum- For example, both the psychoanalytic and social learning
mary of this view, noting that to learning theorists, dependency models predict that people will be conflicted (i.e., strongly am-
is regarded as "a class of behaviors, learned in the context of the bivalent) regarding underlying dependency needs. However,
infant's dependency relationship with his mother. . . . Al- the roots of this conflict differ in the two models. In psycho-
though the first dependency relationship is a specific one, de- analysis, infantile dependency needs are hypothesized to be
pendency is viewed as generalizing to subsequent interpersonal unconscious. Thus, dependency conflict within the psychoan-
relationships" (p. 970). alytic model is conceptualized in terms of a struggle between
Although some social learning theories emphasize the im- unconscious dependency needs and conscious prohibitions
portance of modeling as well as reinforcement in the develop- against expressing these needs. In social learning theory, de-
ment of dependent behaviors (e.g., Bandura, 1977), social learn- pendency conflict arises from inconsistent socialization prac-
ing theories have in common the fundamental hypothesis that tices that almost inevitably occur during early and middle
people learn to be dependent. Implicit in the social learning childhood. On the one hand, children are taught to obey fig-
perspective is the assumption that dependent behaviors are ex- ures of authority (e.g., parents, teachers) and to depend on fig-
hibited because they are rewarded, were rewarded, or—at the ures of authority for nurturance, protection, and guidance. On
very least—are perceived by the dependent person as likely to the other hand, children are taught to be creative, autonomous,
bring rewards. In short, this model hypothesizes that individual and independent in work and in play. Conflict is created as the
differences in childhood and adult dependency result from vari- child attempts to resolve these inconsistent messages and to
ations in the degree to which passive, dependent behavior was determine which type of behavior—dependent or independent
reinforced by the primary caretaker during infancy and early —is appropriate in a particular situation.
childhood. The most important similarity between the psychoanalytic
In the past two decades, there has been an increasing empha- and social learning models of dependency involves the role of
sis on the role of cognitive processes (rather than conditioned dependency-related cognitions in the development and expres-
responses) as mediators of passive and dependent traits and
sion of dependent behaviors. In both models, cognitive pro-
behaviors (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978; Mischel, cesses are regarded as a central determinant of the behaviors
1973; Mischel & Peake, 1982). As the emphasis in social learn- that are exhibited by dependent people in different contexts
ing theory has shifted from conditioned responses to cognitive
and settings. Not surprisingly, however, dependency-related
processes, dependency has become increasingly linked with
cognitions are conceptualized differently in the psychoanalytic
depression. Thus, social learning theorists now view depen-
and social learning models. In psychoanalytic theory, mental
dency primarily in terms of a cognitive (or attributional) style in representations of the parents and other significant figures are
which a person perceives him- or herself as powerless, helpless,
hypothesized to play a key role in determining the degree to
and unable to influence the outcome of events in a positive way.
which a person experiences (and expresses) strong dependency
Distortions in the processing of information regarding causes
needs. In social learning theory, beliefs and expectations re-
(and effects) of positive and negative events propagate the de-
garding rewards and punishments associated with expressing
pendent person's belief in his or her own ineffectiveness, rein-
(or not expressing) dependency needs are regarded as a central
force the idea that he or she needs to rely on others for guidance
determinant of a person's dependency-related behaviors.
Clearly, mental representations are not the same thing as be-
liefs and expectations. However, internalized mental represen-
worth (1972), Sears (1972), and others, attachment and dependency are tations and beliefs regarding acceptable interpersonal behav-
best conceptualized as distinct and different—albeit related—con- iors have several important features in common. Mental repre-
cepts. sentations and beliefs and expectations are, above all, cognitive
ROBERT F. BORNSTEIN

structures. Moreover, both involve the construction of abstract either dependent or oral dependent content. Because subjects
categories representing conceptually and functionally similar are not aware of what information the experimenter plans to
stimuli and events. In addition, both require an ability to apply extract from their responses, projective dependency measures
(i.e., generalize) past experiences to novel situations. have low face validity and are relatively immune from self-re-
The fact that cognitive processes play a central role in the port and self-presentation biases. Examples of projective inter-
dynamics of dependency in both the psychoanalytic and social personal dependency measures include certain TAT-derived
learning models turns out to be very important. Although both dependency scales (e.g., Kagan & Mussen, 1956). Masling et al.'s
the psychoanalytic and social learning models have received (1967) ROD is an example of a projective oral dependency mea-
some support from empirical studies of the dependent person- sure.
ality, the ensuing review of the literature will show that depen- A detailed discussion of the construct validity of these
dency-related cognitions are the key to understanding the di- various dependency measures is beyond the scope of this arti-
verse behaviors that are exhibited by dependent people in cle. Fisher and Greenberg (1985), Masling (1986), and Masling
various situations and settings. and Schwartz (1979) provide extensive information in this area.
However, three issues regarding the construct validity of these
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Assessing Dependency dependency measures are worth noting in the present context.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

First, scores on each of the dependency scales discussed in


Because the concept of dependency is of interest to re- detail in this review (i.e., the ROD, DP Scale, MMPI Dy scale,
searchers in a wide variety of areas, numerous measures of DEQ Dependency scale, MID, Blacky Test, and LK Oral De-
dependency have been developed. Among the more widely pendency scale) have been shown to correlate positively with
used measures are Navran's (1954) Minnesota Multiphasic Per- some direct measure of dependent, help-seeking behavior (see
sonality Inventory (MMPI) Dependency (Dy) scale; Masling, Fisher & Greenberg, 1985; Masling & Schwartz, 1979, for re-
Rabie, and Blondheim's (1967) Rorschach Oral Dependency views of these studies).
(ROD) scale; Sinha's (1968) Dependence Proneness (DP) Scale; Second, scores on different dependency scales typically
the Dependency subscale of Blatt, DAfflitti, and Quinlan's show at least moderate positive intercorrelations (see Bornstein,
(1976) Depressive Experiences Questionnaire (DEQ); a factor- Galley, & Leone, 1986; Bornstein, Poynton, & Masling, 1985;
analytically derived Measure of Interpersonal Dependency Fitzgerald, 1958; Hirschfeld et al., 1989; Kagan & Moss, 1960;
(MID; Hirschfeld et al, 1977); and the Blacky Test Oral Depen- Klein, 1989; Masling, 1986; Sinha, 1968; Zuckerman et al.,
dency scale (Blum, 1949). Other dependency researchers have 1961).
used combinations of subscales from Edwards's (1959) Personal Finally, as the ensuing review will demonstrate, studies using
Preference Schedule (EPPS), the Lazare-Klerman (LK) Trait different measures of dependency have produced consistent
Scale (Lazare, Klerman, & Armor, 1966,1970), the Holtzman findings. Specifically, in studies of parenting style, suggestibil-
Inkblot Technique (HIT) Dependency Scale (S. Fisher, 1970), ity, interpersonal compliance, help seeking, interpersonal sensi-
and TAT-derived dependency measures (Kagan & Mussen, tivity, affiliative behavior, performance anxiety, depression,
1956). Finally, a few investigators have used behavioral mea- alcoholism, smoking, and eating disorders, different types of
sures of dependency (Stein & Sanfilipo, 1985), peer nomination dependency measures (i.e., objective vs. projective, oral depen-
techniques (Fitzgerald, 1958), interview ratings (Zuckerman, dency scales vs. measures of interpersonal dependency) yielded
Levitt, & Lubin, 1961), or self-report measures designed for use highly similar results.
in a single study (Tesser & Blusiewicz, 1987).
Dependency measures can be classified along two dimen- Development of Dependency
sions: content (i.e., measures of interpersonal dependency vs.
measures of oral dependency) and format (i.e., objective vs. pro- In the following sections, I discuss research on (a) the epigen-
jective). Thus, there are four general types of dependency esis of dependency in infancy and early childhood, (b) the stabil-
scales. Objective interpersonal dependency scales require sub- ity of dependent personality traits from childhood through
jects to respond to direct questions regarding dependent adulthood, (c) sex differences in dependency, and (d) the rela-
thoughts, feelings, and behaviors. Typically, these measures tionship of dependency to sex role orientation.
have high face validity (i.e., are obviously tapping dependency-
related traits). Examples of objective interpersonal dependency The Epigenesis of Dependency
measures are Sinha's (1968) DP scale and Navran's (1954)
MMPI Dy scale. During the 1940s, 1950s and 1960s, numerous researchers
In contrast, objective oral dependency measures are closely investigated the relationship of infantile feeding and weaning
tied to psychoanalytic theory and therefore require subjects to experiences to later dependency. The general approach used in
respond to questions regarding preoccupation with food, eat- these studies involved obtaining retrospective reports of feed-
ing, and other oral activities in addition to questions regarding ing and weaning behaviors from the mother and assessing the
dependent thoughts, feelings, and behaviors. Lazare et al.'s relationship of various feeding and weaning variables (e.g., dura-
(1966, 1970) LK Oral Dependency scale is an example of an tion of breastfeeding, rigidity of feeding schedule, breast vs.
objective oral dependency measure. bottle feeding, severity of weaning) to some measure of child-
Finally, projective dependency measures require subjects to hood or adult dependency (e.g., Goldman-Eisler, 1951; Hein-
respond to ambiguous stimuli such as inkblots or drawings. stein, 1963; Sears, Rau, & Albert, 1965; Sears, Whiting, Nowlis,
Subjects' responses are then scored by the experimenter for & Sears, 1953; Thurston & Mussen, 1951). Results in this area
THE DEPENDENT PERSONALITY

were mixed: Some studies found a relationship between infan- Baumrind (1971), Bhogle (1983), McPartland and Epstein
tile feeding experiences and later dependency (e.g., Heinstein, (1975), Roe and Siegelman (1963), and Winder and Rau (1962)
1963), but other studies found no consistent relationship be- found that parental authoritarianism was associated with in-
tween these variables (e.g., Sears et al, 1965). However, there are creased dependency during middle and late childhood.
some methodological problems with studies of the feeding-de- Results from studies of infant-parent interactions, investiga-
pendency relationship that rely on mothers' retrospective re- tions of parenting style, and studies of parental perceptions in
ports. For example, it is not clear whether mothers are able (or adult subjects suggest that parental overprotectiveness and
willing) to provide accurate retrospective reports of infantile authoritarianism may play a significant role in determining
feeding and weaning activities. Self-presentation bias, need for level of dependency. Highly consistent results in this area were
approval, and numerous other factors potentially influence the obtained in independent samples of American (McCranie &
accuracy of these reports. Bass, 1984; Vaillant, 1980), Indian (Bhogle, 1983; Ojha & Singh,
Given the methodological limitations of studies in this area, 1988), and British subjects (Parker & Lipscombe, 1980), despite
and the mixed results obtained in these investigations, it is dif- the fact that these studies used different measures and experi-
ficult to draw strong conclusions regarding the relationship of mental methodologies. The cross-cultural and cross-method-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

infantile feeding and weaning experiences to later dependency. ological consistency of results regarding the parenting style-de-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

More consistent findings come from studies that assessed the pendency relationship attests to the robustness and generaliz-
influence of the overall infant-caretaker relationship on later ability of these findings.
dependency and examined differences in the parenting styles What is the link between parental overprotectiveness and
of parents of dependent and nondependent children (e.g., Bho- authoritarianism and the development of dependent personal-
gle, 1983; Finney, 1961; Gordon & Tegtemeyer, 1983; Hatfield, ity traits? Parental overprotectiveness and authoritarianism
Ferguson, Rau, & Alpert, 1967; Kagan & Moss, 1960; McCord, may serve simultaneously to reinforce dependent behaviors in
McCord, & Thurber, 1962; Murphy, 1962; Ojha & Singh, 1988; children of both sexes and to prevent the child from developing
Parker & Lipscombe, 1980; Roe & Siegelman, 1963; Sroufe et independent, autonomous behaviors (because the parents do
al., 1983; Winder & Rau, 1962).2 not permit the child to engage in the kinds of trial-and-error
In an early study in this area, Finney (1961) interviewed the learning that is involved in developing a sense of independence
mothers of 31 boys enrolled in a child guidance clinic, obtain- and mastery during childhood). The results of studies by Fin-
ing information regarding a variety of dimensions related to the ney (1961), Fu, Hinkle, and Hanna (1986), Fu, Hinkle,
infant-mother relationship (e.g., maternal protectiveness, rigid- Shoffner, et al. (1984), McCord et al. (1962), Sears et al. (1965),
ity of discipline, expression of affection). Clinicians' and and Sroufe et al. (1983) support this hypothesis. In each of these
teachers' ratings were used to assess level of dependency in the investigations, parental reinforcement of dependent behavior
child subjects. Significant correlations were obtained between (or punishment of independent behavior) during early child-
maternal protectiveness ratings and the child's dependency hood predicted level of dependency during later childhood
score (r = .37) and between ratings of a mother's tendency to and/or adolescence.
reinforce dependent behavior and her child's dependency score Although results in this area indicate that parental authori-
(r = .40). Similar results were reported by Gordon and Tegte- tarianism and overprotectiveness predict the development of
meyer (1983), Hatfield et al. (1967), Kagan and Moss (1960), dependent behaviors in children, it is important to note that
and Murphy (1962). dependent behaviors exhibited by the child may also serve to
Studies of parenting styles that used retrospective reports ob- encourage and reinforce overprotective, dependency-fostering
tained from parents and their (adult) children produced results behavior in the parents. A number of studies have demon-
generally consistent with those of Finney (1961) and others. For strated that dependent behaviors exhibited by children elicit
example, Ojha and Singh (1988) found that parental overpro- strong protective behaviors in both mothers and fathers (e.g.,
tectiveness (assessed via Ojha's, 1972, Parental Behavior Inven- Hunt, Browning, & Nave, 1982; Marcus, 1975, 1976). Other
tory) was associated with increased dependency (assessed via studies have demonstrated that dependent behaviors in chil-
Sinha's, 1968, DP scale) in their college-age children. Parker and dren can result in increased parental demandingness (see Mac-
Lipscombe (1980) similarly found that reports of parental over- coby, 1980). Thus, whereas the epigenesis of dependency may
protectiveness (assessed via Parker, Tupling, & Brown's, 1979, well lie in parental overprotectiveness and/or authoritarianism,
Parental Bonding Instrument) were associated with increased the relationship of child dependency and parental behavior is
dependency (assessed via Blatt et al.'s, 1976, DEQ Dependency synergistic and is characterized by mutual influence and recip-
scale) in a mixed-sex sample of adult medical patients. rocal reinforcement.
Whereas a number of studies found parental overprotective-
ness to be associated with increased dependency in children, 2
adolescents, and adults, several studies also found parental Throughout this review I use the term nondependent rather than
authoritarianism to predict later dependency. For example, independent to refer to people who obtain low scores on various depen-
Vaillant (1980) found that memories of the parents as harsh and dency measures. I chose to use this term because the majority of de-
pendency measures are structured in such a way that a low score
demanding during childhood were associated with elevated lev- merely indicates a lack of dependent thoughts, feelings and/or behav-
els of dependency in a sample of 184 male undergraduates who iors. A low score on most dependency measures does not necessarily
were first studied around age 20 and then reassessed at age 50. indicate a high degree of independence. Thus, the control subjects to
Similar results were obtained by McCranie and Bass (1984) in a whom dependent people are compared are most accurately described
sample of 86 female undergraduates. Along different lines, as nondependent, rather than independent.
ROBERT F. BORNSTEIN

Dependency, Gender, and Sex Role tudinal study of dependency illustrates the general pattern of
results obtained in most studies of this issue. Kagan and Moss
When self-report measures of dependency are used, the vast found that early childhood dependency scores were signifi-
majority of studies investigating gender differences in adult cantly better predictors of dependency scores in adulthood for
dependency have found higher levels of dependency in women women than for men.
than in men (e.g., Birtchnell & Kennard, 1983; Chevron, Quin- The causes of the increasing gender differences in depen-
lan, & Blatt, 1978; Conley, 1980; Lao, 1980; H. Ojha & Singh, dency levels with increasing age may well lie in traditional sex
1985; Singh & Ojha, 1987; Vats, 1986). Similar gender differ- role socialization practices. As Spence and Helmreich (1978)
ences in dependency are found in school-age children of note, boys are generally discouraged from expressing openly
various ages when self-report measures are used (Chadha, 1983; dependent feelings and needs, yet girls have historically been
Ederer, 1988; Golightly, Nelson, & Johnson, 1970; H. Ojha & encouraged to exhibit these feelings, because passive, depen-
Singh, 1972). Furthermore, comparable gender differences on dent behavior has traditionally been regarded as consonant
self-report dependency measures are found in American with the female (i.e., feminine) sex role. Parents, teachers, older
(Chevron et al., 1978), British (Birtchnell & Kennard, 1983), siblings, and other role models (e.g., fictional heros and her-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

German (Ederer, 1988) and Indian subjects (Singh & Ojha, oines; see White, 1986) encourage children—-either subtly or
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

1987). directly—to conform to traditional sex role expectations. Thus,


In contrast, when projective measures of dependency are although projective tests and other "subtle" measures suggest
used, researchers typically find that men and women (and boys that boys and girls (and men and women) have comparable
and girls; see Gordon & Tegtemeyer, 1983) show similar levels dependency needs, cultural factors may serve to encourage the
of dependency (Bornstein, Leone, & Galley, 1988; Greenberg& overt expression of these needs in women and discourage the
Bornstein, 1989; Juni, Masling, &Brannon, 1979; Mills & Cun- overt expression of these needs in men.
ningham, 1988; Shilkret & Masling, 1981; Weiss, 1969; Weiss & If in fact men and women have comparable underlying de-
Masling, 1970). These patterns are found in both normal (i.e., pendency needs (as studies using projective measures of depen-
nonclinical) subjects and in psychiatric inpatients and outpa- dency suggest) but the overt expression of these needs is largely
tients. In short, women report higher levels of dependency than a function of sex role socialization, then self-reported depen-
men on self-report measures, but men and women receive com- dency should vary as a function of the degree to which men and
parable dependency scores on projective measures. women adopt traditional sex roles. Specifically, to the degree
What accounts for the different patterns of results produced that a woman views herself as departing from the traditional
by self-report and projective dependency measures? The most female sex role, she should be less willing to acknowledge de-
likely explanation has to do with the face validity of the mea- pendency needs on self-report measures. Conversely, to the de-
sures. As noted earlier, self-report measures of dependency typi- gree that a man regards himself as departing from the tradi-
cally ask the subject to respond to direct questions about de- tional male sex role, he should show higher levels of depen-
pendent traits, feelings, and behaviors. Clearly, men will be less dency on self-report tests. A number of studies have addressed
willing than women to acknowledge dependent traits and feel- this issue, directly or indirectly (Anderson, 1986; Birtchnell &
ings, even if they are aware of them. However, projective mea- Kennard, 1983; Chevron et al., 1978; Ojha & Singh, 1985; Wat-
sures of dependency ask subjects for open-ended descriptions son, Biderman, & Boyd, 1989; Welkowitz, Lish, & Bond, 1985;
of ambiguous stimuli, such as inkblots or drawings. These Zuroff, Moskowitz, Wielgus, Powers, & Franko, 1983).
open-ended descriptions are then scored for dependent (or oral Birtchnell and Kennard (1983) compared the MMPI Dy
dependent) content, but of course the subject is unaware of scores of married women who reported that they and their
what content areas are of interest to the experimenter. Conse- spouse conformed to traditional sex roles (i.e., the husband was
quently, the subject will be unable to disguise or distort their viewed as the dominant marriage partner; n = 130 couples) to
projective test responses on the basis of social desirability and the Dy scores of women who reported a reversal of traditional
self-presentation needs. Thus, despite the fact that subjects' sex roles in their marriage (i.e., the wife was viewed as the domi-
scores on self-report and projective measures of dependency are nant partner; n = 46 couples). As predicted, women who re-
generally positively intercorrelated (Bornstein et al., 1985; Fitz- ported ascribing to the traditional marital role obtained signifi-
gerald, 1958; Zuckerman et al., 1961), self-report measures con- cantly higher scores on the MMPI Dy scale than did women
sistently yield substantially greater sex differences in depen- who reported a reversal of traditional sex roles in their
dency scores than do projective measures. marriage. Similar results were obtained by Ojha and Singh
It is informative to contrast the findings obtained regarding (1985) in their investigation of dependency and marriage role
gender differences in dependency in adults with the findings attitudes in Indian subjects.
obtained in studies of children. Longitudinal studies of depen- Several studies have assessed directly the relationship of de-
dency in children typically find little or no difference in boys' pendency to sex role orientation, obtaining highly consistent
and girls' dependency levels during early childhood (Maccoby results. For example, Welkowitz et al. (1985) examined the rela-
& Jacklin, 1974). However, gender differences in dependency tionship of DEQ Dependency score to sex role orientation (as-
increase with increasing age (Kagan & Moss, 1960), and by the sessed via the Bern, 1974, Sex Role Inventory [BSRI]) in a
time that children reach school age, girls almost invariably mixed-sex sample of 131 undergraduates. They found a signifi-
show significantly higher dependency levels than do boys on cant positive correlation between DEQ Dependency score and
self-report measures (Chadha, 1983; Golightly et al., 1970; BSRI Femininity score in men (r = .43), and a similar (but
Yeger & Miezitis, 1985). Kagan and Moss's (1960) classic longi- nonsignificant) relationship between dependency and feminin-
THE DEPENDENT PERSONALITY

ity in women (r = .20). BSRI Masculinity score was negatively effect was significantly greater for dependent than nondepen-
correlated with DEQ Dependency score in subjects of both dent subjects. These results suggest that dependency is asso-
sexes. Anderson (1986), Chevron et al. (1978), Watson et al. ciated with increased suggestibility in laboratory settings and
(1989), and Zuroffet al. (1983) obtained virtually identical find- that this effect is particularly pronounced when the source of
ings in mixed-sex samples of undergraduate subjects. information is a high-status person rather than a peer. Highly
The results of studies assessing the relationship of depen- similar findings were subsequently reported by Tribich and
dency to sex role orientation clearly support the hypothesis that Messer (1974) in a sample of male undergraduates.
overt expression of dependency needs in both men and women Using a very different paradigm (ratings of slogan credibil-
is a function of the degree to which they ascribe to traditional ity), subject sample (Indian college students), and measure of
sex roles. However, studies in this area do not address the ques- dependency (Sinha's 1968, DP scale), H. Ojha (1972) found that
tion of whether men and women who show "masculine" char- dependent subjects of both sexes rated a series of political slo-
acteristics actually perceive themselves as having low levels of gans as more credible and convincing when they were asso-
dependency needs or instead perceive themselves as having ciated with high-prestige individuals than when they were asso-
strong dependency needs but are unwilling to acknowledge ciated with low-prestige individuals. However, no effects of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

these needs on self-report measures. Furthermore, these stud- source prestige on slogan credibility were found for nondepen-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ies do not provide information regarding the causal relation- dent subjects in H. Ojha's (1972) sample.
ship between dependency and sex role. It is possible that depar- Dependency and yielding. Studies of dependency and inter-
ture from the traditional sex role influences the overt expres- personal yielding are generally consistent with findings regard-
sion of dependency needs in men and women. However, an ing the dependency-suggestibility relationship and provide fur-
equally tenable interpretation of these findings is that level of ther evidence supporting the hypothesis that the behavior of
dependency somehow plays a role in determining sex role char- dependent individuals is more strongly affected by high-status
acteristics in adults. A longitudinal study of the dependency- than low-status persons. Two experiments investigated the de-
sex role relationship would begin to address this issue. pendency-yielding relationship using an Asch (1956) confor-
mity paradigm. First, Kagan and Mussen (1956) found that
Dependency: Interpersonal Correlates high TAT dependency scores were associated with increased
yielding to the majority opinion in an Asch-type experiment in
Studies examining the effects of dependency on social behav- which male subjects made judgments regarding the length of
ior can be d ivided into three categories: (a) studies of suggestibil- lines, with confederate subjects providing erroneous opinions
ity, yielding, and compliance, (b) studies of help-seeking behav- before the actual subject offered his judgment. Similarly, Mas-
ior, affiliative tendencies, and sensitivity to interpersonal cues, ling, Weiss, and Rothschild (1968) found that dependent male
and (c) studies of performance anxiety and response to evalua- subjects were more likely than nondependent subjects to acqui-
tion by figures of authority. esce to the opinions of confederates when estimating the num-
ber of clicking sounds in a modified Asch-type conformity
Dependency, Suggestibility, Yielding, and Compliance experiment.
One study in this area (Bornstein, Masling, & Poynton, 1987)
Because the dependent person is hypothesized to be highly obtained the opposite pattern of results (i.e., an inverse relation-
motivated to please other people in order to obtain nurturance ship between dependency and yielding). In this experiment,
and support (Masling, 1986; Millon, 1981), dependency should dependent and nondependent male undergraduates were se-
be associated with suggestibility, interpersonal yielding, and lected according to scores on Masling et al.'s (1967) ROD scale.
compliance with others' requests, demands, and expectations. Subjects were informed that they were taking part in a study of
Each of these hypothesized relationships has been examined the decision-making process. Pairs consisting of one dependent
empirically. and one nondependent subject were constructed, and subjects
Dependency and suggestibility. One of the first studies in were asked individually to attempt to determine the gender of
this area was conducted by Jakubczak and Walters (1959), who 10 poets after receiving only brief poem excerpts. The experi-
investigated the relationship between dependency and suggest- menter then selected three poems on which the subjects had
ibility in a sample of twenty-four 9-year-old boys. Subjects were disagreed and asked the 2 subjects to discuss these poems for 10
first divided into high- and low-dependent groups on the basis min to resolve their disagreement and reach a consensus deci-
of responses to a projective dependency measure. Each subject sion regarding the gender of the three poets.
then participated in a standard autokinetic effect experiment, Bornstein et al. (1987) found that dependent subjects were
with erroneous judgments regarding the movement of a sta- significantly less likely than nondependent subjects to change
tionary light source provided by either a high-status confeder- their initial opinions: In 35 of 50 dyads (70%), the nondepen-
ate (i.e., an adult male) or a same-sex peer. Two findings dent subject yielded to the opinion of the dependent subject on
emerged from Jakubczak and Walters's study. First, there was a the majority of poems. At first, this finding seems counterin-
main effect of subject dependency on susceptibility to the auto- tuitive, and inconsistent with earlier studies of dependency and
kinetic effect: Dependent subjects were more susceptible than yielding. However, postexperiment interviews revealed that de-
nondependent subjects to the effect, regardless of the status of pendent subjects were particularly concerned with making a
the confederate. Second, subject dependency and confederate good impression on the experimenter. The majority of depen-
status interacted to predict susceptibility to the autokinetic ef- dent subjects cited this as the primary reason that they did not
fect: The influence of confederate status on susceptibility to the change their initial opinions. Because the subjects' initial opin-
10 ROBERT F. BORNSTEIN

ions were known to the experimenter (unlike an Asch-type ex- a semester in order to fulfill the introductory psychology re-
periment, in which the subject is not required to alter an initial search participation requirement. Each subject's ROD scores
"public" opinion), changing the initial opinion involved the were then compared to the week in the semester that he or she
risk that the experimenter would regard the subject in a nega- completed the required experiments. Masling et al. and Born-
tive way. stein and Masling hypothesized that dependent subjects would
Thus, when confronted with choosing between impressing a complete the research participation requirement earlier in the
figure of authority (the experimenter) or getting along with a semester than would nondependent subjects, because depen-
peer (the other subject), the dependent persons opted to stand dent subjects should be more concerned than nondependent
by their initial opinions and thereby impress the authority fig- subjects with pleasing their psychology professor by complying
ure. In this respect, Bernstein et al.'s (1987) results are consis- with the professor's request that they complete their research
tent with findings indicating that dependent people are more requirement in a timely manner. Strong support for this hy-
strongly influenced by the opinions of authority figures than pothesis was obtained. In both experiments, there was a signifi-
peers (Jakubczak & Walters, 1959; H. Ojha, 1972; Tribich & cant inverse relationship between ROD score and week in the
Messer, 1974). semester at which the research participation requirement was
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Dependency and compliance. A number of studies have in- completed.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

vestigated the relationship of dependency to compliance with The only study in this area that assessed self-reported compli-
the demands and expectations of others. For example, Agrawal ance rather than actual compliant behavior is also the only
and Rai (1988) divided a mixed-sex sample of 120 undergradu- study that failed to find the predicted dependency-compliance
ate subjects into high-, moderate-, and low-dependent groups relationship. Biaggio, Godwin, and Baldwin (1984) selected
according to scores on Sinha's (1968) DP Scale. They then asked groups of dependent and nondependent undergraduate sub-
subjects to attempt to solve a series of four puzzles as quickly as jects on the basis of scores on Navran's (1954) MMPI Dy scale.
possible. The first two puzzles were solvable, but the second They then had subjects read a series of vignettes describing
two were unsolvable. Subjects were instructed to continue work- interpersonal interactions in which one person made an unrea-
ing on the puzzles until they had solved all four; the dependent sonable demand on another person (e.g., an employee asks a
measure of compliance used in this study was the amount of co-worker to stay late at work and complete his tasks so that he
time that subjects persisted in attempting to solve the two un- can leave work early). Subjects were asked to make three 7-point
solvable puzzles. As predicted, Agrawal and Rai found a signifi- ratings for each vignette: (a) the degree to which they would
cant positive relationship between DP score and persistence in comply with the request if it was made to them, (b) the amount
the puzzle-solving task: High-DP subjects spent the longest of anger that they would feel toward the requestor, and (c) the
amount of time on the task, followed by moderate-DP subjects. amount of sympathy that they would feel for the requestor.
Low-DP subjects spent significantly less time attempting to Biaggio et al. (1984) found no differences between dependent
solve the unsolvable puzzles than did the high- or moderate-DP and nondependent subjects on any rating dimension. They at-
subjects. tributed their nonsignificant findings to the artificiality of the
Using a very different procedure, measure of compliance, design of this experiment, and it is clear—as Biaggio et al. sug-
and measure of dependency, Weiss (1969) obtained results gen- gest—that the use of self-report rather than behavioral mea-
erally consistent with those of Agrawal and Rai (1988). Weiss sures of compliance might well have played a role in producing
selected dependent and nondependent undergraduate subjects the negative results obtained in this study. However, it is also
according to scores on the Blacky Test and informed the sub- worth noting, in the context of the present review, that depen-
jects that they were participating in a study of perceptual pro- dent subjects would not have been expected to report increased
cesses in normal college students. She then presented to the compliance with the excessive demands described in Biaggio et
subjects a series of slides with different numbers of dots ran- al.'s vignettes. As studies by Bornstein et al. (1987) and others
domly placed and asked them to estimate the number of dots demonstrated, dependent subjects are more concerned with
on each slide. Half the subjects were told that "college students pleasing figures of authority than with accommodating the de-
like you typically overestimate the number of dots on these mands of peers. In the Biaggio et al. study, acquiescence to
slides," and half were told that "college students like you typi- unreasonable demands from a peer might well be interpreted
cally underestimate the number of dots on these slides." Weiss by the subject as reflecting negatively on them. For example,
found that dependent subjects' estimates were more strongly acquiescing to unreasonable demands might cause the experi-
influenced by the experimenter's "expectations" than were menter to view the subject as weak or "wishy-washy." Thus, even
nondependent subjects' estimates: Dependent subjects over- or if the dependent subjects wanted to comply more strongly than
underestimated the number of dots in accordance with the per- nondependent subjects in Biaggio et al.'s experiment (and it is
ceived expectations of the experimenter, but nondependent not at all clear from these data that this is the case), self-presen-
subjects showed relatively little compliance with the experi- tation needs may well have obviated any dependency effects
menter's expectations. that did in fact exist.
Studies by Masling, O'Neill, and Jayne (1981)and Bornstein Overall, studies of suggestibility, yielding, and compliance
and Masling (1985) provide additional support for the hypothe- produce strong, consistent results, which may be summarized
sized dependency-compliance link. In these investigations, as follows. First, dependency is associated with a general ten-
ROD scores (Masling et al., 1967) were obtained from samples dency to be influenced by the opinions of others, to yield to
of introductory psychology students who were required to par- others in interpersonal transactions, and to comply with others'
ticipate in several psychology experiments during the course of expectations and demands. However, when placed in a position
THE DEPENDENT PERSONALITY 11

in which they must choose between pleasing a peer or pleasing menter was present; the predicted pattern of results was not
a figure of authority, the dependent person will typically opt for found for female subjects with male experimenters. Shilkret
pleasing the authority figure. This pattern of results is not sur- and Masling suggested that the sexual connotations of a female
prising: Given the dependent person's concern with being nur- subject asking a male authority figure for help may have ob-
tured and supported by others, it is clear that choosing to please viated any dependency-related differences in help-seeking be-
authority figures rather than peers simply reflects the depen- havior.
dent person's attempt to maximize gain, that is, to curry favor Overall, results from studies of dependency and help-seeking
with the individual most likely to be able to offer the guidance, behavior are consistent with the hypothesis that dependent peo-
support, and protection that the dependent person seeks. ple ask for guidance and assistance more often than do nonde-
pendent people. However, the results of Shilkret and Masling's
Dependency, Help Seeking, Interpersonal Sensitivity, and (1981) study further indicate that subject and experimenter
gender may interact to determine the likelihood that a depen-
Affiliation
dent subject will ask for help in the laboratory setting. Clearly,
Because dependent people are hypothesized to feel helpless further research is needed to assess the generalizability of this
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

and in need of guidance and support from others, they should result to nonlaboratory settings. In addition, research is needed
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

show elevated rates of help-seeking behaviors in a variety of to determine whether the dependency-help seeking relation-
settings. Several studies have addressed this issue directly. Fur- ship is exhibited with peers as well as figures of authority. In all
thermore, if obtaining support from others is important to the studies conducted to date examining the dependency-help
dependent person, dependency should be associated with sensi- seeking relationship, the potential helper was an authority fig-
tivity to interpersonal cues: To the extent that the dependent ure (i.e., a teacher or experimenter), and it is not clear from these
person is able to infer accurately the attitudes, beliefs, and feel- studies whether similar behaviors would be exhibited by de-
ings of others, he or she should be better able to obtain the pendent people when peers, rather than authority figures, are
support that he or she desires. In this context, researchers have potential helpers.
examined the relationship between dependency and interper- Dependency and interpersonal sensitivity. Masling, O'Neill,
sonal sensitivity in both laboratory and field settings. Finally, and Katkin (1982) tested the hypothesis that dependent people
several studies have assessed the relationship of dependency to would be more sensitive than nondependent people to warm
various indirect measures of affiliative tendencies. versus cold treatment by a confederate. In this experiment, un-
Dependency and help seeking. Early studies of dependency dergraduate male subjects were divided into dependent and
and help seeking use male subjects and male experimenters nondependent groups on the basis of scores on Masling et al.'s
only and produced consistent results. For example, Bernardin (1967) ROD scale. They were informed that they were taking
and Jessor (1957) found that dependent male subjects exhibited part in a study of how people get to know each other. Subjects
significantly more help-seeking behavior than did nondepen- then interacted for 10 min with a male confederate who treated
dent male subjects in a laboratory problem-solving situation. them either in a warm, friendly manner or in a cold, distant
Diener (1967) and Sinha and Pandey (1972) obtained similar manner. Subjects' electrodermal responses (EDRs) were re-
results in samples of American and Indian undergraduates. corded before, during, and after the interaction period.
Similar findings were also obtained by Flanders, Anderson, Masling et al. (1982) found that dependent subjects re-
and Amidon (1961) and Sroufe et al. (1983) in mixed-sex sam- sponded differentially to warm versus cold treatment by the
ples of elementary and nursery school children. confederate: Dependent subjects' EDRs increased significantly
Only Shilkret and Masling (1981) assessed directly the effects from baseline (preinteraction) to postconversation period only
of subject and experimenter gender on the dependency-help- in the cold condition. In the warm condition, dependent sub-
seeking relationship. Shilkret and Masling asked 80 undergrad- jects' EDRs remained unchanged from baseline to postconver-
uate subjects (40 women and 40 men) to attempt to solve a series sation period. In contrast, nondependent subjects showed no
of difficult puzzles (e.g., anagrams, jigsaw puzzles), with the differential responding to warm versus cold treatment by the
caveat that they could ask the experimenter for help if they felt confederate.
that they needed assistance at any time during the experiment. An earlier study by Masling, Johnson, and Saturansky (1974)
Half the subjects were assigned a male experimenter; half were suggested that under certain conditions, dependent people are
assigned a female experimenter. Before participating in the more accurate social perceivers than are nondependent people.
puzzle-solving task, subjects had been divided into high- and This study comprised two experiments. In the first, a mixed-
low-dependent groups on the basis of scores on Masling et al.'s sex sample of undergraduates was divided into dependent and
(1967) ROD scale. The dependent measure of help-seeking be- nondependent groups on the basis of scores on Masling et al.'s
havior used in Shilkret and Masling's study was the number of (1967) ROD scale. Each subject was then paired with another
times that a subject asked the experimenter for help. subject, and the 2 members of each dyad were given 15 min to
In three of four comparisons in Shilkret and Masling's (1981) get acquainted with each other. After the conversations, sub-
experiment, the predicted positive relationship between depen- jects individually completed a questionnaire that asked about
dency and help seeking was obtained. Dependent male subjects their beliefs, attitudes, and personal characteristics. Finally,
asked for help more frequently than did nondependent male each subject was given an identical questionnaire and was asked
subjects, regardless of the gender of the experimenter. However, to complete it as they thought that their partner would. The
dependent female subjects asked for help more frequently than measure of interpersonal sensitivity in this study was the con-
nondependent female subjects only when a female experi- cordance of the subject's guesses with the partner's self-ratings.
12 ROBERT F. BORNSTE1N

Masling et al. (1967) obtained mixed results in this experiment. that—as hypothesized—dependent subjects showed greater
Dependent men were better predictors of their male partner's arousal in the alone condition than in the together condition in
responses than were nondependent men. However, no differ- this experiment, but nondependent subjects showed no differ-
ences in predictive accuracy were found for dependent versus ential responding in the two conditions.
nondependent women, nor were dependent subjects of either Using a very different procedure, subject sample, and mea-
sex more accurate predictors than nondependent subjects in sure of anxiety, Keinan and Hobfoll (1989) obtained results re-
mixed-sex dyads. markably similar to those of Masling, Price, Goldband, and
Masling et al. (1974) then conducted a more naturalistic fol- Katkin (1981). In Keinan and Hobfoll's study, 67 pregnant
low-up study to test the replicability and generalizability of Israeli women were divided into dependent and nondependent
their initial results. In the follow-up study, a similar methodol- groups on the basis of their scores on a self-report measure of
ogy was used except that Peace Corps volunteers were used interpersonal dependency. Twelve hours postdelivery, each
instead of undergraduate subjects. As in the first study, depen- woman reported whether her husband had been present in the
dent male subjects were significantly more accurate than non- delivery room and provided a rating of the amount of anxiety
dependent male subjects in their inferences regarding room- that she had experienced during her delivery. Consistent with
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

mates' attitudes, beliefs, and personal characteristics. However, Masling, Price, Goldband, and Katkin's earlier findings,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

no effect of dependency on the accuracy of female subjects' Keinan and Hobfoll found that dependent women were signifi-
inferences was found. cantly more anxious during delivery when the husband was
Juni and Semel (1982) performed a modified replication of absent than when he was present. However, nondependent
Masling et al.'s (1974) experiment. In Juni and Semel's study, women reported comparable levels of anxiety regardless of
undergraduate students participating in a psychology seminar whether or not the husband was present during the delivery.
were asked to make judgments regarding the attitudes, inter- These results offer strong support for the hypothesis that the
ests, and personal beliefs of the female seminar leader after a presence of others acts as a stress reducer in dependent people.
brief (i.e., 10-min) discussion period. In contrast to Masling et Along different lines, Juni et al. (1979) tested the hypothesis
al.'s results, Juni and Semel found that dependency predicted that dependent people's affiliative needs would result in in-
accuracy of students' descriptions of the discussion leader for creased willingness to touch another person when the experi-
female subjects only: The correlation between ROD score and mental situation was constructed to permit interpersonal
accuracy of descriptions was .44 for women and -.08 for men. touching. In this study, a mixed-sex sample of undergraduates
Thus, research to date supports the hypothesis that depen- was divided into dependent and nondependent groups based
dency is associated with interpersonal sensitivity in both chil- on ROD scores. Subjects were then asked to participate in a
dren and adults. However, two qualifications of this conclusion "coaching" task, in which they were instructed to guide a blind-
warrant mention. First, with the exception of Juni and Semel's folded subject (actually a confederate) though a maze, using
(1982) results, the dependency-interpersonal sensitivity rela- whatever techniques (e.g., verbal instructions, physical direc-
tionship in adults is generally stronger for men than for women. tion) they chose. Half the subjects were assigned a same-sex
Second, in every sample examined, the accuracy of cross- confederate; half were assigned an opposite-sex confederate.
gender predictions has been unrelated to dependency. This lat- The number of times that the subject touched the confederate
ter result is not too surprising, in light of the fact that our per- and the total amount of time that the subject spent touching the
ceptions of—and inferences regarding—opposite-sex people confederate were recorded covertly by the experimenter.
are likely to be strongly influenced by a number of factors unre- Juni et al. (1979) found a significant positive correlation be-
lated to level of dependency (e.g., degree of sexual attraction). tween ROD score and the proportion of "coaching" time spent
Dependency and affiliative behavior. Studies of dependency touching the confederate (r = .39). Dependent subjects also
and affiliative tendencies have used four types of dependent touched the confederate significantly more often than did non-
variables, none of which, unfortunately, is a direct measure of dependent subjects. There were no sex differences on either
need for affiliation. To examine the dependency-affiliation touching measure, nor were there any Sex of Subject X Sex of
relationship, Masling, Price, Goldband, and Katkin (1981) as- Confederate interactions in this experiment. Consistent with
sessed autonomic arousal in social isolation; Keinan and Hob- Juni et al.'s results, Hollender et al. (1970) found that dependent
foil (1989) assessed state anxiety during childbirth; and several undergraduate women expressed greater desire than nonde-
studies (Hollender, Luborsky, & Harvey, 1970; Juni et al., 1979; pendent women for physical contact with their romantic
Sroufe et al., 1983) assessed physical touching. partners. Similarly, Sroufe et al. (1983) found that dependent
Masling, Price, Goldband, and Katkin (1981) divided a sam- nursery school children sought physical contact with the
ple of undergraduate men into dependent and nondependent teacher more frequently than did nondependent children.
groups based on their ROD scores and then placed each subject
in a soundproof chamber for 40 min, under one of two condi- Dependency, Evaluation, and Performance Anxiety
tions: Half the subjects were alone for the 40-min period, and
the remaining subjects were seated alongside a male confeder- The results of studies of the dependency-help seeking rela-
ate who was instructed to behave in a neutral manner. The tionship raise an interesting question: How will the behavior of
subjects (and confederates) were asked to complete several cleri- dependent people be affected when they are forced to work
cal tasks (e.g., checking phone numbers from a directory) during independently and are aware that their performance is being
the experiment. Subjects' EDRs were recorded throughout the evaluated by a figure of authority? Both psychoanalytic theory
40-min period. Masling, Price, Goldband, and Katkin found (Masling, 1986) and the social learning model (Walters & Parke,
THE DEPENDENT PERSONALITY 13

1964) predict that dependency should be associated with in- reflect differences in performance anxiety or differences in
creased anxiety (hence diminished performance) on indepen- response style in dependent and nondependent subjects.
dent problem-solving and learning tasks. However, Holt's One other study is noteworthy in the present context: Juni
(1966, 1970) reviews of early research on the relationship of (1981) found that dependent subjects (selected according to
dependency to cognitive style suggested that certain forms of scores on Masling et al.'s, 1967, ROD scale) were more likely
dependency are associated with creativity and cognitive flexibil- than nondependent subjects to request feedback regarding
ity. Thus, the relationship of dependency to performance in their performance in a typical psychology experiment. This
problem-solving and structured learning tasks remains open to result is consistent with the hypothesis that dependent subjects
question. have higher levels of performance anxiety than do nondepen-
Recent studies have begun to address this issue. Three exper- dent subjects in at least one respect: Obtaining feedback regard-
iments examined directly the relationship between dependency ing their performance in a psychology experiment may be one
and performance anxiety in laboratory problem-solving situa- way for the dependent person to minimize the anxiety asso-
tions (Alam, 1986; R. S. Ojha, 1978; Sinha & Pandey, 1972), and ciated with being evaluated by a figure of authority.
three field studies investigated the relationship between depen- Dependency and academic performance. It is difficult to
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

dency and academic performance (Lao, 1980; Sansanwal, Jar- predict the relationship between dependency and academic per-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ial, & Dandel, 1982; Tesser & Blusiewicz, 1987). formance, because two of the dependency-related traits that are
Dependency and performance anxiety. Alam (1986) divided most relevant to academic performance appear to work in op-
60 undergraduate men into high- and low-dependency groups position to one another. Studies by Alam (1986), R. S. Ojha
on the basis of scores on Sinha's (1968) DP scale. He then asked (1978), and others suggest that dependency may be associated
the subjects to memorize a series of briefly presented nonsense with performance anxiety, which should hinder academic per-
syllables in preparation for a recall test. Thirty minutes later, formance. However, research on the dependency-compliance
subjects were asked to recall as many of the syllables as possible. relationship, described earlier, suggests that dependent people
Subjects were tested individually by a male experimenter. Alam are particularly concerned with pleasing figures of authority
found that as hypothesized, high-DP subjects showed signifi- (e.g, Bernstein & Masling, 1985). This should lead to increased
cantly poorer performance on the recall test than did low-DP motivation among dependent students to perform well in aca-
subjects. He concluded that performance anxiety inhibited the demic settings, in order to please teachers and parents. Given
ability of high-DP subjects to recall the previously learned mate- the interplay of these two factors, it is not surprising that re-
rial. However, because performance anxiety was not assessed search on dependency and academic performance has pro-
directly in this study (e.g., by a postexperiment interview or duced mixed results.
questionnaire), Alam's results may be due to some other factor In the first study of this issue, Lao (1980) divided a mixed-sex
or combination of factors. For example, it is possible that de- sample of 365 high school students into dependent and nonde-
pendent and nondependent subjects in Alam's experiment re- pendent groups on the basis of a self-report measure of inter-
called the same number of syllables but that dependent subjects personal dependency. She then compared the high school grade
may have used a higher criterion level for reporting syllables, in point averages (GPAs) of students in her dependent and nonde-
order to avoid making any "false-positive" (i.e., incorrect) re- pendent groups and found that dependent students had signifi-
sponses. cantly lower GPAs overall than did nondependent students.
R. S. Ojha's (1978) findings are consistent with those of Alam Unfortunately, Lao did not control for academic ability in this
(1986) but also suggest that dependent subjects may well use study. Thus, it is impossible to ascertain from these results
higher criterion levels than nondependent subjects when re- whether dependency actually predicts academic performance
sponding to test items in a typical psychology experiment. In or whether dependent and nondependent students in Lao's sam-
Ojha's experiment, undergraduate male subjects completed ple differed in academic ability. Similar methodological prob-
Sinha's (1968) DP scale and then took part in a choice reaction lems characterize Sansanwal et al.'s (1982) study of dependency
time (CRT) study in which they were asked to indicate as and academic performance in junior high school students.
quickly as possible which of two stimulus lights (red or green) A better designed study of this issue was conducted by Tesser
was illuminated. Fifteen CRT trials were administered to each and Blusiewicz (1987). They divided a mixed-sex sample of 107
subject. Ojha found a significant positive relationship between tenth graders into dependent and nondependent groups on the
DP score and mean CRT: The higher a subject's DP score, the basis of scores on a 10-item dependency questionnaire. Aca-
longer his CRT latency. Sinha and Pandey (1972) similarly ob- demic achievement was assessed by calculating a predicted
tained a significant positive correlation (r= .53) between under- GPA for each student on the basis of standardized intelligence
graduate subjects' DP scores and the amount of time that they test scores and comparing the predicted GPA to the student's
took to respond to test items during a laboratory problem-solv- actual GPA. A strong relationship between dependency and
ing task. underachievement was found: Dependent students showed sig-
Performance anxiety might account for these results, as both nificantly poorer academic performance (i.e., lower GPAs than
R. S. Ojha (1978) and Sinha and Pandey (1972) suggested. Alter- predicted on the basis of standardized test scores) than did
natively, dependent subjects' slower reaction and response nondependent students.
times might reflect a strategy wherein the dependent subject is
particularly careful and deliberate in responding in order to Dependency and Psychopathology
avoid giving incorrect answers. Clearly, additional work re- Dependency has been theoretically linked to numerous psy-
mains to be done to investigate whether findings in this area chological disorders, including depression, schizophrenia, pho-
14 ROBERT F. BORNSTEIN

bias, substance use disorders, conversion disorders, and eating sion link are possible. First, some researchers have suggested
disorders (Masling & Schwartz, 1979). The theoretical links that dependency somehow predisposes people to depression
between dependency and psychopathology are most explicit in (see O'Neill & Bornstein, 1991, for a summary of this view).
the psychoanalytic model (see S. Fisher & Greenberg, 1985), but Second, some researchers have argued that dependency is a
social learning theorists have also regarded dependency as a product of depression, following rather than preceding the on-
risk factor for certain psychological disorders (see Bandura, set of depressive symptomatology (Akiskal, Hirschfeld, & Yere-
1977; Beck, 1967). Unfortunately, despite much theoretical spec- vanian, 1983). Finally, some researchers suggest that depression
ulation, the relationship between dependency and many psy- and dependency are both products of some underlying variable
chological disorders remains largely unexamined. I focus pri- (e.g., a dysfunctional attributional style; see Abramson et al.,
marily on those disorders that have received substantial atten- 1978).
tion from researchers. Thus, in the following sections, I review Although research has demonstrated that dependency levels
the empirical literature examining the relationship between de- of formerly depressed people remain somewhat elevated even
pendency and (a) depression, (b) alcohol abuse and dependence, after depressive symptomatology is no longer present (Hirsch-
(c) tobacco dependence, and (d) obesity and eating disorders. feld, Klerman, Andreason, Clayton, & Keller, 1986; Hirsch-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

feld, Klerman, Clayton, & Keller, 1983; Wittenborn & Maurer,


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Dependency and Depression 1977), experimental and quasi-experimental studies also indi-
cate that dependency levels and depression levels covary in nor-
Dozens of correlational studies have assessed the relation- mal and clinical subjects. Specifically, research has demon-
ship between level of dependency and presence of (or level of) strated that changes in level of depression are associated with
depression (e.g., Blatt et al., 1976; Blatt, Quinlan, Chevron, changes in dependent thoughts, feelings, and behaviors
McDonald, & Zuroff, 1982; Brown & Silberschatz, 1989; (Hirschfeld, Klerman, Andreason, Clayton, & Keller, 1986;
Chevron et al., 1978; Hirschfeld, Klerman, Clayton & Keller, Hirschfeld, Klerman, Clayton, & Keller, 1983; Klein et al.,
1983; Hirschfeld, Klerman, Gough, Barrett, Korchin, & Cho- 1988; Rossman, 1988). Conversely, manipulation of underlying
doff, 1977; Hirschfeld et al., 1989; Klein, 1989; Klein, Harding, dependency needs produces changes in depression levels (Zur-
Taylor, & Dickstein, 1988; O'Neill & Bornstein, 1991; Pilkonis, off & Mongrain, 1987).
1988; Pilowski, 1979; Pilowsky & Katsikitis, 1983; Robins, Klein et al. (1988) conducted a longitudinal study of the de-
1990; Robins & Block, 1988; Robins, Block, & Peselow, 1989; pendency-depression relationship in a sample of 78 female
Smith, O'Keeffe, & Jenkins, 1988; Zuroff & Mongrain, 1987). subjects. DEQ Dependency scores and clinical depression rat-
The vast majority of these investigations found a significant, ings were collected from subjects at the start of the study and
positive relationship between level of dependency and level (or again 6 months later. Klein et al. found that, as predicted, de-
incidence) of depression. Furthermore, the dependency-de- pressed subjects showed significantly higher dependency scores
pression relationship is found both in nonclinical subjects than did control subjects. However, depressed subjects who
(Bornstein & Johnson, 1990; Robins, 1990) and in members of showed a remission of depressive symptoms at 6-month follow-
clinical (i.e., psychiatric inpatient and outpatient) populations up also showed a significant decrease in DEQ Dependency
(Hirschfeld et al., 1977; Klein et al, 1988). However, the magni- scores from Time 1 to Time 2. In contrast, depressed subjects
tude of the dependency-depression relationship is generally who showed little or no change in depression level from Time 1
somewhat higher in men than in women (Blatt et al., 1976; to Time 2 also showed no change in dependency scores during
Klein, 1989; O'Neill & Bornstein, 1991). course of the study. Virtually identical findings were obtained
Information regarding the magnitude of the dependency-de- by Hirschfeld et al. (1983). Similarly, Stein and Sanfilipo (1985)
pression relationship was provided by Nietzel and Harris found that changes in depression level predicted changes in
(1990), who conducted a meta-analysis of studies assessing the overt dependent behaviors in a mixed-sex sample of nonclinical
co-occurrence of dependency and depression in various subject subjects, and Rossman (1988) found that changes in depression
groups. Mean dependency-depression score correlations in level were associated with changes in dependency scores in a
Nietzel and Harris's study sample ranged from .19 (for studies sample of Austrian army recruits.
using the Zung Depression Scale [Zung, 1965] as a measure of Along different lines, Zuroff and Mongrain (1987) demon-
depression) to .33 (for studies using the Beck Depression Inven- strated that manipulating underlying dependency needs pro-
tory [BDI; Beck, 1967]). A few studies obtained somewhat duces predictable changes in depression levels. In this study,
higher dependency-depression correlations than those re- dependent and nondependent female undergraduates were se-
ported by Nietzel and Harris (e.g., Brown & Silberschatz, 1989, lected according to scores on the DEQ Dependency scale. Sub-
obtained a correlation of .53 between DEQ Dependency scores jects then listened to one of two audiotaped interpersonal in-
and BDI scores in members of a psychiatric outpatient sample; teractions. In one interaction, which was intended to be particu-
O'Neill & Bornstein, 1991, found a correlation of .50 between larly upsetting to the dependent subjects, a woman was rejected
ROD scores and MM PI Depression scores in a sample of male by her boyfriend (the rejection audiotape). In the other interac-
psychiatric inpatients). However, overall, the association be- tion, which was designed to be upsetting to self-critical rather
tween dependency and depression is best described as being in than dependent subjects, a woman's graduate school applica-
the small-to-moderate range (Rosenthal, 1984). Dependency tion was turned down (the failure audiotape). After exposure to
scores typically account for 10-20% of the variance in depres- the audiotapes, subjects completed several measures of state
sion scores. depression. Zuroff and Mongrain found that dependent sub-
Three interpretations of the observed dependency-depres- jects showed significantly higher depression levels after expo-
THE DEPENDENT PERSONALITY 15

sure to the rejection audiotape than after exposure to the failure Dependency and Alcoholism
audiotape. Furthermore, dependent subjects showed signifi-
Several lines of evidence suggest that dependency might be
cantly higher depression levels than did nondependent subjects
associated with alcohol abuse and dependence. Classical psy-
following exposure to the rejection audiotape. As predicted,
choanalytic theory postulates a dependency-alcoholism link in
dependent subjects did not show increased depression relative
that dependent persons are hypothesized to rely on oral activi-
to nondependent subjects after exposure to the failure audio-
ties (e.g., drinking, eating, smoking) to cope with anxiety (Ber-
tape. trand & Masling, 1969). Consistent with this hypothesis, de-
ZurofFand Mongrain's (1987) results suggest that only cer- pendent and alcoholic individuals obtain similar scores on a
tain types of stressful events (e.g., those related to interpersonal variety of personality measures (e.g., depression, field depen-
rejection) produce increased depression in dependent people. dence, various defense mechanism scales). Furthermore, de-
Apparently, whereas dependency in and of itself may not pre- pendent people obtain low scores on measures of ego strength
dict the onset of depressive illness (Hirschfeld et al., 1989), de- (Nacev, 1980), self-esteem (Ederer, 1988), and perceived self-ef-
pendency acts as a diathesis that—when coupled with events in ficacy (Bornstein et al., 1988), all of which are associated with
the environment that activate dependent feelings—predicts increased risk for alcohol abuse and dependence (Vaillant,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

changes in depressive symptomatology. In this context, several


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

1983).
experiments have examined the interaction of dependency and Numerous correlational studies confirm that there is an asso-
stressful life events as predictors of depression (Hammen, Elli- ciation between alcoholism and dependency (Bertrand & Mas-
cott, & Gitlin, 1989; Hammen, Ellicott, Gitlin, & Jamison, ling, 1969; Conley, 1980; Lawlis & Rubin, 1971; Lemert, 1962;
1989; Hammen, Marks, Mayol, & DeMayo, 1985; Robins, 1990; Poldrugo & Forti, 1988; Spolter, Tokar, & Gocka, 1978; Tog-
Robins & Block, 1988; Smith et al., 1988). nazzo, 1970; Wiener, 1956; Weiss & Masling, 1970; Wolowitz &
Hammen et al. (1985) divided a mixed-sex sample of college Barker, 1968). In these investigations, alcoholic subjects obtain
students into dependent and nondependent groups on the basis significantly higher dependency scores than nonalcoholic sub-
of DEQ Dependency scores. Four months later, subjects re- jects (i.e., abstainers or social drinkers), even when potential
ported the number of negative interpersonal events (e.g., rejec- confounding variables (e.g., overall level of psychopathology)
tion by a friend) and negative achievement-related events (e.g., are controlled. Of course, these studies do not address the ques-
failure on a test) that they had experienced since the first ses- tion of whether dependency actually predisposes people to alco-
sion. Level of depression at follow-up was assessed via the BDI. hol abuse or dependence. Dependency might well predict risk
Hammen et al. (1985) found that dependent subjects reported for alcoholism, but an equally plausible interpretation of these
elevated levels of depression at follow-up only when they experi- data is that alcoholism somehow causes an increase in depen-
enced high levels of negative interpersonal stressors. There was dent feelings, thoughts, and behaviors.
no relationship between depression scores and level of stressful Longitudinal studies of the dependency-alcoholism rela-
achievement-related events in dependent subjects. Further- tionship clearly support the latter hypothesis. M. C. Jones
more, nondependent subjects who experienced high levels of (1968,1971), Kammeier, Hoffman, and Loper (1973), and Vail-
negative interpersonal events did not show elevated levels of lant (1980) conducted prospective studies of the dependency-
depression at follow-up. These findings not only support the alcoholism relationship and obtained consistent results. In each
hypothesis that dependency interacts with stressful life events of these investigations, premorbid dependency levels did not
to predict the onset of depressive symptoms but further suggest predict subsequent risk for alcoholism. However, Vaillant
that—consistent with the findings of Zuroff and Mongrain (1980) found that a variety of dependency-related traits (i.e.,
(1987)—only those negative life events that are specifically re- dependent thoughts, feelings, and behaviors; passivity; pessi-
lated to dependent feelings and dependency needs produce in- mism; self-doubt) showed a significant increase after the onset
creased depression in dependent subjects. Hammen, Ellicott, of alcoholism in his sample of male subjects, who were assessed
and Gitlin (1989), Hammen, Ellicott, Gitlin, and Jamison periodically on a variety of personality and psychopathology
(1989), and Robins (1990) obtained similar results in mixed-sex measures between the ages of 20 and 50.
samples of psychiatric inpatients and outpatients.
Robins and Block (1988) and Smith et al. (1988) also obtained
similar results in mixed-sex undergraduate samples, although
Dependency and Smoking
in both of these studies, a variety of stressful life events—not A number of studies support the psychoanalytic hypothesis
only those events related to interpersonal stress—interacted that dependency should be associated with elevated rates of
with preexisting dependency levels to predict the onset of de- cigarette smoking (J. M. Fisher & Fisher, 1975; Jacobs, Ander-
pressive symptomatology. Thus, whereas the majority of studies son, et al., 1966, Jacobs, Knapp, et al., 1965; Jacobs & Spilken,
in this area indicate that dependency and negative interper- 1971; Kimeldorf & Geiwitz, 1966; Kline & Storey, 1980; Veld-
sonal events interact to predict level of depression in both clini- man & Brown, 1969). Not only do cigarette smokers obtain
cal and nonclinical subjects, the results of Robins and Block's significantly higher dependency scores than nonsmokers (Ja-
and Smith et al.'s recent investigations suggest that other types cobs, Anderson, et al., 1966, Jacobs, Knapp, et al., 1965; Veld-
of stressful iife events may also increase depression levels in man & Brown, 1969), but studies have demonstrated that there
dependent persons. The specificity of the dependency-life is a significant positive correlation between level of dependency
events-depression link warrants further attention from re- and smoking frequency (Jacobs & Spilken, 1971; Kline &
searchers. Storey, 1980). Furthermore, the dependency-smoking relation-
16 ROBERT F. BORNSTEIN

ship has been observed in high school (Jacobs et al., 1965), col- the dependency-obesity link. First, some studies (e.g., Fried-
lege (Veldman & Brown, 1969), and community samples man, 1959; Mills & Cunningham, 1988) do not control for po-
(Fisher & Fisher, 1975). A variety of dependency measures and tential confounding variables (e.g., overall level of psychopathol-
indices of smoking behavior have been used in these studies, ogy), so that observed dependency-obesity relationships in
generally yielding consistent results. these investigations might reflect, in whole or in part, the ef-
Vaillant (1980) further found that level of dependency as- fects of these variables. In addition, researchers examining the
sessed at age 20 predicted subsequent smoking frequency in a dependency-obesity link do not distinguish subjects whose
sample of 184 male college graduates. Vaillant's results suggest weight problems have a physiological (e.g., glandular) cause
that dependency actually predisposes people to cigarette smok- from subjects whose obesity is purely psychological (i.e., behav-
ing, rather than simply being a correlate or product of smoking ioral). Thus, the subjects in these studies are not homogenous
behavior. Because Vaillant's longitudinal sample included only with respect to the etiology of their weight problems. Finally,
men, however, the degree to which dependency predisposes examiners administering the ROD and other widely used pro-
women to smoke remains unexamined. jective dependency measures typically are not blind to the sta-
Vaillant's (1980) results are consistent with the psychoanaly- tus of different subjects (i.e., obese vs. normal weight). Thus,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tic hypothesis that dependent people tend to rely on oral activi- examiners who are aware of the experimental hypothesis might
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ties (e.g., smoking) to cope with anxiety. However, a plausible somehow encourage obese and normal weight subjects to re-
alternative interpretation of these results is that the increased spond differently on projective dependency measures.
rates of cigarette smoking observed in the dependent men in Consistent with this hypothesis, all studies that produced
Vaillant's sample reflect dependent people's susceptibility to positive results in this area used projective measures of depen-
interpersonal influence. It may be that the dependent men in dency (i.e., the ROD, TAT, or Blacky Test). However, of the two
Vaillant's sample were more strongly influenced than were studies reporting nonsignificant results, one (Bornstein &
nondependent men by peer pressure to smoke during late ado- Greenberg, 1991) used a projective measure and the other
lescence. In this context, it is noteworthy that the subjects in (Keith & Vandenberg, 1974) used a self-report measure. The
Vaillant's sample typically initiated smoking during the 1950s Rorschach examiners in Bornstein and Greenberg's study were
and early 1960s, before strong cautionary messages regarding blind to the experimental hypotheses throughout the testing
the dangers of cigarette smoking were commonplace. Today, an procedures, but in all studies of the dependency-obesity link
adolescent considering whether to smoke is confronted with (at that obtained positive results, examiners were aware of the ex-
least) two conflicting messages. One message comes from perimental hypotheses. Thus, findings to date may well be due
those peers who may encourage the person to smoke to fit in. more to experimenters' expectations than to a real depen-
An opposing message comes from the surgeon general and dency-obesity relationship.
other figures of authority (e.g., teachers), who strongly discour- Until better controlled (preferably longitudinal) studies of
age cigarette smoking. the dependency-obesity relationship are conducted, it will be
Thus, the social pressures surrounding cigarette smoking difficult to draw firm conclusions regarding this issue. More
have changed considerably since the subjects in Vaillant's (1980) consistent results come from studies of dependency and eating
sample made their initial decisions regarding whether to disorders (i.e., anorexia and bulimia) in female psychiatric inpa-
smoke. Interestingly, given the dependent person's desire to tients, outpatients, and college students.
please figures of authority rather than peers (Bornstein et al., In one study of this issue, Bornstein and Greenberg (1991)
1987) and the strong antismoking messages conveyed by figures compared the ROD scores of matched samples of obese (n = 18),
of authority today, a longitudinal study of the dependency- eating-disordered (n = 16), and non-eating-disordered, normal
smoking link involving today's college students might yield pre- weight female psychiatric inpatients (n = 17). ROD scores were
cisely the opposite results from those obtained by Vaillant. derived from Rorschach protocols that had been administered
by clinical psychology interns blind to the hypotheses of the
study. Although Bornstein and Greenberg found no difference
Dependency, Obesity and Eating Disorders between obese and control subjects' dependency scores (this
Several studies have found that obese subjects obtain signifi- finding is discussed earlier), the eating-disordered subjects in
cantly higher scores than do normal weight subjects on a vari- Bornstein and Greenberg's sample obtained significantly
ety of dependency measures (Friedman, 1959; Masling et al., higher ROD scores than did subjects in the other two groups.
1967; Mills & Cunningham, 1988; Weiss & Masling, 1970). In Virtually identical findings were reported by Tisdale, Pendel-
addition, there has been at least one cross-cultural replication ton and Marler (1990), and Yager, Landsverk, Edelstein, and
of the dependency-obesity relationship: Masling et al. and Hyler (1989) in samples of female psychiatric inpatients and
Weiss and Masling obtained very similar results in samples of outpatients.
American outpatients (Masling et al., 1967) and Israeli inpa-
tients (Weiss & Masling, 1970) undergoing treatment for obe- Discussion and Conclusions
sity. In both studies, obese subjects showed significantly higher
dependency scores than did normal-weight subjects. However, In light of the wide variety of topics examined by depen-
two experiments have reported negative results in this area, dency researchers, and the different subject groups, experimen-
finding no relationship between dependency and obesity tal procedures, and dependency measures used in different
(Bornstein & Greenberg, 1991; Keith & Vandenberg, 1974). studies, the degree to which developmental, social, and clinical
There are several methodological problems with studies of investigations of dependency have produced consistent results
THE DEPENDENT PERSONALITY 17

is remarkable. Parallel findings have emerged in a number of behaviors (see Maccoby, 1980). In other words, authoritarian
important areas (e.g., with respect to dependent people's ten- parenting (in general) should be associated with an authoritar-
dency to be influenced more strongly by figures of authority ian feeding and weaning style. The degree to which qualities of
than peers). Moreover, studies involving subjects from different the infantile feeding situation parallel qualities of the overall
cultures (i.e., American, British, Indian, Italian, German, parent-child relationship is an important empirical issue that
Israeli, Austrian) have produced highly consistent findings (see, warrants further attention from researchers. Until this issue is
e.g., research on parenting style, sex role, suggestibility, compli- addressed, it will be difficult to compare directly the predictive
ance, help seeking, affiliative behavior, performance anxiety, validities of the psychoanalytic and social learning models with
depression, and alcoholism). respect to the parent-child dynamic during infancy and early
Similar results were found in men and women in most areas childhood.
of dependency research (i.e., in studies of parenting style, sug- The psychoanalytic and social learning models not only
gestibility, compliance, help seeking, affiliative behavior, per- make different predictions regarding the etiology of dependent
formance anxiety, and smoking). However, in two areas (i.e., personality traits, they also make different predictions regard-
interpersonal sensitivity and depression), findings were some- ing the dynamics of dependency. Specifically, the psychoanaly-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

what stronger for men than for women. This pattern of results tic model hypothesizes that dependency should be associated
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

may reflect the fact that in Western societies, dependency is with preoccupation with oral activities (e.g., eating, drinking,
typically viewed as a feminine trait (Spence & Helmreich, smoking) as a means of coping with anxiety, while the social
1978). Consequently, whereas men are discouraged from ex- learning view does not predict that dependent people should
pressing dependent feelings, women have historically been en- engage in these activities more frequently than nondependent
couraged to express such feelings openly. Because overt depen- people. Results in this area are mixed. Consistent with the psy-
dent behavior is less common in men than in women (Stein & choanalytic view, dependency predicts the likelihood of ciga-
Sanfilipo, 1985), dependency in men is a more unusual and rette smoking in men. Similarly, dependency is associated with
noteworthy trait than is dependency in women. In this context, eating disorders in women (although these correlational studies
it is not surprising that dependency in men is a slightly better do not address the question of whether dependency precedes or
predictor of certain traits and behaviors than is dependency in follows the onset of eating disorder symptomatology). However,
women. the finding that dependency does not predict risk for alcohol-
ism is inconsistent with the psychoanalytic view. Unfortunately,
The Predictive Validities of the Psychoanalytic and Social studies of dependency and obesity are so flawed methodologi-
Learning Models of Dependency cally that the relationship between these variables remains
open to question.
Although the results of this review offer some support for Studies that assess the covariation of dependency scores with
both the psychoanalytic and social learning models of depen- scores on indices of preoccupation with food- and mouth-re-
dency, the present results also suggest that both models are lated activities are also mixed, although findings in this area are
limited in their ability to account for the diverse behaviors that somewhat more in line with the social learning model than
are exhibited by dependent persons in various situations and with the psychoanalytic view. Although some factor-analytic
settings. Simply put, neither model has received unequivocal and correlational studies have found that level of dependency is
support. However, the social learning view is somewhat more positively correlated with degree of preoccupation with food-
strongly supported by the present findings than is the psy- and mouth-related activities (e.g., Goldman-Eisler, 1951; Ja-
choanalytic view. In particular, findings regarding parenting mison &Comrey, 1968; Mills& Cunningham, 1988), other simi-
style, marriage role attitudes, and sex role orientation indicate lar studies found no relationship between these variables (e.g.,
that early socialization experiences play a significant role in the Kline & Storey, 1980). Furthermore, Shilkret and Masling
etiology of dependent personality traits. Results of these studies (1981) found a nonsignificant relationship (r = —.06) between
further suggest that sex role socialization may be a particularly scores on the Rorschach Dependency and Food- and Mouth-
important predictor of adult dependency. These findings are Related Activities scales in a mixed-sex sample of undergradu-
consistent with a social learning view of the dependent person- ates. Bornstein and Greenberg (1991) also found a nonsignifi-
ality. cant relationship between Rorschach dependency and food/
Although findings regarding the role of early socialization mouth scores in a sample of female psychiatric inpatients. If
experiences in the etiology of dependency are consistent with future studies confirm that dependency and preoccupation
the social learning view, it would be premature to interpret with food and eating are in fact orthogonal traits, this would
these findings as contradicting the classical psychoanalytic contradict a central prediction made by the classical psychoan-
view of dependency as rooted in the infantile, oral phase of alytic model.
development. Although it is true that studies of dependency
and infantile feeding and weaning behaviors have produced Beyond the Psychoanalytic and Social Learning Models
inconsistent, inconclusive results, methodological limitations
of Dependency
of these investigations do not allow strong conclusions to be
drawn from their results. In fact, it seems likely that the same It is time to go beyond the psychoanalytic and social learning
qualities of the parent-child relationship that predict later de- models of dependency in order to understand more completely
pendency (i.e., overprotectiveness and authoritarianism) would the dynamics of the dependent personality. In the remainder of
also be manifest in the mother's early feeding and weaning this discussion, I focus on four central issues in this area. First, I
18 ROBERT F. BORNSTEIN

suggest that the traditional "deficit" view of dependency may in a wide variety of situations and settings, albeit in different
be overly narrow and unnecessarily pessimistic. Second, I offer ways. Thus, the dependent person yields to group pressure in
some hypotheses regarding the "core" motivation of the depen- most situations (Masling et al., 1968), except when doing so
dent person, which can help to reconcile the wide range of might displease a figure of authority (Bornstein et al., 1987).
(ostensibly conflicting) behaviors exhibited by dependent peo- The dependent person is cooperative in experimental settings
ple. Third, I discuss the critical role of dependency-related cog- (Bornstein & Masling, 1985), shows high levels of performance
nitions in determining the motivations, behaviors, and affective anxiety (R. S. Ojha, 1978), and is responsive to subtle interper-
responses of dependent people. Finally, I suggest that to fully sonal cues emitted by peers (Masling et al., 1974), experi-
understand and reliably predict the behavior of dependent peo- menters (Weiss, 1969), and teachers (Juni & Semel, 1982). Ter-
ple in different situations and settings, we must begin to de- mination, or the perceived threat of termination, of important
velop interactionist models of dependency rather than relying interpersonal relationships increases the dependent person's
exclusively on trait-oriented approaches. level of depression (Hammen, Marks, Mayol, & DeMayo, 1985)
and may place her at risk for a variety of physical illnesses as
Positive and Negative Aspects of Dependency well (Greenberg & Bornstein, 1988a).
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Given dependent people's strong motivation to obtain and


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Overall, the picture of the dependent person that emerges maintain supportive relationships, the apparent inconsistency
from this review is considerably more complex than one might in their behavior across different situations is easy to under-
expect based on the traditional psychological view of depen- stand. Dependent people simply choose to exhibit behaviors
dency. The vast majority of theoreticians and researchers have that they believe will maximize their chances of obtaining and
focused primarily on the negative consequences of dependent maintaining such relationships. When passive, compliant be-
personality traits (e.g., Ainsworth, 1969; Millon, 1981). Consis- havior seems likely to achieve this goal, the dependent person
tent with this viewpoint, the present review confirms that de- chooses to act in a passive manner. When assertive, active be-
pendency is associated with a number of "negative" traits (e.g., havior seems more likely to achieve this goal, the dependent
suggestibility, conformity) and with risk for certain psychologi- person becomes active and assertive. Exhibiting passive, compli-
cal disorders (e.g., depression, tobacco addiction). However, de- ant behavior in certain situations and active help-seeking behav-
pendency is also associated with such positive traits as the abil- ior in others simply represents an attempt on the part of the
ity to infer accurately the attitudes and beliefs of others (i.e., dependent person to fulfill the underlying goal of obtaining
interpersonal sensitivity) and with a desire to perform well in and maintaining the supportive, protective relationships that
psychology experiments. Thus, instead of being simply a prob- they so strongly desire.
lem, deficit, or flaw, as many researchers have suggested, de- Thus, the compliant behavior that is sometimes exhibited by
pendency is associated with both positive and negative quali- the dependent person is best understood not as genuine passiv-
ties. ity and lack of assertiveness, but rather as a kind of self-presen-
The prevailing view of dependent people as uniformly pas- tation strategy that is intended to make certain kinds of impres-
sive and compliant is also contradicted by the present findings. sions on others and to achieve specific goals. As E. E. Jones and
In certain situations (e.g., when asked to serve as a subject in a Pittman (1982) note, "by stressing his inability to fend for him-
psychology experiment), dependency is indeed associated with self and emphasizing his dependence on others, the [dependent
passivity and compliance. However, in other situations, the de- individual ] makes salient a norm of obligation or social respon-
pendent person is anything but passive. Studies confirm that sibility" (p. 247). Research confirms that passive, dependent
dependent persons ask for feedback on psychological tests behaviors do in fact serve as social cues that elicit supportive
more readily than do nondependent persons (Juni, 1981) and and helping behaviors from others (Baker & Reitz, 1978; Berko-
ask for help when attempting to solve difficult problems in the witz & Daniels, 1963, 1964; Harris & Ho, 1984; Taylor et al.,
laboratory more frequently than do nondependent individuals 1982). Apparently, the dependent person knows that exhibiting
(Shilkret & Masling, 1981). Furthermore, dependent people passive, helpless behavior can, in certain situations, be a useful
seek medical attention more quickly than do nondependent way of obtaining nurturance and support. Whether this knowl-
people when physical symptoms appear (Greenberg & Born- edge is implicit (hence unverbalizable) or explicit (hence accessi-
stein, 1988b). Although these behaviors are all characterized by ble to conscious awareness) is an empirical question that has
help seeking, they are also characterized by an assertive, active not yet been addressed.
stance.
The Critical Importance of Dependency-Related
The "Core" Motivation of the Dependent Person Cognitions
How can we reconcile the seemingly inconsistent findings The hypothesis that the dependent person's core motivation
that dependent people are passive and acquiescent in certain is to obtain and maintain nurturant, protective relationships is
contexts and relatively active and assertive in others? By examin- consistent with the finding that overprotective, authoritarian
ing the motivations that underly the dependent person's behav- parenting predicts level of dependency in childhood, adoles-
ior in different situations. The present review suggests that one cence, and adulthood. Because early relationships with the par-
central goal underlies much of the dependent person's behav- ents and other significant figures play a central role in the con-
ior: obtaining and maintaining nurturant, supportive relation- struction of the self-concept, the child of overprotective parents
ships. This core motivation of the dependent person is reflected may come to believe that he or she cannot function adequately
THE DEPENDENT PERSONALITY 19

without the guidance and protection of others, particularly fig- behavior of the dependent person can only be understood fully
ures of authority (Baumrind, 1973). Furthermore, because (and predicted accurately) with reference to the dependent per-
early relationships with the parents create particular expecta- son's underlying cognitive constructs. As Dweck and Leggett
tions for future interpersonal relationships (Bornstein et al., (1988) note, the explanatory power of dispositional variables
1986; Waters & Deane, 1985), parental overprotectiveness will "lies in their ability to predict what behaviors will be exhibited
lead to an expectation on the part of the child that he or she will in various situations, not in their prediction that the same be-
be nurtured and cared for by others. Similarly, parental authori- havior will be exhibited across situations" (p. 270).
tarianism will lead the child to believe that the way to maintain Because dependent people interpret different situations as
good relationships with others is to acquiesce to their requests, involving different demands, opportunities, and risks, they
expectations, and demands (Baumrind, 1971; Maccoby, 1980). choose to behave in somewhat different ways in different con-
In short, the behavior of the dependent person in social settings texts. Underlying these surface differences in behavior, how-
may be traced to particular beliefs about the self and other ever, is a fundamental consistency: When the prototypic behav-
people, which in turn may be traced to early experiences within iors of the dependent person in different situations and settings
the family. are interpreted in the context of the dependent person's most
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Thus, cognitive structures (i.e., self and object representa- important cognitive constructs (i.e., a view of the self as power-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tions) that are formed in response to early experiences within less and ineffectual and a belief that others can provide needed
the family will influence the motivations, behaviors, and affec- guidance and protection) and goals (i.e., a desire to be guided
tive responses of the dependent person in predictable ways. A and supported by others), apparent inconsistencies in the behav-
perception of oneself as powerless and in need of guidance and iors exhibited in different settings disappear.
support from others will, first and foremost, have motivational Because mental representations of self and others play an
effects: A person with such a self-concept will be motivated to important role in determining the behavior of dependent peo-
seek guidance, support, protection, and nurturance from other ple in different settings, continued research on the object rela-
people. These self-concept-based motivations in turn produce tional aspects of dependency is warranted. There have been
particular patterns of dependent behavior: The person who is several studies of dependent people's self-representations (e.g.,
highly motivated to seek the guidance, protection, and support Bornstein et al., 1988) and of dependent people's mental repre-
of others will behave in ways that maximize the probability that sentations of parents and other significant figures (e.g., Blatt,
they will obtain the guidance, protection, and support that they Wein, Chevron, & Quinlan, 1979). The results of this review
desire. Finally, a representation of the self as powerless and suggest that these cognitive constructs represent important
ineffectual will have important affective consequences (e.g., links among the disparate behaviors exhibited by dependent
fearofabandonment, performance anxiety, fearof negative eval- people in different situations and settings. To the extent that the
uation). dependent person's mental representations of significant indi-
viduals (e.g., self, parents, peers, teachers, doctors, therapists)
Toward an Interactionist Model of Dependency are elucidated, our ability to understand and predict the behav-
ior of dependent persons in different contexts is likely to in-
The fact that beliefs and expectations regarding self and crease.
others appear to play a central role in directing the behavior of
the dependent person suggests that the traditional disposi-
References
tional view of dependency may be overly narrow and somewhat
limited in its ability to predict the behavior of the dependent Abramson, L., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. (1978). Learned help-
person in different situations. Although adult dependency is a lessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnor-
stable personality trait (see Fisher & Greenberg, 1985; McMa- mal Psychology, 87, 49-74.
hon, Flynn, & Davidson, 1985; Millon, 1987), the behavior of Agrawal, K., & Rai, S. N. (1988). Post-noise frustration tolerance as a
the dependent person can only be completely understood with function of controllability of noise and dependence proneness. In-
dian Journal of Psychometry and Education, 19, 85-89.
reference to the context in which it is exhibited. In other words,
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1969). Object relations, dependency and attach-
although the core underlying motivation of the dependent per- ment: A theoretical review of the infant-mother relationship. Child
son remains constant, the behavior that is exhibited by the Development, 40, 969-1025.
dependent person changes somewhat from situation to situa- Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1972). Attachment and dependency: A compari-
tion, reflecting (a) the person's perceptions of the demands, son. In J. L. Gewirtz (Ed.), Attachment and dependency (-pp. 97-137).
expectations, and behavioral constraints that characterize a New York: Wiley.
given situation and (b) his or her beliefs regarding what behav- Akiskal, H. S., Hirschfeld, R. M. A., & Yerevanian, B. I. (1983). The
iors are most likely to produce the desired outcome, given those relationship of personality to affective disorders. Archives of General
demands, expectations, and constraints. Psychiatry, 40,801-810.
The present findings therefore suggest that to understand Alam, K. (1986). An experimental study of retention in relation to
more fully the behavior of the dependent person, one must use approval motive and dependence proneness. Journal of Psychologi-
cal Research, 30, 144-149.
an interactionist approach rather than relying solely on the tra- Anderson, K. L. (1986). Androgeny, flexibility and individualism.
ditional trait-oriented models. In keeping with Mischel's(1973, Journal of Personality Assessment, 50, 265-278.
1979,1984) view that the behavior of people is best understood Asch, S. E. (1956). Studies of independence and conformity: A minor-
as proactive and directed by their beliefs and expectations re- ity of one against a unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs,
garding self and others, the present review suggests that the 70(9, Whole No. 416).
20 ROBERT F. BORNSTEIN

Baker, L. D., & Reitz, H. J. (1978). Altruism toward the blind: Effects of depression: An empirical study. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 2. 241-
sex of helper and dependency of victim. Journal of Social Psychol- 249.
ogy, 104,19-28. Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behav- Bowlby, J. (1973). Separation: Anxiety and anger. New York: Basic
ior change. Psychological Review, 84, 191-215. Books.
Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Develop- Bowlby, J. (1980). Loss: Sadness and depression. New York: Basic
mental Psychology Monograph, 4(1, Pt. 2). Books.
Baumrind, D. (1973). The development of instrumental competence Brown, J. D, & Silberschatz, G. (1989). Dependency, self-criticism and
through socialization. In A. D. Pick (Ed.), Minnesota symposium on depressive attributional style. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98,
child psychology (Vol. 7, pp. 3-46). Minneapolis: University of Min- 187-188.
nesota Press. Chadha, N. K. (1983). A study of dependence proneness and test anxi-
Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental and theoretical ety in school-going children. Indian Psychological Review, 24, 34-38.
aspects. New York: Holber. Chevron, E. S., Quinlan, D. M., & Blatt, S. J. (1978). Sex roles and
Bern, S. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgeny. Journal gender differences in the experience of depression. Journal of Ab-
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42,155-162. normal Psychology. 87, 680-683.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Berkowitz, L., & Daniels, L. R. (1963). Responsibility and dependency. Conley, J. J. (1980). Family configuration as an etiological factor in
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, 429-436. alcoholism. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 670-673.
Berkowitz, L., & Daniels, L. R. (1964). Affecting the salience of the Diener, R. G. (1967). Prediction of dependent behavior in specified
social responsibility norm: Effects of past help on the response to situations from psychological tests. Psychological Reports, 20, 103-
dependency relationships. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychol- 108.
ogy, 68, 275-281. Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to
Bernardin, A., & Jessor, R. (1957). A construct validation of the Ed- motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256-273.
wards Personal Preference Schedule with respect to dependency. Ederer, E. (1988). Dysthymia, psychosocial dependency, and self-es-
Journal of Consulting Psychology, 21, 63-67. teem in ten-year-old boys and girls. Studla Psychologica, 30, 227-
Bertrand, S., & Masling, J. (1969). Oral imagery and alcoholism. Jour- 235.
nal of Abnormal Psychology, 74, 50-53. Edwards, A. L. (1959). Manual for the Edwards Personal Preference
Bhogle, S. (1983). Antecedents of dependency behavior in children of Schedule. New York: Psychological Corporation.
low social class. Psychological Studies, 28, 92-95. Finney, J. C. (1961). Some maternal influences on children's personal-
Biaggio, M. K., Godwin, W H., & Baldwin, H. K. (1984). Response to ity and character. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 63, 199-278.
interpersonal request styles by dependent and overcontrolled-hos- Fisher, J. M., & Fisher, S. (1975). Response to cigarette deprivation as a
tile personalities. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 40, 833-836. function of oral fantasy. Journal of Personality Assessment, 39, 381-
Birtchnell, 1, & Kennard, J. (1983). What does the MMP1 Dependency 385.
scale really measure? Journal of Clinical Psychology, 39, 532-543. Fisher, S. (1970). Body experience in fantasy and behavior. New York:
Blatt, S. J., D'Afflitti, J. P., & Quintan, D. M. (1976). Experiences of Appleton-Century-Crofts.
depression in normal young adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Fisher, S., & Greenberg, R. R (1985). The scientific credibility of Freud's
85. 383-389. theories and therapy. New York: Columbia University Press.
Blatt, S. J., Quinlan, D. M., Chevron, E. S., McDonald, C, & Zuroff, D. Fitzgerald, B. J. (1958). Some relationships among projective test, in-
(1982). Dependency and self-criticism: Psychological dimensions of terview and sociometric measures of dependent behavior. Journal of
depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, 113- Abnormal and Social Psychology, 56, 199-203.
124. Flanders, N. A., Anderson, J. P., & Amidon, E. J. (1961). Measuring
Blatt, S. J., Wein, S. J., Chevron, E. S., & Quinlan, D. M. (1979). Parental dependence proneness in the classroom. Educational and Psycholog-
representations and depression in normal young adults. Journal of ical Measurement, 21, 575-587.
Abnormal Psychology, 88, 388-397. Freud, S. (1953). An outline of psychoanalysis. In J. Strachey (Ed. and
Blum, G. S. (1949). A study of the psychoanalytic theory of psychosex- Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of
ual development. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 39, 3-99. Sigmund Freud(Vo\. 23, pp. 141 -279). London: Hogarth Press. (Orig-
Bornstein, R. E, Galley, D. J., & Leone, D. R. (1986). Parental represen- inal work published 1938)
tations and orality. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50, 80-89. Friedman, J. (1959). Weight problems and psychological factors. Jour-
Bornstein, R. F., & Greenberg, R. P. (1991). Dependency and eating nal of Consulting Psychology, 23, 524-527.
disorders in female psychiatric inpatients. Journal of Nervous and Fu, V R., Hinkle, D. E., & Hanna, M. A. K. (1986). A three-generation
Mental Disease, 179, 248-252. study of the development of individual dependency and family inter-
Bornstein, R. E, & Johnson, J. G. (1990). Dependency and psychopa- dependence. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 112,153-171.
thoiogy in a nonclinical sample. Journal of Social Behavior and Per- Fu, V R., Hinkle, D. E., Shoffner, S., et al. (1984). Maternal dependency
sonality. 5, 417-422. and childrearing attitudes among mothers of adolescent females.
Bornstein, R. F, Leone, D. R., & Galley, D. J. (1988). Rorschach mea- Adolescence, 19, 795-804.
sures of oral dependence and the internalized self-representation in Goldman-Eisler, F. (1951). The problem of "orality" and its origin in
normal college students. Journal of Personality Assessment, 52,648- early childhood. Journal of Mental Science, 97, 765-782.
657. Golightly, C., Nelson, D., & Johnson, J. (1970). Children's Dependency
Bornstein, R. F, & Masling, J. M. (1985). Orality and latency of volun- Scale. Developmental Psychology, 3,114-118.
teering to serve as experimental subjects: A replication. Journal of Gordon, M., & Tegtemeyer, P. F. (1983). Oral dependent content in
Personality Assessment, 49, 306-310. children's Rorschach protocols. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 57,
Bornstein, R. E, Masling, J. M., & Poynton, F. G. (1987). Orality as a 1163-1168.
factor in interpersonal yielding. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 4,161- Greenberg, J. R., & Mitchell, S. J. (1983). Object relations in psychoana-
170. lytic theory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bornstein, R. F, Poynton, F. G, & Masling, J. M. (1985). Orality and Greenberg, R. P., & Bornstein, R. F. (1988a). The dependent personal-
THE DEPENDENT PERSONALITY 21

ity: I. Risk for physical disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 2, heavy cigarette smoking in males. Journal of Nervous and Mental
126-135. Disease, 141,161-171.
Greenberg, R. P., & Bornstein, R. F. (1988b). The dependent personal- Jacobs, M. A., & Spilken, A. Z. (1971). Personality patterns associated
ity: II. Risk for psychological disorders. Journal of Personality Dis- with heavy cigarette smoking in male college students. Journal of
orders, 2,136-143. Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 37, 428-432.
Greenberg, R. P., & Bornstein, R. F. (1989). Length of psychiatric hospi- Jakubczak, L. F., & Walters, R. H. (1959). Suggestibility as dependency
talization and oral dependency. Journal of Personality Disorders, 3, behavior. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 59, 102-107.
199-204. Jamison, K., & Comrey, A. L. (1968). Further study of dependence as a
Hammen, C, Ellicott, A., & Gitlin, M. (1989). Vulnerability to specific personality factor. Psychological Reports, 22, 239-242.
life events and prediction of course of disorder in unipolar de- Jones, E. E., & Pittman, T. S. (1982). Toward a general theory of strate-
pressed patients. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 21, 377- gic self-presentation. In J. Suls (Ed.), Psychological perspectives on
388. the self (pp. 231-262). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hammen, C. L., Ellicott, A., Gitlin, M., & Jamison, K. R. (1989). Jones, M. C. (1968). Personality correlates and antecedentsof drinking
Sociotropy/autonomy and vulnerability of specific life events in pa- patterns in adult males. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol-
tients with unipolar depression and bipolar disorders. Journal of ogy, 32. 2-12.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Abnormal Psychology, 98, 154-160. Jones, M. C. (1971). Personality antecedents and correlates of drinking
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Hammen, C. L., Marks, T, Mayol, A., & DeMayo, R. (1985). Depres- patterns in women. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
sive self-schemas, life stress and vulnerability to depression. Journal 36,61-69.
of Abnormal Psychology, 94, 308-319. Juni, S. (1981). Maintaining anonymity vs requesting feedback as a
Harlow, H. F. (1958). The nature of love. American Psychologist, 13, function of oral dependency. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 52, 239-
673-685. 242.
Harris, M. B., & Ho, J. (1984). Effects of degree, locus and controllabil- Juni, S., Masling, J. M., & Brannon, R. (1979). Interpersonal touching
ity of dependency and sex of subject on anticipated and actual help- and orality. Journal of Personality Assessment, 43, 235-237.
ing. Journal of Social Psychology, 122, 245-255. Juni, S., & Semel, S. R. (1982). Person perception as a function of
Hatfield, J. S., Ferguson, P. E., Rau, L., & Alpert, R. (1967). Mother- orality and anality. Journal of Social Psychology, 118, 99-103.
child interaction and the socialization process. Child Development, Kagan, J., & Moss, H. A. (1960). The stability of passive and dependent
38, 365-414. behavior from childhood through adulthood. Child Development,
31, 577-591.
Heinstein, M. I. (1963). Behavioral correlates of breast-bottle regimes
under varying parent-infant relationships. Monographs of the Soci- Kagan, J., & Mussen, P. (1956). Dependency themes on the TAT and
ety for Research on Human Development, 28,1-61. group conformity. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 20, 29-32.
Kammeier, M. L., Hoffman, H., & Loper, R. G. (1973). Personality
Hirschfeld, R. M. A., Klerman, G. L., Andreason, N. C., Clayton, P. J.,
characteristics of alcoholics as college freshman and at time of treat-
& Keller, M. B. (1986). Psychosocial predictors of chronicity in de-
ment. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 34, 390-399.
pressed patients. British Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 648-654.
Kaul, V, Mathur, P., & Murlidharan, R. (1982). Dependency and its
Hirschfeld, R. M. A., Klerman, G. L., Clayton, P. J., & Keller, M. B. antecedents. Indian Educational Review, 17, 35-46.
(1983). Personality and depression: Empirical findings. Archives of
Keinan, G., & Hobfoll, S. E. (1989). Stress, dependency and social sup-
General Psychiatry, 40, 993-998.
port: Who benefits from husband's presence in delivery? Journal of
Hirschfeld, R. M. A., Klerman, G. L., Gough, H. G., Barrett, J., Kor- Social and Clinical Psychology, 8, 32-44.
chin, S. J., & Chodoff, P. (1977). A measure of interpersonal depen- Keith, R. R., & Vandenberg, S. G. (1974). Relation between orality and
dency. Journal of Personality Assessment, 41, 610-618. weight. Psychological Reports, 35, 1205-1206.
Hirschfeld, R. M. A., Klerman, G. L., Lavori, P, Keller, M. B., Grif- Kimeldorf, C, & Geiwitz, P J. (1966). Smoking and the Blacky orality
fith, P., & Coryell, W (1989). Premorbid personality assessments of factors. Journal of Protective Techniques, 30, 167-168.
first onset of major depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, Klein, D. N. (1989). The Depressive Experiences Questionnaire: A
345-350. further evaluation. Journal of Personality Assessment, 53, 703-715.
Hollender, M. C, Luborsky, L., & Harvey, R. (1970). Correlates of the Klein, D. N., Harding, K., Taylor, E. B., & Dickstein, S. (1988). Depen-
desire to be held in women. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 14, dency and self-criticism in depression: Evaluation in a clinical popu-
387-390. lation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 399-404.
Holt, R. R. (1966). Measuring libidinal and aggressive motives and Kline, P., & Storey, R. (1980). The etiology of the oral character. Journal
their controls by means of the Rorschach test. In D. Levine (Ed.), of Genetic Psychology, 136, 85-94.
Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 14, pp. 1 -47). Lincoln: Uni- Lao, R. C. (1980). Differential factors affecting male and female aca-
versity of Nebraska Press. demic performance in high school. Journal of Psychology, 104,119-
Holt, R. R. (1970). Artistic creativity and Rorschach measures of adap- 127.
tive regression. In B. Klopfer, M. M. Meyer, F. B. Brawer, & W G. Lawlis, G. F, & Rubin, S. E. (1971). A 16PF study of personality pat-
Klopfer (Eds.), Developments in the Rorschach technique(pp. 37-63). terns in alcoholics. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 32, 318-
New York: Harcourt-Brace. 327.
Hunt, E., Browning, P., & Nave, G. (1982). A behavioral exploration of Lazare, A., Klerman, G. L., & Armor, D. J. (1966). Oral, obsessive and
dependent and independent mildly mentally retarded adolescents hysterical personality patterns. Archives of General Psychiatry, 14,
and their mothers. Applied Research in Mental Retardation, 3,141- 624-630.
150. Lazare, A., Klerman, G. L., & Armor, D. J. (1970). Oral, obsessive and
Jacobs, M. A., Anderson, L. S., Champagne, E., Karush, N., Richman, hysterical personality patterns: Replication of factor analysis in an
S. J., & Knapp, P. H. (1966). Orality, impulsivity and cigarette smok- independent sample. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 7, 275-290.
ing in men. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 143, 207-219. Lemert, E. M. (1962). Dependency in married alcoholics. Quarterly
Jacobs, M. A., Knapp, P. H., Anderson, L. S., Karush, N., Meissner, R., Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 23, 590-609.
& Richman, S. J. (1965). Relationship of oral frustration factors with Livesley, W J., Schroeder, M. L., & Jackson, D. N. (1990). Dependent
22 ROBERT F. BORNSTEIN

personality disorder and attachment problems. Journal of Personal- Murphy, L. B. (1962). The widening world of childhood. New York: Basic
ity Disorders, 4, \ 31 -140. Books.
Maccoby, E. E. (1980). Social development. New York: Harcourt-Brace. Nacev, V (1980). Dependency and ego strength as indicators of pa-
Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psychology of sex differ- tients' attendance in psychotherapy. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
ences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. 36,691-695.
Marcus, R. F. (1975). The child as elicitor of parental sanctions for Navran, L. (1954). A rationally derived MMPI scale to measure depen-
independent and dependent behavior. Developmental Psychology, dence. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 18, 192.
11, 443-452. Nietzel, M. T, & Harris, M. J. (1990). Relationship of dependency and
Marcus, R. F. (1976). The effects of children's emotional and instru- achievement/autonomy to depression. Clinical Psychology Review,
mental dependent behavior on parental response. Journal of Psychol- 10, 279-297.
ogy, 92, 57-63. Ojha, H. (1972). The relation of prestige suggestion to rigidity and
Masling, J. M. (1986). Orality, pathology and interpersonal behavior. dependence proneness. Journal of Psychological Research, 16, 70-
In J. Masling(Ed.), Empirical studies of psychoanalytic theories (Vol. 73.
2, pp. 73-106). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Ojha, H., & Singh, R. 1. P. (1972). Personality factors and marriage role
Masling, J. M., Johnson, C, & Saturansky, C. (1974). Oral imagery, attitudes. Psychological Studies, 17, 13-16.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

accuracy of perceiving others, and performance in Peace Corps Ojha, H., & Singh, R. R. (1985). Relationship of marriage role attitude
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

training. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 414-419. with dependence proneness and insecurity in university students.
Masling, X M., O'Neill, R. M., & Jayne, C. (1981). Orality and latency Psychologia, 28, 249-253.
of volunteering to serve as experimental subjects. Journal of Personal- Ojha, H., & Singh, R. R. (1988). Childrearing attitudes as related to
ity Assessment, 45, 20-22. insecurity and dependence proneness. Psychological Studies, 33,75-
Masling, J. M., O'Neill, R. M., & Katkin, E. S. (1982). Autonomic 79.
arousal, interpersonal climate and oralily. Journal of Personality and Ojha, R. S. (1978). Reaction time as a function of dependence prone-
Social Psychology, 42, 529-534. ness. Journal of Psychological Research, 22, 151-152.
Masling, J. M., Price, J., Goldband, S., & Katkin, E. S. (1981). Oral O'Neill, R. M., & Bornstein, R. F. (1991). Orality and depression in
imagery and autonomic arousal in social isolation. Journal of Person-
psychiatric inpatients. Journal of Personality Disorders, 5, 1-7.
ality and Social Psychology, 40, 395-400.
Parker, G., & Lipscombe, P. (1980). The relevance of early parental
Masling, J. M., Rabie, L., & Blondheim, S. H. (1967). Obesity, level of
experiences to adult dependency, hypochondriasis and utilization of
aspiration, and Rorschach and TAT measures of oral dependence.
primary physicians. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 53, 355-
Journal of Consulting Psychology, 31, 233-239. 363.
Masling, J. M., & Schwartz, M. A. (1979). A critique of research in
Parker, G., Tupling, H., & Brown, L. B. (1979). A parental bonding
psychoanalytic theory. Genetic Psychology Monographs, WO, 257-
instrument. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 52, 1-10.
307.
Pilkonis, P. A. (1988). Personality prototypes among depressives:
Masling, J. M., Weiss, L., & Rothschild, B. (1968). Relationships of oral
Themes of dependency and autonomy. Journal of Personality Dis-
imagery to yielding behavior and birth order. Journal of Consulting
orders, 2, 144-152.
and Clinical Psychology, 32, 89-91.
McCord, J., McCord, W, &Thurber, E. (1962). Some effects of paternal Pilowski, I. (1979). Personality and depressive illness. Acta Psychiat-
absence on male children. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychol- nca Scandinavia, 60, 170-176.
ogy, 64, 361-369. Pilowski, I., & Katsikitis, M. (1983). Depressive illness and depen-
McCrame, E. W, & Bass, J. D. (1984). Childhood family antecedents of dency. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavia, 68,11-14.
dependency and self-criticism. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, Poldrugo, F, & Forti, B. (1988). Personality disorders and alcoholism
3-8. treatment outcome. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 21, 171-176.
McMahon, R. C, Flynn, P. M., & Davidson, R. S. (1985). Stability of Robins, C. J. (1990). Congruence of personality and life events in de-
the personality and symptom scales of the Millon Clinical Multiax- pression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 393-397.
ial Inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 231-234. Robins, C. J., & Block, P. (1988). Personal vulnerability, life events, and
McPartland, J. M., & Epstein, J. L. (1975). An investigation of the inter- depressive symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
action of family and social factors in open school. Baltimore: Centre 54, 847-852.
for the Social Organization of Schools, Johns Hopkins University. Robins, C. J., Block, P., & Peselow, E. D. (1989). Relationsof sociotropic
Millon, T. (1981). Disorders of personality: DSM-HI Axis 2. New York: and autonomous personality characteristics to specific symptoms in
Wiley. depressed patients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 86-87.
Millon, T. (1987). Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory—// manual. Roe, A., & Siegelman, M. (1963). Parent-Child Relation Question-
Minneapolis, MN: National Computer Systems. naire. Child Development, 34, 355-369.
Mills, I K., & Cunningham, J. (1988). Oral character and attitudes and Rosenthal, R. (1984). Meta-analytic procedures for social research. Bev-
behavior related to food and eating. Psychological Reports, 63, 15- erly Hills, CA: Sage.
18. Rossman, P. (1988). Interpersonal dependency: A risk factor for de-
Mischel, W (1973). Toward a cognitive social learningreconceptualiza- pressive disorders? Psychologische Beitrage, 30, 18-28.
tion of personality. Psychological Review, 80, 252-283. Sansanwal, D. N., Jarial, G. S., & Dandel, R. (1982). An experimental
Mischel, W (1979). On the interface of cognition and personality: study of the effect of dependency and adjustment on the achieve-
Beyond the person-situation debate. American Psychologist, 34, ment of students studying through programmed learning material
740-754. using different response modes. Indian Educational Review, 17,140-
Mischel, W (1984). Convergences and challenges in the search for con- 146.
sistency. American Psychologist, 39, 351-364. Sears, R. R. (1972). Attachment, dependency and frustration. In J. L.
Mischel, W, & Peake, P. K. (1982). Beyond deja vu in the search for Gewirtz (Ed.), Attachment and dependency (pp. 1-27). New York:
cross-situational consistency. Psychological Review, 89, 730-755. Wiley.
THE DEPENDENT PERSONALITY 23

Sears, R. R., Rau, L., & Alpert, R. (1965). Identification and child Walters, R. H., & Parke, R. D. (1964). Social motivation, dependency
rearing. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. and susceptibility to social influence. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Ad-
Sears, R. R., Whiting, J. W M, Nowlis, V, & Sears, P. S. (1953). Some vances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 231-276). San
child rearing antecedents of aggression and dependency in young Diego, CA: Academic Press.
children. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 47,135-236. Waters, E., & Deane, K. E. (1985). Defining and assessing individual
Shilkret, C. J., & Masling, J. M. (1981). Oral dependence and depen- differences in attachment relationships. Monographs of the Society
dent behavior. Journal of Personality Assessment, 45,125-129. for Research in Child Development, 50, 41-65.
Singh, R. R., & Ojha, S. K. (1987). Sex difference in dependence prone- Watson, P. J., Biderman, M. D., & Boyd, C. (1989). Androgeny as syn-
ness, insecurity and self-concept. Manas, 34, 61-66. thetic narcissism: Sex role measures and Kohut's psychology of the
Sinha, J. B. P. (1968). A test of dependence proneness. Journal of Psycho- self. Sex Roles, 21, 175-207.
logical Research, 12, 66-70. Weiss, L. R. (1969). Effects of subject, experimenter and task variables
Sinha, J. B. P., & Pandey, J. (1972). The processes of decision making in on compliance with the experimenter's expectation. Journal of Pro-
dependence-prone persons. Journal of Psychological Research, 16, jective Techniques and Personality Assessment, 33, 247-256.
35-37. Weiss, L. R., & Masling, J. (1970). Further validation of a Rorschach
Smith, T. W, O'Keeffe, J. L., & Jenkins, M. (1988). Dependency and measure of oral imagery: A study of six clinical groups. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 76, 83-87.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

self-criticism: Correlates of depression or moderators of the effects


Welkowitz, J., Lish, J. D, & Bond, R. N. (1985). The Depressive Experi-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

of stressful events? Journal of Personality Disorders, 2, 160-169.


Spence, J. T, & Helmreich, R. L. (1978). Masculinity and femininity: ences Questionnaire: Revision and validation. Journal of Personality
Their psychological dimensions, correlates and antecedents. Austin: Assessment, 49, 89-94.
University of Texas Press. West, M, Livesley, W J., Reiffer, L., & Sheldon, A. (1986). The place of
Sperling, M. B., & Berman, W H. (1991). An attachment classification attachment in the life events model of stress and illness. Canadian
ofdesparate love. Journal of Personality Assessment, 56, 45-55. Journal of Psychiatry, 31, 202-207.
White, H. (1986). Damsels in distress: Dependency themes in fiction
Spoiler, B. M., Tokar, J. T, & Gocka, E. F. (1978). Dependency and
for children and adolescents. Adolescence, 21, 251-256.
alcoholic recidivism. Psychological Reports, 43, 538.
Wiener, G. (1956). Neurotic depressives and alcoholics: Oral Rors-
Sroufe, L. A., Fox, N. E., & Pancake, V R. (1983). Attachment and chach percepts. Journal of Protective Techniques, 20, 435-455.
dependency in developmental perspective. Child Development, 54, Winder, C. L., & Rau, L. (1962). Parental attitudes associated with
1615-1627. social deviance in preadolescent boys. Journal of Abnormal and So-
Stein, N., & Sanfilipo, M. (1985). Depression and the wish to be held. cial Psychology, 64, 418-424.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41, 3-9. Wittenborn, J. R., & Maurer, H. S. (1977). Persistent personalities
Taylor, R. D, Messick, D. M., Lehman, G. A., & Hirsch, J. K. (1982). among depressed women. Archives of General Psychiatry, 34, 968-
Sex, dependency and helping revisited. Journal of Social Psychology, 971.
118, 59-65. Wolowitz, H. M., & Barker, M. J. (1968). Alcoholism and oral passivity.
Tesser, A., & Blusiewicz, C. O. (1987). Dependency conflict and under- Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 29, 592-597.
achievement. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 5, 378-390. Yager, J., Landsverk, J., Edelstein, C. K., & Hyler, S. E. (1989). Screen-
Thurston, J. R., & Mussen, P. H. (1951). Infant feeding gratification ing for Axis II personality disorders in women with bulimic eating
and adult personality. Journal of Personality, 19, 449-458. disorders. Psychosomatics, 30, 255-262.
Tisdale, M. J., Pendelton, L., & Marler, M. (1990). MCMI characteris- Yeger, T, & Miezitis, S. (1985). Pupil sex as it relates to the pupil-
tics of DSM-III-R bulimics. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55, teacher dependency relationship. International Journal of Women's
477-483. Studies, 8, 457-464.
Tognazzo, D. P. (1970). Oral Rorschach content in a group of alco- Zuckerman, M., Levitt, E. E., & Lubin, B. (1961). Concurrent and
holics. Psychiatrica Generate e Dell-ete Evolutiva, 8, 323-325. construct validity of direct and indirect measures of dependency.
Tribich, D., & Messer, S. (1974). Psychoanalytic character type and Journal of Consulting Psychology, 25, 316-323.
status of authority as determiners of suggestibility. Journal of Con- Zung, W W K. (1965). A self-rating depression scale. Archives of Gen-
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 842-848. eral Psychiatry, 12, 63-70.
Vaillant, G. E. (1980). Natural history of male psychological health: Zuroff, D. C., & Mongrain, M. (1987). Dependency and self-criticism:
VIII. Antecedents of alcoholism and orality. American Journal of Vulnerability factors for depressive affective states. Journalof Abnor-
Psychiatry, 137, 181-186. mal Psychology, 96,14-22.
Vaillant, G. E. (1983). The natural history of alcoholism. Cambridge, Zuroff, D. C., Moskowitz, D. S., Wielgus, M. S., Powers, T. A., & Franko,
MA: Harvard University Press. D. L. (1983). Construct validation of the Dependency and Self-Criti-
Vats, A. (1986). Birth order, sex and dependence-proneness in Indian cism scales of the Depressive Experiences Questionnaire. Journalof
students. Psychological Reports, 58, 284-286. Research in Personality, 17, 226-241.
Veldman, D. J., & Brown, O. H. (1969). Personality and performance
characteristics associated with cigarette smoking among college Received February 4,1991
freshmen. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33, 109- Revision received September 9,1991
119. Accepted September 9,1991 •

Вам также может понравиться