Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 28

YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY 44:177–204 (2001)

A Proper Study for Mankind: Analogies From the


Papionin Monkeys and Their Implications
for Human Evolution
Clifford J. Jolly

Department of Anthropology, New York University, New York, New York 10003

KEY WORDS human evolution; evolutionary analogies; baboons; Papionini; hominins

ABSTRACT This paper’s theme is that analogies Another source of phylogenetic uncertainty is the pos-
drawn from the cercopithecine tribe Papionini, especially sibility of gene-flow by occasional hybridization between
the African subtribe Papionina (baboons, mangabeys, and hominins belonging to ecologically and adaptively distinct
mandrills), can be a valuable source of insights about the species or even genera. Although the evidence is unsatis-
evolution of the human tribe, Hominini, to complement factorily sparse, it suggests that among catarrhines gen-
homologies found in extant humans and/or African apes. erally, regardless of major chromosomal rearrangements,
Analogies, involving a “likeness of relations” of the form intersterility is roughly proportional to time since clado-
“A is to B, as X is to Y,” can be usefully derived from genetic separation. On a papionin analogy, especially the
nonhomologous (homoplastic) resemblances in morphol- crossability of Papio hamadryas with Macaca mulatta and
ogy, behavior, ecology, or population structure. Pragmat- Theropithecus gelada, crossing between extant hominine
ically, the papionins are a fruitful source of analogies for genera is unlikely to produce viable and fertile offspring,
hominins because they are phylogenetically close enough but any hominine species whose ancestries diverged less
to share many basic attributes by homology, yet far than 4 ma previously may well have been able to produce
enough that homoplastic modifications of these features hybrid offspring that could, by backcrossing, introduce
are easily recognized as such. In “The Seedeaters,” an alien genes with the potential of spreading if advanta-
analogy between Theropithecus among baboons and Aus- geous. Selection against maladaptive traits would main-
tralopithecus africanus among hominines was the source tain adaptive complexes against occasional genetic infil-
of a widely discussed (and often misrepresented) diet- tration, and the latter does not justify reducing the
based scenario of hominin origins that explained previ- hybridizing forms to a conspecific or congeneric rank.
ously unassociated hominin apomorphies, interpreted Whether reticulation could explain apparent parallels in
basal hominins as nonhuman rather than prehuman pri- hominin dentition and brain size is uncertain, pending
mates, and accommodated a basal hominin adaptive radi- genetic investigation of these apparently complex traits.
ation of at least two lines. Widespread papionin taxa (such as Papio baboons and
Current usage recognizes an even more extensive evo- species-groups of the genus Macaca), like many such or-
lutionary radiation among the basal hominins, originat- ganisms, are distributed as a “patchwork” of nonoverlap-
ing no earlier than about 7 ma, with multiple lineages ping but often parapatric forms (allotaxa). Morphologi-
documented or inferred by 2.5 ma. Although multilin- cally diagnosable, yet not reproductively isolated, most
eage clades (especially the Paranthropus clade) within allotaxa would be designated species by the phylogenetic
this complex are widely recognized, and emerge from species concept, but subspecies by the biological species
sophisticated, parsimony-based analyses, it is sus- concept, and use of the term “allotaxa” avoids this incon-
pected that in many cases, developmental or functional sistency. A line of contact between allotaxa typically coin-
homoplasies are overwhelming the phylogenetic signal cides with an ecotone, with neighboring allotaxa occupy-
in the data. The papionin analogy (specifically the split- ing similar econiches in slightly different habitats, and
ting of the traditional, morphology-based genera Cerco- often exhibiting subtle, adaptive, morphological differ-
cebus and Papio mandated by molecular evidence) illus- ences as well as their defining differences of pelage. “Hy-
trates the power of these factors to produce erroneous brid zones,” with a wide variety of internal genetic struc-
cladograms. Moreover, the rapid deployment of basal tures and dynamics, typically separate parapatric
hominins across varied African habitats was an ideal allotaxa. Current models attribute the formation and
scenario for producing morphologically undetectable ho- maintenance of allotaxa to rapid pulses of population ex-
moplasy. There seems to be no foolproof way to distin- pansion and contraction to and from refugia, driven by
guish, a priori, homologous from homoplastic resem- late Neogene climatic fluctuations. An overall similarity in
blances in morphology, but one pragmatic strategy is to depth of genetic diversity suggests that papionin taxa
severely censor the datset, retaining only resemblances such as Papio baboons, rather than extant humans, may
or differences (often apparently trivial ones) that cannot present the better analogy for human population struc-
be reasonably explained on the basis of functional re- ture of the “prereplacement” era. Neandertals and Afro-
semblance or difference, respectively. This strategy may Arabian “premodern” populations may have been analo-
eliminate most morpological data, and leave many fossil gous to extant baboon (and macaque) allotaxa:
taxa incertae sedis, but this is preferable to unwar- “phylogenetic” species, but “biological” subspecies. “Re-
ranted phylogenetic confidence. placement,” in Europe, probably involved a rapidly sweep-

©2001 WILEY-LISS, INC.


DOI 10.1002/ajpa.10021
178 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 44, 2001
ing hybrid zone, driven by differential population pressure In general, extant papionin analogies suggest that the
from the “modern” side. Since the genetic outcome of hy- dynamics and interrelationships among hominin popula-
bridization at allotaxon boundaries is so variable, the tions now known only from fossils are likely to have been
problem of whether any Neandertal genes survived the more complex than we are likely to be able to discern
sweep, and subsequent genetic upheavals, is a purely em- frrom the evidence available, and also more complex than
pirical one; if any genes passed “upstream” across the can be easily expressed in conventional taxonomic termi-
moving zone, they are likely to be those conferring local nology. Yrbk Phys Anthropol 44:177–204, 2001.
adaptive advantage, and markers linked to these. © 2001 Wiley-Liss, Inc.

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Homology, Homoplasy, and Analogy ..................................................................................................................... 179


Hominin Origins, Theropithecus, and “Seed-Eaters” ........................................................................................... 181
Homoplasy, Cryptic Symplesiomorphy, and the Diversity of Early Hominins .................................................. 183
Rheboons, Geboons, and Early Hominin Reticulations ........................................................................................ 188
Hybrid Zones and Population Replacements in Baboons and Humans ............................................................. 193
Discussion and Conclusions ................................................................................................................................... 200
Acknowledgments .................................................................................................................................................... 201
Literature Cited ...................................................................................................................................................... 201

To know thyself, look not to apes alone; some troglodytophiles, it should be emphasized at
A proper study for mankind’s— baboon . . . the outset that nothing in this paper is intended to
Alexander Papyoe (with apologies to Alexander Pope)
detract from the unique insights that can be drawn
from studies of the living hominoids, and especially
This paper is expanded from a luncheon talk pre- the extant nonhuman Homininae (“hominine apes”),
sented to the American Association of Physical An- Pan and Gorilla. Its thesis is simply that there is
thropologists in San Antonio, April, 2000. Its title information that is distinct from and ancillary to
and theme recognize the meetings’ host: the South- these—insights that arise not from homology but
west Foundation for Biomedical Research, and the from analogy, using resemblances and differences
outstanding contribution that the Foundation’s ba- that have evolved in a different clade, rather than
boons and scientists have made to anthropological, those resulting from common ancestry.
primatological, and medical knowledge for over 30 Alexander Papyoe suggests that papionins, espe-
years. The title of course parodies Alexander Pope’s cially the African papionins (subtribe Papionina),
dictum, and its conceit was that a hitherto unrecog- are likely to be a particularly fruitful source of use-
nized papionin contemporary—“Alexander Pa- ful analogies for hominin evolution: not simply
pyoe”— claimed equal relevance for his taxon. It is hominin origins, but also diversity, paleoecology,
used here as a peg on which to hang some thoughts and the population structure of all hominins. Unlike
about the use of analogies in understanding human the hominine apes, which are just three species of
evolution. two genera, the African papionins present a complex
It was also during the San Antonio meetings that phylogenetic picture, and include adaptive arrays of
we learned of the death of Professor Sherwood widely different ages, so that a variety of evolution-
Washburn, who, among many other contributions to ary phenomena is exemplified by extant as well as
our field, was among the first to appreciate the rel- extinct taxa. Also unlike the African apes, which as
evance of the large terrestrial monkeys to under- far as is known have always been primarily ever-
standing human evolution’s earlier stages. This pa- green forest dwellers, baboons of several genera
per is therefore dedicated to Sherry Washburn— have shared nonrainforest habitats in sub-Saharan
with respect, affection, and sadness that we cannot Africa with the hominins ever since their respective
enjoy his response to its contents. lineages emerged. Late Neogene climatic and biotic
Its theme is that the members of the tribe Pap- fluctuations that affected hominin distribution, di-
ionini (baboons, macaques, mangabeys, drills, and versity, and adaptations impacted the baboons in
their extinct relatives), and particularly its African parallel ways, rather than inversely, as was presum-
subtribe, Papionina (all but macaques), can bring ably the case for forest-dwelling apes. We can there-
fresh insights to the interpretation of the diversity, fore expect the papionins to provide bio-historical as
adaptations, ecology, and population structure of well as phylogenetic and functional analogues.
species within our own lineage, the tribe Hominini Finally, most extant papionin lineages are now
(⫽ family Hominidae in the older convention). Be- firmly anchored phylogenetically by multiple lines of
cause a comparable argument presented just over 30 molecular evidence (Disotell, 2000), and a reason-
years ago (Jolly, 1970a, 1972a,b) stirred the wrath of able, if approximate, timescale for the whole radia-
Jolly] PAPIONIN ANALOGIES FOR HOMININ EVOLUTION 179
tion is provided by calibrating molecular diver- that homology cannot be used a priori to sort rele-
gences (Tosi et al., 2000; Disotell, personal vant from irrelevant characters for cladistic analy-
communication) against the paleontologically docu- sis, although we can distinguish characters that are
mented first appearance of Theropithecus apomor- likely to be homologous from those that are not. The
phies at about 4.5 ma (Kalb et al., 1982; Delson, definition also ignores some fundamental issues,
1993; Gundling and Hill, 2000). The overall pap- such as the weaselish attributes of verbal character-
ionin chronology is comparable to that of the homi- state descriptions, and the difficulty of defining “in-
nines (Pan-Gorilla-Homo), while the African sub- heritance” in any but a genetic context (and not
clade (Papionina) seems to have diversified within unambiguous, even there) (Cartmill, 1994).
much the same time frame as the hominin subclade If “homology” is used in this way, then its ant-
(Ruvolo et al., 1991; Disotell, personal communica- onym is “homoplasy,” a shared character-state that
tion). is not derived from common ancestry (Lockwood and
After a short examination of the concept of anal- Fleagle, 1999). A homologous resemblance results
ogy, the paper focuses on four areas of recurrent from a common ancestry in which the shared char-
contention where a papionin analogy may make a acter-state appeared by a single evolutionary tran-
contribution: 1) the origins of the hominin ancestral sition; in the ancestry of a homoplastic resemblance
lineage, and specifically, the nature of the first hu- there are at least two such transitions.
man ancestor not shared with any other living pri- Analogy is not identical to homoplasy, although it
mate; 2) homoplasy and the cladistics of “basal” Plio- also involves nonhomologous resemblances. Accord-
Pleistocene hominins; 3) the probability of ing to the Oxford English Dictionary, analogy im-
occasional hybridization and gene flow between plies an “equivalency or likeness of relations,” which
basal hominin lineages; and 4) the status of the is classically stated in the form “A is to B, as X is to
various populations of “archaic” and “premodern” Y” (thus, A is the analogue of X). For example, in his
Homo sapiens, i.e., the Neandertals and their con- discussion of the relationship between “higher men-
temporaries. Each of these topics is of course much tal powers” and cerebral development in human an-
too extensive to be comprehensively reviewed in a cestry, Darwin (1871, p. 54) points out that:
single paper. The most I can hope to do is simply
indicate some areas within them where Alexander “We meet with closely analogous facts in the insects, for in ants
the cerebral ganglia are of extraordinary dimensions, and in all
Papyoe—as spokesmonkey for the baboons— can the Hymenoptera these ganglia are many times larger than in the
suggest fresh viewpoint. less intelligent orders, such as beetles.”
HOMOLOGY, HOMOPLASY, AND ANALOGY
Formally stated, Darwin’s analogy is: “with re-
Although the distinction between homology and spect to both central nervous system complexity
analogy is commonly regarded as one of the funda- and intelligence, ants are to insects, as human
mentals of evolutionary theory, both concepts can be beings are to primates,” and its implication is that
slippery, and have stimulated a considerable body of there is a functional relationship between the two
literature whose analysis is far beyond this essay’s attributes. Darwin is not, of course, suggesting
scope. The following brief discussion of usages is that an ant’s cerebral ganglia (“not so large as the
scarcely original, but will serve to define this paper’s quarter of a pin’s head”) resemble a human brain
stance, which reflects a strong nominalist bias and a in size, structure, function, or any other respect
distrust of reifications. except their unusual size for an insect. This illus-
In an evolutionary context, homology describes an trates the important difference between “analogy”
attribute of individuals (either actual individual or- and “resemblance,” concepts that are often con-
ganisms, or abstracted representations of a taxon). fused.
Structures are often described as homologous (as in Not all analogical propositions are expressed ex-
“the wing of bats is homologous to the human arm,” actly in the “A is to B . . .” format, but they always
or “the wing of bees is not homologous to the wing of include the “likeness of relations.” For example, pa-
bats”), but this usage can be confusing (is a bat’s leontologists often take dental dimensions from two
wing homologous to a pterodactyl’s?). Ambiguity is extant populations (A and B) known to represent
reduced if the term homology is applied not to struc- good species, and use them as a standard by which
tures, but to states of a variable character (“‘posses- to determine whether two species are present in a
sion of a trunk with fore- and hindlimbs’ is a homol- mixed bag of fossil teeth. If X and Y are heaps of
ogous character-state of chiropterans and humans”). fossils sorted by whatever criteria the paleontologist
A homologous character-state is one whose shared chooses, he or she is testing a hypothesis in the form
status derives from inheritance from a common an- of an analogy something like: “when they were alive,
cestor. (In this context, the distinction between synap- the source population of X had the same sort of
omorphies and symplesiomorphies is irrelevant; relation to the source population of Y, as A presently
both are equally homologous, differing only with has to B” (e.g., they were reproductively isolated,
respect to the ancestor from which the shared state ecologically distinct, fully diagnosable, or conformed
is derived.) This definition of homology, of course, to whatever other species criterion is adopted). Sim-
presupposes a known cladogram, and this means ilarly, a paleoecologist might call upon information
180 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 44, 2001
about sympatric chimpanzees and gorillas to under- dwelling stimulated carnivory. It was quite quickly
stand the interactions of large-bodied hominids co- shown, however, that Pan troglodytes practiced op-
existing at a late Miocene site, proposing an analogy portunistic carnivory in other (probably all) habi-
in the form: “N-pithecus related ecologically to M- tats. This discovery suggested that carnivory, or at
pithecus, as chimpanzees today relate ecologically to least the ability to adopt it when advantageous to do
gorillas.” Whereas “homoplasy” connotes any resem- so, was a shared ancestral (homologous) trait of
blance acquired independently, an analogy, in the hominins and chimpanzees. It was a component of
sense used here, is a particular kind of logical trian- the behavioral repertoire of the last common ances-
gulation that uses significant patterning of indepen- tor, but no more likely to be significant to hominin
dently acquired behavioral, morphological, or phys- divergence than any other such trait (unless, of
iological traits to help understand and interpret course, there had been archaeological or anatomical
evolutionary events in adaptational and functional evidence for elaboration of carnivorous behavior in
terms. Not all such traits may be homoplasies in the the stem hominin). The two interpretations thus
usual sense. In the example of analogous species have quite different implications for the context in
distinctness, for instance, diagnosticity and/or ge- which simple, chimpanzee-style hunting appeared,
netic isolation is a condition shared by the ana- and its possible role as a prime mover in hominin
logues, but it would be stretching the usage to call it evolution, but they are easily confounded, and quite
a homoplasy. Conversely, many homoplasies (espe- often are. By contrast, when Schaller and Lowther
cially, but not exclusively, in the primary structure (1969) compared and contrasted the hunting-scav-
of molecules) are certainly the product of pure enging strategies of African carnivores with those of
chance. Lacking any functional significance, such early hominins, there was no doubt that they were
random resemblances are unlikely to be incorpo- using an analogy.
rated into a useful analogy. Darwin’s ant-brain example is unusual in the
Although some useful analogies can be drawn great phylogenetic distance between the analogues.
from close phylogenetic relatives, a close relation- Useful analogies, especially for anatomical function,
ship of (A ⫹ B) to (X ⫹ Y) does not necessarily make are often more readily found among relatives that
for a more illuminating comparison. There seems to are less extremely distant. This fact may bias our
be little justification, for example, for the practice of search for analogies toward closer relatives, but it is
unquestioningly using Pan troglodytes and Gorilla important to note that this bias is purely pragmatic;
gorilla to represent “good species” when attempting the logic of analogy does not require that structures
to deduce alpha taxonomy from a sample of fossil whose function is compared should be “homologous”
hominin skulls or teeth. Chimpanzees and gorillas (in any sense). For example, flightlessness has
are, of course, good species, but they are not sister- evolved many times on small oceanic islands, among
taxa (Ruvolo et al., 1991); the geometry of the homi- both insects and birds; for the cause to be analogous,
nine cladogram as currently understood means that the wings of birds and beetles do not have to be
no pair of hominin species can be as distantly related homologous structures. One could, in fact, argue
as are chimpanzees to gorillas. The chimpanzee-go- that an analogy is all the more powerful if the pairs
rilla comparison is likely to provide a poor analogy of analogues are not closely related. As with Dar-
for sorting out hominin lineages, underestimating win’s ants, the more phylogenetically distant the
the number of newly emerged species present in a analogues, the more striking the coincidence, and
fossil sample. For hominins, or any case where re- the more obvious the fact that a parallel adaptation
cent speciation is possible, a more appropriate com- has occurred, and demands an explanation.
parison is with other clusters of good, but minimally The papionins are ideally situated, phylogeneti-
differentiated species: lemurs of the genus Eulemur, cally, to provide analogies for hominin evolution. As
perhaps, or Cercopithecus monkeys, or dogs of the fellow catarrhines, they share (by homology) many
genus Canis. attributes of hominin structure and function. They
Another drawback with drawing analogies from are basically arboreal (but often secondarily terres-
close relatives is that it can be hard to make the trial) animals that live in permanent, bisexual asso-
important distinction between analogous and ho- ciations, experience their world predominantly via
mologous resemblances. Chimpanzee carnivory is a their sense of vision, and use their hands to feed as
case in point. In the early 1960s, Goodall reported well as to locomote. They use 8 incisor teeth for
the first cases of meat-eating by chimpanzees (Good- initial food preparation, 20 cheek-teeth for chewing,
all, 1986), at Gombe. Most commentators, noting and 4 canines for agonistic interactions. All these
that Gombe is a “savanna” (actually, woodland-mo- homologies comprise a similar groundplan, liable to
saic) habitat, interpreted hunting as an adaptation be modified in similar ways. Yet the papionins are
that appeared separately in early hominins and sa- also far enough removed from the hominins that
vanna chimpanzees as each moved “out of the for- when analogous forces do modify one of the shared
est.” This made it a homoplastic behavior, suggest- catarrhine attributes, producing a parallelism, it is
ing a significant analogy between savanna recognized as such, and is not mistaken for homol-
chimpanzees and supposed savanna-dwelling early ogy. A gelada baboon’s precision-gripping hand, for
hominins, with the strong implication that savanna- instance, resembles a human hand in some of its
Jolly] PAPIONIN ANALOGIES FOR HOMININ EVOLUTION 181
functionally related proportions, but it could never Contemporary interpretations of the hominin apo-
be mistaken for anything other than a cercopithe- morphies, essentially unchanged since Darwin
coid’s. (1871), emphasized not so much their origins as
their functional interdependence. Bipedal stance,
HOMININ ORIGINS, THEROPITHECUS, AND for example, was favored because it freed the hands
“SEED-EATERS” for tool-use, tool-use led to reduction in the relative
size of the incisor and canine teeth, and to special-
The “seed-eater” idea (Jolly, 1970a, 1972a,b) was ization of the hand for tool use and manufacture,
based on an analogy that can be stated as: “[in some which in turn favored more efficient bipedal adap-
respects] Australopithecus is to Pan, as Theropithe-
tations, and so on. As Darwin (1871, p. 51) put it:
cus is to Papio” (it might better have been stated,
“[in some respects] Australopithecus is to the homi- “Man alone has become a biped; and we can, I think, partly see
nine morphotype, as Theropithecus is to the pap- how he has come to assume his erect attitude. . . Man could not
ionin morphotype,” but New York in the late 1960s have attained his present dominant position in the world without
was still in the terminal Prehennigian). My excuse the use of his hands, which are so admirably adapted to act in
obedience to his Will. . . But the hands and arms could hardly
for summarizing this ancient story here is twofold: have become perfect enough to have manufactured weapons, or to
first, it illustrates the use of analogy, and, second, it have hurled stones and spears with a true aim, as long as they
affords an opportunity to correct a few of the misin- were habitually used for locomotion. . . From these causes alone it
terpretations that, barnacle-like, encrust its aging would have been an advantage to man to become a biped. . .”
hulk. A more complete review of the current status
of “Seed-Eaters” will be presented elsewhere. Darwin’s statement borders on teleology, but it is
This analogy was used to construct a novel sce- saved by his cautious insertion of “partly,” and by
nario for the origin of the hominin clade, by explain- his passing reference, earlier on the page, to an
ing, in functional and ecological terms, the fact that antecedent cause, a “change in its manner of procur-
a particular suite of new traits had appeared in the ing subsistence, or to some change in the surround-
earliest undoubted hominin species (at the time, ing conditions” (Darwin, 1871, p. 51)—what today
Australopithecus africanus). It is important to note we would describe, less elegantly, as an ecological
that the analogy, like any such exercise, could not, prime mover, responsible for assembling the ele-
and did not attempt to, address three other, equally ments of the positive feedback system.
important but quite different questions: “Where Paleoanthropologists of the mid-1960s enjoyed a
does Australopithecus fit within the primate phylo- considerable advantage over Darwin in having
gram?”; “What living species most closely resemble much more direct evidence about this prime mover:
Australopithecus?”; and “What anatomical changes in naturalistic studies of wild great apes, in paleo-
separated Australopithecus from its chimpanzee- ecological information about the context of hominin
like ancestor, and what are their functional impli- origins, and especially in the derived anatomy of
cations?” These problems require identification and hominins very close to the stem itself. Very little
functional analysis of Australopithecus apomorphies attention, however, was directed to stem hominin
in the context of the homologous traits shared with anatomy itself as evidence for the nature of hominin
its closest known relatives, i.e., hominine apes and cladogenesis. If the anatomy of basal hominins was
human beings. These are, of course, major, ongoing evaluated functionally (as opposed to phylogeneti-
concerns of paleoanthropology, that continue to pro- cally), it was from the viewpoint of the “path from
duce more refined descriptions and interpretations ape to man,” most commentators apparently assum-
in response to new fossil discoveries, work on living ing that the nature of that path was self-evident,
apes, and the application of more sophisticated com- and that a vaguely imagined move from “forest” to
parative and functional anatomical methods. They “savanna” was sufficient to set the basal hominins
can be tackled only from a base of knowledge of the on their way along it. Basal hominin features were
behavior, physiology, and anatomy of extant homi- evaluated according to their resemblance to humans
nine apes and humans, and they fully justify the or to apes, and human-like conditions were unques-
focus of paleoanthropology upon these species. But tioningly interpreted as evidence for the beginnings
the focus of “Seed-Eaters” was on a different prob- of human-like behavior, e.g., a hand with relatively
lem. At the time, the phyletic position of Australo- short phalanges and a long, powerful, and mobile
pithecus, and the major features of its anatomy, had pollex was attributed to tool-making, even in the
been satisfactorily interpreted (e.g., by Le Gros absence of artifactual evidence.
Clark, 1955), to the extent allowed by the current Besides their tendency to wander into teleology,
state of knowledge. Australopithecus was indeed a such explanations suffer from a lack of generality.
basal hominin. In brain size and overall character- As a reviewer of this paper’s first draft put it, “All
istics, it resembled a hominine ape, but it was a anthropologists are bedeviled by the fact that there
biped (of a sort), with small canine teeth in both is only one living primate (indeed mammal) that is
sexes, and incisor teeth that were small relative to habitually bipedal and that manufactures stone
cheek-teeth. What was not satisfactorily explained tools regularly.” It is indeed the case that if we insist
was why these apomorphies had appeared. that the only relevant species are those that share
182 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 44, 2001
both the adaptations we are trying to interpret, and tions to culture and tool-using. This was true even of
the complete suite of homologous, heritage traits in those, like a habitual bipedal stance and long, pow-
which they are embedded, the only relevant species erful pollex, that were not paralleled in Theropithe-
for explaining the evolution of hominin traits such cus. Instead, it was suggested that all the dental-
as bipedalism are hominins themselves. And since gnathic homoplasies comprised a single functional
all of them (so far as we know) owe their bipedalism complex related to mastication, summarized as
to the same, ancestral event, any explanation for “back-tooth dominance,” and that this, together with
this event, no matter how ingenious, is a “Just So lower-crowned canine teeth that permitted molar
Story,” applicable only to the case from which it is cusps to wear more evenly, postural changes associ-
derived. This problem is not anthropology’s alone; ated with australopithecine-style bipedalism, and a
every evolutionary transition in any species occurs hand with a precise thumb-index pincer and a pal-
against the background of a unique heritage result- mar pocket, could all be related functionally to
ing from an idiosyncratic evolutionary history. “small-hard-object feeding.” Moreover, “small-hard-
Analogy offers a way out of the dilemma by allow- object feeding” also described the most salient de-
ing, in fact insisting, that the feature to be inter- rived feature of the extant Theropithecus gelada’s
preted (habitual truncal erectness supported from ecology, though in its case the food objects requiring
below by the hindlimb, for example) be decoupled thorough mastication were grass blades, rhizomes,
from its context of heritage features. The basal and corms, rather than seeds, and the gelada is a
hominin transition to bipedalism, for instance, can habitual, upright-trunked squatter, rather than a
then be linked to a wider universe of similar events, biped. The stem hominin was presumed to be about
some of which may prove illuminating. We can ask as culture-bearing, artifact-using, and carnivorous
not only “What caused hominins to become bipeds?” as extant chimpanzees, but the staple of its mainly
but rather “What common factors, if any, are asso- vegetarian diet was tough savanna-woodland seeds
ciated with adoption of hands-free, upright posture rather than forest fruits. In the original formulation,
or gait in other vertebrates?” Posed this way, the grass seeds gathered in edaphic grasslands were
question yields a slew of potentially analogous tran- imagined to be the staple. Later (Jolly and Plog,
sitions in the ancestries of clades as varied as thero- 1987), in response to work by Walker (1981) and
pod dinosaurs, kangaroos, and gerenuks. Not all will Peters (1982), as well as my own observations in
prove illuminating, but some may stimulate fresh Ethiopia, I identified the tough but nutritious fruits,
insights. seeds, and pods of thornbush shrubs such as acacias
“Seed-Eaters” found an explanatory analogy in and Grewia as a more likely dietary focus than grass
the many parallels between trends seen in Thero- seed.
pithecus, especially large extinct forms assigned to “Seed-Eaters” also pointed out that Theropithe-
T. oswaldi (Jolly, 1972b), and canonical descriptions cus and Australopithecus each have apomorphies
of Australopithecus africanus. Since A. africanus that are not paralleled in the other, and these too
was at the time the earliest and least derived known are a significant component of the analogy, be-
hominin, its apomorphies were presumed to be cause they differentiate the respective econiches
adaptive reflections of the origin of the hominin of the analogs. For example, Theropithecus has
clade itself. Yet the relatively small incisors, espe- high-crowned, complex, molar teeth functionally
cially of the extinct Theropithecus oswaldi, paral- similar to those of other graminivores (grass-eat-
leled those of Australopithecus, and artifact-use ers). Hominins have low-crowned, thick-enameled
seemed an unlikely explanation.1 Moreover, many molars suitable for milling and crushing. Another
other distinctive, derived features of Australopithe- contrast in apomorphic traits, not apparent in
cus jaws and teeth were also paralleled in Thero- 1970, also underscores a difference between gra-
pithecus, especially the large, extinct species. Some minivory and granivory (seed-eating). Theropithe-
of the Theropithecus-Australopithecus parallelisms cus gelada appears to have a smaller brain than
involved features generally assumed to be functional similarly sized Papio baboons (Martin, 1993),
correlates of tool use or other aspects of culture, while Australopithecus, by some interpretations,
while others did not. A reevaluation of the functional may show modest apomorphic brain enlargement.
implications of all the recognized apomorphies of If confirmed, the difference might reflect a nutri-
Australopithecus concluded that few, if any, of these tional contrast between leguminous seeds eaten
derived traits demanded an explanation as adapta- by basal hominins (high in calories, protein, lipids,
and essential fatty acids) and grassy herbage
eaten by geladas (comparatively low in nutrients
1
“Seed-Eaters”, following the conventional wisdom, assumed that per unit bulk). It might also reflect a difference in
Australopithecus dental proportions differed from those of apes, espe- the distribution of the foods: relatively dispersed
cially chimpanzees, mainly because incisors had become smaller. In and patchy in space and time for savanna-wood-
fact, as Wolpoff (1973) soon showed, cheek-tooth enlargement as much land seeds, more continuous for grasses and herbs
as incisal reduction was the cause of the changed proportions, espe-
cially in the newly described, and even more basal, A. afarensis. This,
in the gelada’s montane grassland.
of course, fitted the dietary scenario even better, and the artifact- I thought that a diet-based explanation for homi-
driven model much more poorly. nin origins was appealingly parsimonious. It ex-
Jolly] PAPIONIN ANALOGIES FOR HOMININ EVOLUTION 183
plained the evolution of the small-hard-object feed- culture-driven econiche, and instead invoking di-
ing adaptations in hominins, the independent etary and habitat shifts as causal factors, “Seed-
evolution of functionally equivalent features in Eaters” represented basal hominins not as prehu-
Theropithecus, and the parallelism between them. mans whose importance was measured purely by
Compared to previous explanations, it covered a how far they had traveled down the road from “ape”
greater variety of derived basal hominid features, to “man,” but as nonhuman primates, subject to the
and linked features not previously considered part of same forces of diversification, speciation, and adap-
the same functional complex (e.g., large molars and tive radiation as other contemporary mammalian
adept hands). It was also economical in proposing that taxa. Since all hominins did not have to lie on the
the initial ape-hominin divergence involved a rela- same adaptive path, the large-jawed, large-toothed
tively small and simple change in dietary emphasis: a “robust” forms, seen as direct derivatives of the first
minimal ecological change of a kind thoroughly famil- phase of hominin evolution, were able to live along-
iar from mammalian paleontology, rather than a one- side the “human” branch, which by this time had
of-a-kind leap into a multifaceted protoculture. built a new, more carnivorous, artefact-based
Not all colleagues were convinced. In some cases, econiche on the seed-eater base.
the problem seems to have been that the critic mis- In the last few years, new discoveries have begun
interpreted “analogy” as “identity,” mentally con- to document just how extensive a radiation actually
nected “model” and “baboon,” and jumped to the occurred among the basal hominins. Two, possibly
conclusion that a living baboon was being used as an three, genera (Ardipithecus, Kenyanthropus, and
avatar of an early hominin. (This is equivalent to the still mysterious Ororin), and three species of
interpreting Darwin’s analogy to mean that humans Australopithecus (A. bahrelghazali, A. anamensis,
have brains like those of ants.) Others seemed to and A. garhi), have joined the roster of hominins of
feel, mistakenly, that the importance and relevance the 6 –1.5-mya period, and the moribund genus
of chimpanzees were being slighted. In yet other Praeanthropus has been revived. (Leakey et al.,
cases, the objection seems to have been based on a 1995, 2001; Asfaw et al., 1999; White et al., 1994;
feeling that an event as momentous as the origin of the Haile-Selassie, 2001; Brunet et al., 1995). Some au-
human lineage must surely have been caused by some- thorities remove one or more lineages of “early
thing more complex and unique than a not-very-pro- Homo” from the genus and assign them to distinct
found change of dietary emphasis—a case, I think, of ramifications (Wood and Collard, 1999; Wolpoff,
the “humans are special” bias that tends to afflict 1999). In part, this multiplication of taxa is a prod-
biological anthropology, and also of confusing a simple uct of shifts in evolutionary and taxonomic philoso-
prime mover with its multifarious consequences. phy and practice. There is less antipathy to propos-
Pending the more extensive review, it bears stat- ing new names in cases where the evidence is
ing, for the record, that “Seed-Eaters” advanced inconclusive, and wider recognition that the shape of
none of the following propositions. Each has been set organic evolution in general is “bushier” than previ-
up as a straw man and gleefully demolished by one ously appreciated. The phylogenetic species concept,
colleague or another: and acceptance of clade-based taxonomic usage that
abhors paraphyletic and “wastebasket” taxa, have
1) that Australopithecus (or hominins in general) tended to multiply recognized species. As always,
were more closely related, phylogenetically, to some newly named hominin forms may ultimately
baboons than to chimpanzees. disappear into synonymy. Nevertheless, even allow-
2) that Australopithecus ate grass like a gelada. ing for shifting taxonomic fashion and uncertainties
3) that Theropithecus is a “seed-eating baboon.” in alpha taxonomy, it is clear that much of the newly
4) that basal hominins inhabited “vast, dry sa- described diversity represents biological reality.
vanna grasslands” (Spenser, 1997, p. 201). Moreover, most of this “new” diversity has been rec-
5) that the “Seed-Eater” idea stood or fell with the ognized in hominins from a single broad eco-geo-
hominin status of Ramapithecus. graphic zone: the northern savanna-woodland belt,
6) that Australopithecus ate only seeds. and especially its extension in eastern equatorial
7) that all the derived features of Australopithecus Africa. Much of the rich basal hominin material
were paralleled in Theropithecus, or vice versa. from South Africa, which derives from very different
8) that Theropithecus resembled early hominins ecological and geographical settings (Bromage and
more closely than it did Papio baboons or other Schrenk, 1999), has yet to be fully described and
monkeys. interpreted within the current, more schizophilic
9) that male T. gelada have small canine teeth. taxonomic climate. It can safely be predicted that
10) that Australopithecus (or any other early homi- even more basal hominin genera and species will
nin) resembled a Theropithecus baboon more ultimately be recognized.
closely overall than it did African apes, specifi-
HOMOPLASY, CRYPTIC SYMPLESIOMORPHY,
cally, the chimpanzee.
AND THE DIVERSITY OF EARLY HOMININS
By explicitly decoupling the origins and early evo- The recognition of multiple lineages early in homi-
lution of hominins from the concept of an expansive, nin history raises the methodological question of
184 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 44, 2001
how their cladistic and phylogenetic relationships the one hand, and the two groups of mangabeys on
can be retrieved from the distribution of morpholog- the other.
ical character-states. The extant papionins, for Other homoplasies, such as those affecting facial
which we have a reliable molecular phylogeny, pro- length (and correlated characters) in the African
vide an informative, if not altogether optimistic, papionins, can be attributed to a less obvious but
analogy. probably even more powerful phenomenon: ances-
Opinions differ as to whether molecular data are tral patterns of relative growth that independently
best considered independently, or combined with produce similar phenotypes in animals of compara-
morphology, in phylogenetic reconstruction (Kluge, ble overall size. The relationship between facial and
1989). As molecular data become more comprehen- cranial length in Papionini is an extreme case of
sive, and methods of analysis more sophisticated, positive allometric growth, and was one of the ear-
the case for building molecular trees independent of liest to be expressed mathematically (Huxley, 1932).
other evidence becomes compelling, because they In overall skull proportions, the adults of small pa-
provide a powerful heuristic tool: a map on which to pionin species resemble the juveniles of larger ones,
plot the evolution of morphological character-states while the adults of large-skulled forms (mandrills,
(Collard and Wood, 2000). This method assumes geladas, and Papio baboons) all have relatively long
that the molecular tree accurately represents phy- faces. If the developmental relationship itself (rath-
logeny, an assumption adopted in the following dis- er than its size-determined phenotypic expression in
cussion, without implying that present molecular adult skulls) is considered the character-state, it is a
datasets are adequate, or that current analytical plesiomorphy of papionins, and the homoplasy that
methods are infallible. links baboons and mandrills is not facial length but
The accepted molecular phylogeny of the papionin absolute skull (and body) size. Whether long faces
radiation (e.g., Harris and Disotell, 1998; Disotell, are labeled a phenotypic homoplasy, or a develop-
1996; summarized in Disotell, 2000) reproduces mental plesiomorphy, the trait is equally misleading
many of the features of morphology-based trees (e.g., and phylogenetically uninformative. It is interesting
Strasser and Delson, 1987), but differs in linking to note that a truly derived state of the morphoge-
Papio with Lophocebus, and Mandrillus with Cerco- netic character can be recognized among papionins,
cebus. This particular discrepancy is especially sig-
but not in the long-faced baboons. It occurs in some
nificant because the molecular phylogeny disman-
Sulawesi macaques (Macaca nigra and close rela-
tles two groups traditionally considered single
tives). Their relative facial length is baboon-like, but
genera: large, long-faced, terrestrial or semiterres-
because it is combined with a much smaller, ma-
trial “baboons” (Papio, s.l., becomes Papio ⫹ Man-
caque-sized skull, it implies that a shift in relative
drillus), and smaller, short-faced, arboreal “manga-
beys” (Cercocebus, s.l., becomes Cercocebus ⫹ growth trajectory occurred in the ancestry of this
Lophocebus). We should note in passing that al- particular subclade of macaques (Jolly, 1965; Al-
though it is the papionin case, involving higher pri- brecht, 1977).
mates, that has caught the attention of anthropolo- Another lesson to be drawn from the papionins is
gists, its message—that homoplasies are more how completely the combined effects of true ho-
frequent, and harder to detect morphologically, than moplasy and unrecognized morphogenetic symplesi-
we ever believed— has become almost a cliché in omorphy can obscure real phylogenetic relation-
general evolutionary biology. Every issue of the rel- ships, and powerfully support false ones. We
evant journals seems to include a paper or two sometimes tend to assume, I think, that detailed
(many by morphologists, physiologists, or ecologists) resemblance in complex anatomical features is suf-
that not only revises a cladogram on the basis of ficient evidence for true synapomorphy, because
molecular data, but also uses the resulting homopla- such complexity is unlikely to be duplicated in evo-
sies to construct informative analogies based on the lution. The papionin case illustrates this argument’s
homoplastic acquistion of functionally related traits. flaw: as morphological genetic work is documenting
As a test case for morphological cladistics, the more and more clearly, structural and genetic com-
mangabey-mandrill-baboon case nicely exemplifies plexity are not closely correlated. Though changes in
two major kinds of misleading, nonapomorphic re- shape may be complex in the sense that many struc-
semblance. Whether the baboon or the mangabey tures (and an almost infinite number of possible
character-states are considered ancestral, the re- metrics) are affected, they nevertheless can be du-
vised phylogeny implies major homoplasies in both plicated in parallel evolution, presumably because
skull form and postcranial features. Some of these their genetic basis is comparatively simple. In fact,
were evidently produced by conventional ho- it is not hard to envision an array of related forms in
moplasy: parallel adaptation to similar function. which great diversity in phenotypic characters is
These traits include the degree of posterior angula- determined by the differential expression of a few
tion of the ulnar olecranon process, which is related simple developmental symplesiomorphies. Here, the
to the proportion of climbing and terrestrial locomo- real apomorphies would be the factors underlying
tion in the animal’s locomotor profile (Jolly, 1967), the differential expression of the developmental pat-
and unites semiterrestrial mandrills and baboons on terns; they themselves would be excellent candi-
Jolly] PAPIONIN ANALOGIES FOR HOMININ EVOLUTION 185
dates for cladistic analysis, but their detection is ing characters uncritically to a mechanical cladistic
still in its infancy. analysis is not the solution. The new characters are
As Collard and Wood (2000) recently demon- unlikely to be functionally and developmentally in-
strated, the formal, computer-aided application of dependent of the existing list, resulting in still
cladistic logic to large suites of morphological char- larger bundles of covariant homoplasies, and even
acters is no more likely to give the correct (i.e., stronger statistical support for spurious phylog-
molecule-concordant) answer than is the kind of in- enies. The problem is not too little information, but
tuitive, seat-of-the-pants analysis favored by many too much that is misleading, so pruning the dataset
paleontologists and morphologists. Like some tradi- may be as productive as augmenting it.
tional, premolecular classifications, the PAUP anal- Is it even possible to identify the informative
yses of Collard and Wood (2000) suggest that man- traits and cut away the misleading ones, without a
drills and Papio baboons are sister taxa, and unite molecular crib-sheet? In the mandrill-mangabey
the great apes to the exclusion of Homo. Both mor- case, “good” characters were comparatively few, sub-
phologists and tree-building computer programs are tle, and not concentrated in any one tissue or struc-
misled because the basic logic of parsimony cannot ture. Apart from the dental features noted by
detect cases where an erroneous tree is overwhelm- Fleagle and McGraw (1999), many of them were
ingly supported by suites of unrecognized, correlated irrelevant with respect to function. Obviously, a
homoplasies, and by developmental symplesiomor- more accurate tree would result if homoplasies were
phies concealed by allometry. Parsimony-based sys- excluded at the outset, but there is no foolproof way
tems are designed to recognize clades by concordant, of doing this. The correct cladogram can be built
shared, derived character-states. Suites of pheno- (even by PAUP) only if homoplasies are identified
typic similarities resulting from functional ho- and excluded, but homoplasies are defined by their
moplasy and cryptic symplesiomorphy are not syna- discordance with the correct cladogram.
pomorphic, but they are both derived and shared, While we cannot reliably identify homoplasies by
and they are often distributed concordantly, as “bun- the definitional criterion, we can spot and exclude
dles” or complexes. data that are most likely to be affected by homoplasy
Having shown convincingly that PAUP produces (Haszpruner, 1998). Such data-pruning has become
statistically robust but spectacularly inaccurate unfashionable with the ascendancy of parsimony-
trees when applied to an extensive set of standard based computerized methods, but is implicit in much
craniodental measures, Collard and Wood (2000) intuitive phylogeny building. One strategy devalues
suggested that craniodental data may be intrinsi- characters that are likely to covary because they are
cally inadequate: a gloomy prospect for the paleon- components of a single functional system, identify-
tologists who must rely on such data for phyloge- ing them intuitively (Skelton and McHenry, 1992),
netic reconstruction. Collard and Wood (2000) do not or by using statistical measures of association
sort their characters by utility (as measured by con- (Strait, 2001), and collapsing them into a single vari-
cordance with the molecular phylogeny), but it able to be entered into the analysis. The whole
seems likely those (such as facial length) that are “small-hard-object-feeding” complex might, for ex-
affected by cryptic symplesiomorphy were highly in- ample, be collapsed to a single entity. This will not
fluential. (It should be noted that the analytic remove all homoplasies, but it will ensure that each
method of Collard and Wood (2000) for eliminating functionally or morphogenetically determined deter-
the influence of overall size as a factor in the anal- mined homoplastic “bundle” that is identified con-
ysis also effectively concealed size-related effects.) tributes no more than one data point to the analysis.
The situation may, however, be less hopeless than (It will, of course, also limit the contribution of true
Collard and Wood (2000) imply. We know, after all, synapomorphies, such as those identified by Fleagle
that the morphological data do contain the neces- and McGraw (1999), that comprise, or might com-
sary information, because an informed eye can dis- prise, functional complexes.)
cern the correct story. Primed by strong molecular A complementary, somewhat more radical proce-
hints of the Cercocebus-Lophocebus dichotomy (Cro- dure is to produce a matrix of intertaxon concor-
nin and Sarich, 1976; Hewitt-Emmett et al., 1976), dances, using all available data without regard to
Groves (1978) was able to find cranial, dental, and descriptive level, from the physiological to the gross
soft-part characters that supported it. Similarly, morphological, splitting and rewording comparisons
dental and postcranial character states linking Cer- to make the analysis as comprehensive as possible.
cocebus with Mandrillus, and Papio with Lophoce- This dataset can then be rigorously censored, ex-
bus, could be discerned (Fleagle and McGraw, 1999), cluding any resemblances or differences that can be
once morphologists knew to look for them. Moreover, plausibly explained in terms of documented resem-
once identified, the phylogeny-concordant traits blances or differences in behavior, natural history,
could then be plausibly interpreted in terms of ecol- or morphogenetic processes (Jolly, 1970b). For ex-
ogy and behavior. ample, geladas are close to humans in their “oppos-
If, as this suggests, phylogenetically meaningful ability index,” an expression of the relative lengths
morphological traits are concealed in a mass of un- of thumb and index finger, presumably related func-
informative or misleading information, simply add- tionally to an efficient precision grip (Napier, 1980).
186 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 44, 2001
As a trait, the simple resemblance in proportions there is a component in their degree of resemblance
obviously tells the wrong phylogenetic story (geladas that is “nonadaptive.” In fact, across all extant ca-
and humans are not sister-taxa), but would be ex- tarrhines, no dietary variable explains the dichoto-
cluded because it is easily explained by functional mous distribution of the two molar types, so its
parallelism (parallel adaptation to a particular kind relevance must be “historical”—it can be presumed
of manipulation). It can, however, be broken down to date to the initial divergence of hominoid and
by considering the relative lengths of the digits as cercopithecoid stocks. There is nothing original
separate characters. The gelada achieves its effi- about this way of looking at morphology: it is what
cient opposability by shortening its index finger, Darwin implied when he wrote (1871, p. 153):
while humans have a relatively long thumb. This
difference does carry phylogenetic weight (in this “it appears more correct to pay great attention to the many small
case, against relationship), because the function of resemblances, in giving a truly natural [i.e., phylogeny-based, in
current usage] classification.”
the two mechanisms is closer than their structural
resemblance. It therefore points to two separate Unfortunately, Darwin’s insight in this respect
pathways for achieving the same functional end, has tended to be overshadowed, for morphologists in
and, hence, to separate ancestries: one knuckle- general, and physical anthropologists in particular,
walking, the other digitigrade quadrupedal. by an equally important, but quite distinct, evolu-
Phylogenetically weighty differences and resem- tionary generalization: that major adaptive radia-
blances are also exemplified by the very close and tions have often been founded on major, functionally
detailed similarity in shape of female sexual swell- important changes, the transition from a “monkey-
ing that unites Cercocebus with Mandrillus, and like” to an “apelike” forelimb morphology being a
Lophocebus with Papio (Hill, 1970). Resemblances frequently cited example.
based on the character “sexual swelling occurs/is
The same process (of attempting to spot the “non-
absent” are of low phyletic weight, because the fea-
adaptive,” and therefore phylogenetically weighty,
ture is related functionally to social structure and
aspects of resemblance and difference) should also
mating strategy. But, absent evidence to the con-
be applied to the analysis of fossil forms, but here
trary, we can presume that any of the swelling
the logic is necessarily even less direct. We cannot
shapes seen among African papionin species would
evaluate structural resemblance directly against
perform as signals. To the extent that the resem-
blance of Cercocebus to Mandrillus, and Papio to functional similarity, so we have once again to call
Lophocebus, is closer than function demands, the upon analogy to ask whether, from our knowledge of
shape and position of sexual swellings may be phy- extant cases, such structural resemblances and dif-
logenetically significant. By similar logic, the gela- ferences seem to arise frequently, in similar ecolog-
da’s pectoral sexual skin, though a radical departure ical circumstances, and in comparable combina-
from the papionin norm, loses its phyletic weight tions.
when it is interpreted as a functional correlate of All this is obviously tortuous, subjective, and plau-
squat-feeding. sibility-based; but it is surely a process that any
Yet another example is provided by cercopithecoid experienced evolutionary morphologist routinely ap-
bilophodonty (as opposed to “hominoid” molar struc- plies to judgments about what an organism “really
ture) (Jolly, 1970b). Suppose that all we knew of the is,” usually without making it explicit. No contem-
anatomy of (say) Hylobates lar, Cercopithecus asca- porary paleontologist, given only a posterior man-
nius, and Nasalis larvatus was their dentition. As- dibular fragment and an auditory bulla, would in-
suming that character-states could be unambigu- terpret Archaeolemur as a monkey with a lemuroid
ously defined, a matrix of resemblances would auditory region, rather than a lemur with cerco-
produce some (Hylobates ⫹ Cercopithecus) charac- pithecoid-like molars. Some such mental process as
ter-states, others uniting Cercopithecus and Nasalis, I have described must underlie this interpretation,
and possibly some (Nasalis ⫹ Hylobates). Rather unless we resort to pre-Darwinian notions of “arche-
than proceeding directly to cladistic analysis, the type,” or the idea that some structures or features
“pruning” method would then evaluate each resem- are in some way more “fundamental” than others,
blance or difference against resemblances or dif- and therefore more reliable indicators of phylogeny.
fernces in natural history. We would ask, for in- The logic of pruning or censoring the data in this
stance, whether documented dietary differences way is similar to that of molecular systematists
between Hylobates and Cercopithecus are sufficient when they collect their data base by base, and then
to explain the four-cusp/five-cusp contrast in lower narrow their analysis to third bases and introns, but
molars. Having decided that they are not, we deduce there is an important methodological difference. The
that this character-state difference probably has molecular biologist excludes or devalues first and
phylogenetic weight: it reflects “ancient” evolution- second nucleotides, or whole coding regions of genes,
ary events. The same conclusion would be drawn by generically, and can justify this procedure both em-
comparing cusp number and diet in Cercopithecus pirically (it gives results that are internally consis-
and Nasalis: in this character, their molars are more tent) and theoretically (introns and third bases have
similar than their contemporary diets demand; fewer epigenetic effects, and their variation is there-
Jolly] PAPIONIN ANALOGIES FOR HOMININ EVOLUTION 187
fore less likely to be driven by natural selection). be debated (Strait et al., 1997). In the present state
Precensoring morphological data, on the other hand, of knowledge, these judgments can only be made
cannot, in the present state of knowledge, be simi- individually, case by case. We can only hope that, as
larly generic. A moment’s reflection tells us that it morphological datasets are tested against molecular
will not work if we try to generalize about the phy- phylogenies in more vertebrate taxa, and functional
logenetic information content of structures (“teeth complexes are identified by the recognition of signif-
are better indicators of phylogeny than postcrania”), icant homoplasies, and as the genetics of morpho-
of characters (“molar cusp number is a good phylo- genesis become better understood, regularities will
genetic indicator”), or even of character-states (“bilo- emerge that will make the weighting process less
phodonty reliably identifies primate clades”). The haphazard.
evaluation has to be applied to particular concor- As the gelada example shows, censoring will fre-
dances and discordances in character-states (“the quently remove many hard-won morphological data
shared bilophodonty of Nasalis and Cercopithecus is from the analysis as phylogenetically uninforma-
a good indicator that they are more closely related to tive. Worse yet, some real synapomorphies will be
each other than either is to Hylobates”). discarded, because they are function-based. For ex-
This “pruning” of the data matrix does not replace ample, the dental features (e.g., large, broad, P3)
cladistic analysis. Phylogenetically weighty resem- linking Cercocebus and Mandrillus are actually con-
blances and differences can be either apomorphic cordant with molecular phylogeny, and are probably
(bilophodont molars shared by cercopithecoids) or synapomorphies, but they would be explicable be-
plesiomorphic (“hominoid” molars among eucata- cause they are plausibly explicable as dietary spe-
rrhines) and require the usual Hennigian analysis, cializations (Fleagle and McGraw, 1999), and thus
by whatever logical scheme is preferred: either could have arisen independently. Losing the “real”
brute-force, parsimony-based methods (where the phylogeny by excluding some true synapomorphies
data are numerous, as in molecular phylogenies), or is, I think, preferable to supporting an erroneous one
methods that use inferred character-state polarity by including homoplasies. Uncertainty, which stim-
(preferred by many morphologists). ulates further investigation, is always preferable to
The process of identifying and eliminating poten- unwarranted confidence.
tially misleading characters, and identifying useful This uncertainty will be greatest for fossil taxa,
ones, is the mirror-image of the analogy-building especially those known only from fragmentary re-
process discussed above. To discern functional com- mains. It must be recognized that many such taxa
plexes, analogues are analyzed by subtracting their will inevitably be left in a cladistic limbo, because all
respective heritages, leaving the informative paral- their scorable characters have been rejected. Some
lelisms. To obtain phylogenetic information, the may consider this a weakness; others will prefer it to
common features that might have functional signif- cladograms that are statistically well-supported by
icance are subtracted, hopefully leaving a residue of problematic data.
informative heritage characters. Because the basis In fact, it seems realistic to suppose that there will
of censoring is the functional and developmental be frequent cases in nature where internodal dis-
interpretation of characters, judgments may be tances are so short, and adaptive radiation so fast
changed radically by new information on behavior, and frequent, that homoplasy in adaptive features is
function, and morphogenesis. For example, before virtually complete, leaving little or no morphological
naturalistic studies of gelada baboons (Crook and information to indicate the true phylogeny. While
Aldrich-Blake, 1968) showed them to be specialist, the “real” clades in such groups might be revealed by
bottom-shuffling grazers, their many anatomical pe- analysis of fast-evolving, selectively neutral molec-
culiarities seemed to betoken a long, independent ular markers, recognizably nonhomoplastic, mor-
evolutionary history and great phylogenetic dis- phological indications of the correct relationships
tance from other baboons (Leakey and Whitworth, may be so few, obscure, and trivial, that they will
1958; Jolly, 1966), but this interpretation was im- stand little chance of being found, especially in fossil
mediately invalidated when it was shown that most, material.2
and perhaps all, gelada autapomorphies formed a The basal hominin radiation as a whole may be
functional complex that could be related to unique just such a “difficult” group. Internode intervals
features of present-day gelada ecology (Jolly, must be relatively short, because successive clado-
1970b).
As a formal procedure, however, precensoring
faces some formidable difficulties. It depends en- 2
Note that this scenario does not imply that speciation in these
tirely on reliable information about functional anat- cases is caused by the accumulation of the trivial, nonadaptive mor-
omy and morphogenetic patterns in the group con- phological features whose distribution would be good phylogenetic
cerned, which are then used to make ad hoc, evidence. Reproductive isolation presumably would have resulted
qualitative estimates of phylogenetic information from any combination of the usually postulated factors: differential
ecological adaptation, chance or adaptive fixation of behavioral, phys-
content. Functional and morphogenetic interpreta- iological, or genetic barriers, and so on. Of these, the first would be
tions are generally supported by arguments from discounted because of possible homoplasy, and the others would be
plausibility rather than statistics, and will probably invisible in fossile evidence.
188 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 44, 2001
genetic events are constrained to a total of only an analytic run (Strait et al., 1997) explicitly omit-
about 3– 4 mya between the well-established Pan- ting characters considered (Skelton and McHenry,
Hominini divergence at 6 – 8 mya, which must ante- 1992) to be related to the “heavy-chewing” complex.
date any intra-Hominini divergence, as well as the Yet in spite of this impressive support, the evidence
paleontologically documented time (say, 3.5 ma) at for Paranthropus monophyly has been cogently chal-
which multiple hominin lineages are documented or lenged (McCollum, 1999), on the grounds that all the
inferred. Though a “young” radiation, basal homi- cranial resemblances, some of which are only indi-
nins were geographically widespread, with ranges rectly related to the large-back-tooth, heavy-chew-
eventually extending across much of tropical and ing complex, boil down to a single functional pack-
subtropical Africa. Since each new, clade-founding age with a relatively simple morphogenetic base. If
species presumably originated in a limited home- this interpretation is correct, the shared features in
land, rapid population expansion across suitable Paranthropus species might be true synapomor-
habitats and resultant genetic structuring of the phies, but because they can also be explained by a
expanding population (Templeton, 1998) must have combination of functional parallelism and allomor-
been influential. Furthermore, judging by the distri- phosis, they carry little or no phylogenetic weight. It
bution of contemporary African mammals (Kingdon, remains to be seen how Paranthropus monophyly
1997), suitable hominin habitats were discontinu- will fare against the alternative hypothesis (parallel
ously distributed, with the edaphic flood-plains and derivation of “robust” morphs from separate “nonro-
other broad, habitable, nonrainforest areas con- bust” ancestries) if all morphogenetically related
nected by narrow corridors through less hospitable characters as well as functional complexes are rig-
forests and highlands. The East African transequa- orously excluded from the analysis. The work of
torial savanna-woodland zone was surely a critical McCollum (1999) hints that a few nonfunctionally
pathway between northern and southern hominin correlated dental traits might remain to tell the true
habitats (Bromage and Schrenk, 1999), but as King- story.
don (personal communication) has pointed out, ma- The papionin analogy, then, suggests the general
jor rivers flowing from forested highlands eastwards principle that we should not rely too heavily on even
to the Indian Ocean presented hurdles for expand- the most formally rigorous and apparently well-sup-
ing hominin populations. The first population to ported early hominin cladograms (Strait et al.,
reach a pristine, extensive, and ecologically varied 1997), since, unless the data are prescreened and
stretch of nonforest habitat (such as the southern censored, much of the apparent cladistic structure
African temperate and subtropical grasslands) may be an artifact of functional and developmental
would undergo population expansion, and would homoplasy. But there is yet another source of phy-
also be faced with varied ecological opportunities. logenetic noise to be considered.
Adaptive radiation might well occur within each
RHEBOONS, GEBOONS, AND EARLY
area, with different morphs in the array duplicating
HOMININ RETICULATIONS
those that had appeared in other regions. This is a
phenomenon that is well-documented among extant A major assumption of most studies of the homi-
organisms, with most cases being unsuspected until nin fossil evidence is that relationships among diag-
documented by molecular phylogeny (Schluter and nosable species- or genus-level taxa are accurately
Nagel, 1995; Schluter, 2001; Taylor and McPhail, represented by dendrograms rather than reticula-
1999). The resultant patterning of morphological tions. Cladogenesis is a clean break, in which com-
traits is the least propitious for reconstructing phy- plete genetic isolation between sister lineages is es-
logeny, because internodes representing common tablished either before paleontologically recog-
ancestry, during which informative, synapomorphic nizable ecological and phenotypic divergence, or so
trivia can accumulate, are relatively short, and soon afterwards that the interval between is incon-
adaptive radiations, which spawn homoplasies, are sequential on a geological timescale. This assump-
frequent. tion is almost essential for conventional cladistic
Among basal hominins, the problem is most analysis, which cannot easily accommodate reticu-
clearly exemplified by the taxa usually assigned to lation.
Paranthropus (the South African P. robustus, in- Various lines of evidence, however, suggest that
cluding P. crassidens; and the East African P. boisei this assumption may not be altogether valid for the
and P. aethiopicus). They share relatively large mo- papionins, and perhaps by implication, in hominins
lars, relatively small incisors, very large masticatory also. Papionins show a remarkable ability to hybrid-
muscles, a deep mandibular ramus, and a strongly ize (“crossability”). Hybridization, in this context, is
buttressed facial structure. The generic grouping, defined as the production of offspring by interbreed-
implying a shared and exclusive ancestry, is re- ing of members of genetically differentiated popula-
garded as one of the most firmly established within tions (Barton and Hewitt, 1985). The definition de-
Hominini (Grine, 1988), and was supported consis- liberately does not specify the level of taxonomic
tently in an exhaustive, PAUP-driven cladistic anal- separation of the interbreeding populations. Among
ysis of hominin phylogeny (Strait et al., 1997). In papionins, as in many extant taxa, hybridization
fact, the Paranthropus clade was supported even by occurs across a wide spectrum of taxonomic levels.
Jolly] PAPIONIN ANALOGIES FOR HOMININ EVOLUTION 189
At one extreme, we have rare, unusually artificial, however, to the gelada herd, which suggests that
inter-subtribe crossing, and at the other extreme, they may have been backcrosses to that species
the formation of natural hybrid zones between dif- (Jolly et al., 1997).
ferentiated, parapatric populations of the same, or The genus Macaca is commonly divided into spe-
very closely related, species. Although we might be cies-groups, which are sometimes given subgeneric
tempted to divide this spectrum at the conventional rank (Fooden, 1976). These groups appear to be phy-
“species level” (assuming, of course, that we could logenetically valid, and are ecologically distinct to
agree upon what that is), this would be an artificial the extent that species of different groups often have
and unhelpful division of a continuum. The present overlapping ranges, while species of the same group
discussion concerns crossing between taxa at the do not. The time of divergence between macaque
upper end of the continuum, between taxa that are species-groups is not well-documented paleontologi-
normally allocated to different subtribes, genera, cally, but from molecular evidence (Tosi et al., 2000),
subgenera, or species-groups. is probably in the 3– 4-ma range. Bernstein (1966,
The most phylogenetically distant pair of pap- 1968) documented natural hybrids between mem-
ionins to have produced a well-attested, viable off- bers of sympatric species belonging to two different,
spring are rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) and well-defined species groups, Macaca fascicularis
baboon (Papio) (phylogenetic separation ⫾ 10 ma). (mulatta-group) and M. nemestrina (silenus-group).
Twenty-six offspring (“rheboons”) were produced at It was suggested that interbreeding occurred be-
the Southwest Foundation, of which one male, when cause female M. nemestrina joined a M. fasicularis
adult, was subjected to detailed karyotypic and re- troop after local extermination of male M. nemest-
productive evaluation (Moore et al., 1999). This an- rina (Bernstein, 1966). The hybrids apparently func-
imal was considered behaviorally abnormal (J. Rog- tioned well as normal, long-term members of a M.
ers, personal communication), and histological fascicularis social group (Bernstein, 1968).
examination showed him to be sterile, although, sig- Assuming that in these cases some interspecies-
nificantly, no mismatching of his parental haploid group or intergeneric hybridization, with survival
chromosome sets could be detected. We can presume and backcrossing by at least some of the hybrids,
that if rhesus monkeys (or other macaques) and occurs in the wild, what evolutionary impact can it
Papio baboons were sympatric in the wild, most if have? Can significant effects be ruled out, given that
not all hybrids produced are most unlikely to breed, the two parental populations overlap widely in
and probably would not survive. range, remain completely distinct phenotypically
The best-documented recent case of hybridization and ecologically, and, in the case of Papio and Thero-
between papionin genera is that between Papio and pithecus, are known to have done so for several
Theropithecus (Jolly et al., 1997) (phylogenetic sep- million years?
aration, ⫾ 5 ma). When caged together, they readily Presuming that some of the hybrids produced are
hybridize (Markarjan et al., 1974; Jolly et al., 1997; at least partially viable and fertile, the effect of
J. Rogers and T. Newman, personal communica- sporadic hybridization is to introduce a trickle of
tion), producing F1 offspring, i.e., “geboons.” In cap- genes from one population to the other, the direction
tivity, female F1 geboons are certainly able to func- of flow guided largely by sex-specific, inter-species
tion socially, and to attract Papio males, are both interactions. In the Papio ⫻ Theropithecus gelada
viable and fertile, and produce viable backcross off- case, for example, F1 females would probably re-
spring with Papio males. In fact, an F1 female was main in the baboon troop where they were born, and
mated by a hamadryas male who also had access to presumably would then mate with resident male
hamadryas (and anubis and hybrid) females in the Papio baboons to produce backcross offspring. Male
cage. The resulting female backcross (3/4 Papio F2s would presumably emigrate, and females would
grandparents) (Jolly et al., 1997), when last ob- remain in their mothers’ group. With each genera-
served, was still alive and healthy at age 5, but there tion of backcrossing, the admixture would become
was no indication that she had bred. The fertility of less phenotypically visible. The backcross Papio ⫻
F1 males is also unproven. In parts of Ethiopia, (Theropithecus ⫻ Papio) female produced in Bihere
geladas coexist with anubis or hamadryas baboons Tsige Zoo in Addis Ababa, when observed as a young
(Papio anubis and Papio hamadryas), and occa- adult, was phenotypically very close to a normal
sional natural hybridization is suspected (Dunbar hamadryas female. Her 0.25 gelada inheritance
and Dunbar, 1974). The case reported by Dunbar would not be readily apparent in a wild hamadryas
and Dunbar (1974) occurred in an area that sup- group. Similarly, in the Macaca nemestrina ⫻ M.
ported balanced populations of both species, and fascicularis case, hybrid females would presumably
human disturbance was no more severe than in remain in their mothers’ troop, while males would
most of the gelada’s range. Here, “bachelor” males of disperse to spread their “foreign” genes to other
T. gelada, a harem-holding species, were seen mat- groups.
ing with young female Papio anubis, a species in All this is, of course, quite speculative. The critical
which females are philopatric and polyandrous, and field genetic studies to detect introgression in the
fully adult males are only weakly attracted to sympatric populations of baboons and geladas in
subadult females. The probable hybrids belonged, Ethiopia, and macaques of different species-groups
190 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 44, 2001
in overlap areas of east Asia, have yet to be carried equids, or canids), among which hybrids are com-
out. Parallel cases in other vertebrates, however monly produced between parental species of equiv-
(e.g., Goodman et al., 1999; Lehman et al., 1991; alent degrees of evolutionary divergence.
Arntzen and Wallis, 1991), suggest that cryptic in- One potential objection is that some of these cases
trogression of genetic markers is likely to be much of hybridization, especially those between relatively
more widespread than suggested by the rate of pri- distant parental species (Gee, 1999), occurred in
mary hybridization, or observable phenotypic hy- situations that were entirely artificial (such as in a
bridity. For example, where native red deer (Cervus cage), or where human influence was strongly sus-
elaphus) are sympatric with introduced sika (Cervus pected. As has been noted for many other taxa (e.g.,
nippon) in Britain, only 0.1– 0.2% of matings were Carr et al., 1986; Lehman et al., 1991; Struhsaker et
cross-specific, yet 66% of phenotypic sika, and 33% al., 1988), interspecific hybridization is most likely
of phenotypic red deer, carried allelic evidence of when sex ratios in one or both species are locally
mixed ancestry (Goodman et al., 1999). Presumably, biased. This situation may be an effect of human
this indicates that prezygotic barriers become less disturbance or hunting, but it can also occur natu-
effective as backcrossing progresses, and maladap- rally, especially on the edge of the range of one
tive genetic combinations are weeded out by selec- species. In most papionin societies, wandering bach-
tion. elor males are a perennial feature. An emigrant
As a source of new variation for the recipient male in a marginal population might find few con-
population, genes entering by hybridization are specific mates, and would therefore try to mate with
somewhat comparable to mutations. Unlike muta- any sympatric females with which he shared enough
tions, however, immigrant genes have been tested in of his mate-recognition system. Cross-taxon mating
a donor population. They may be disadvantageous in is presumably rare even when conditions favor it,
their new genetic and ecological setting, but are and favorable conditions are themselves relatively
unlikely to be lethal, and in general the chance of unusual. Nevertheless, they are not unrealistic, and
their being advantageous must be better than for can be expected to occur even without human inter-
random mutations. Moreover, whereas advanta- vention, especially when environments are unsta-
geous, random mutations are most unlikely to recur ble. Even situations mimicking the extreme situa-
in the same population and the same form, hybrid- tion of a zoo colony, bringing together a few
ization will present a steady, even if slow, supply of individuals of species that can interbreed, but rarely
identical “immigrant” alleles for selection in the re- or never meet in the wild, are quite conceivable
cipient population. where species ranges are fragmenting or adjusting
Most immigrant genes that are neutral in effect rapidly to climatically or tectonically driven environ-
will disappear by drift in a few generations, though mental shifts. If it were not for the barriers created
a random minority will persist or even increase. by people, for example, a relatively slight climatic-
Natural selection will presumably remove genes de- vegetational shift in the Arabian peninsula could
termining species-specific adaptive characteristics allow hamadryas baboons to spread from their
of the donor species, those that are directly related present range in the southwest to the Persian Gulf
to mate preference, and any that are functionally coast, and hence into Iraq, Iran, and the Indian
incompatible with the host’s genome at a molecular subcontinent, and eventually “bachelor” hamadryas
level, together with neutral markers closely linked would meet a similarly expanding population of rhe-
to any of these. Any alleles that are universally sus monkeys. While most of the resultant rheboons
advantageous (and hitch-hiking markers genetically would undoubtedly perish without issue, repeated
linked to these) could become rapidly established by natural expimentation might produce a few fertile
positive selection in the recipient species. In evolu- survivors.
tionary perspective, the effect would be homoplasy, It might also be objected that papionin hybridiza-
with similar adaptive features appearing in sepa- tion is a poor analogy for hominins, first because
rate species lineages. such hybridization has never been demonstrated
Is hybridization of this kind (minimal but possibly among extant hominines, and second because the
influential gene flow between fully differentiated propensity to hybridize successfully across wide tax-
taxa, i.e., entities that any paleontologist would be onomic gulfs is probably a papionin peculiarity, de-
happy to call different species, if not genera) likely to pendent on their unusual karyotypic uniformity (not
have occurred among basal hominins? We cannot to mention their notorious sexual promiscuity). This
hope to answer this question directly, since even if a argument is worth examining more closely. It is the
steady trickle of genes passed between populations, case that gorillas, chimpanzees, and bonobos have
the chances of finding a recognizable hybrid individ- never been reliably shown to hybridize in the wild or
ual as a fossil (a true morphological intermediate in captivity. Rumored wild chimpanzee-gorilla hy-
like the F1 geboons; Jolly et al., 1997) must be van- brids have so far always turned out to be one or the
ishingly small. Testing the proposition that basal other, and extreme differences in genital anatomy,
hominins were “crossable” thus depends on the anal- at least, might be expected to limit the possibilities
ogy of the papionins and other vertebrates, espe- of unaided cross-fertilization. Chimpanzee-human
cially mammals (e.g., cervids, camelids, bovines, hybrids are occasionally rumored, but the one sup-
Jolly] PAPIONIN ANALOGIES FOR HOMININ EVOLUTION 191
posed hybrid individual I have seen (“Oliver”) was type, hybrid viability and fertility range from the
certainly a mutilated Pan troglodytes. Pan troglo- sterile and dysfunctional rheboon, to geboons, in
dytes and P. paniscus do not presently meet in the which F1s function and breed, at least in captivity,
wild, and if opposite-sexed adults of the two forms and Macaca fascicularis/nemestrina hybrids, which
have been housed together in captivity, the fact, and were functional and probably fertile in the wild.
its outcome, have not been reported as far as I know. Although the sample of papionin cases is small, and
In general, successful captive breeding of hominine the data are incomplete, they do not support the
apes postdates the era in which primates were notion that members of this tribe are exceptional in
housed haphazardly in mixed-species groups, when their ability to form viable and fertile hybrids. They
most reported monkey hybrids were produced (e.g., are, however, consistent with the hypothesis that in
Gray, 1954). all catarrhines, hybrid fitness is more or less in-
Assuming, however, that papionins are indeed versely proportional to the time since divergence of
much more crossable than extant hominine apes, the parental species. Chimpanzee and gorilla stocks
can the difference be attributed to gross karyotypic separated shortly after rhesus and baboons, twice as
factors? The papionins share a 2N ⫽ 42 karyotype early as baboons and geladas, and about three times
that is very similar in overall morphology, though as long ago as macaque species-groups. If we assume
the Cercocebus-Mandrillus clade appears to carry a a common catarrhine scale of stochastic divergence
minor chromosomal rearrangement as an autapo- leading to reproductive isolation, their lack of natu-
morphy (Dutrillaux et al., 1982), and Macaca fas- ral hybridization is not unexpected.
cicularis may also have its own minor rearrange- Of course, we have no idea when in the course of
ment. The papionin karyotype has also retained a hominin evolution the gross karyotypic autapomor-
high level of structural homology, even in the most phies of the human species were acquired, how they
distantly related branches (Papio vs. Macaca, Moore were distributed across basal hominin lineages, and
et al., 1999). By contrast, the extant hominines are what, if any, effect they had on the ability to hybrid-
karyotypically diverse, with humans (2N ⫽ 46) dif- ize. If the present interpretation is correct, however,
fering from both Pan and Gorilla (2N ⫽ 48) (Nick- we do not need to reconstruct the karyotypes of
erson and Nelson, 1998; Jauch et al., 1992). hominin lineages coexisting 3– 4 million years ago to
The role of gross chromosomal rearrangements in predict that most of them retained an ability to
the evolution of reproductive isolation, especially in exchange genes occasionally. Such lineages cannot
animals, is debatable, however. A recent review con- have been separate from each other for more than 3
cluded that while speciation driven by gross karyo- ma, and from the common chimpanzee stem perhaps
typic rearrangement may occur, the “more widely 1 ma more. The vicariance events, population splits,
held view is that the accumulation of chromosomal and genetic divergences that resulted in hominin
differences between populations is largely incidental cladogenesis were, at the time, much more recent
to speciation” (Rieseberg, 2001, p. 351). Even if chro- than the Papio-Theropithecus split is today. If we
mosomal rearrangements are implicated in the evo- assume a common catarrhine scale of crossability,
lution of hybrid sterility or inviability, changes large basal hominins were, on average, somewhere be-
enough to be seen in a metaphase karyotype may not tween macaque species-groups and baboon-gelada
be more influential than minor changes invisible at in their ability to hybridize. This suggests that
that scale. The better-supported alternative view is whenever extrinsic, premating barriers were low-
that genetic changes, both small and large, and in- ered (e.g., if colonizing individuals of opposite sex
cluding a few large enough to be visible as karyo- and different species entered a habitat isolate simul-
typic mutations, accumulate stochastically, as a side taneously), the basal hominins could have produced
effect of overall divergence. Some of these changes viable and fertile hybrids, both among themselves
may increase genetic isolation, at times dramati- and perhaps also with species of the chimpanzee
cally (O’Neill et al., 2001), by introducing prezygotic clade. Such hybrids could serve as a conduit for the
barriers to hybrid formation, by reducing hybrid flow of advantageous genes between the parental
fitness, or by decreasing recombination rates populations, or could even be, themselves, the
(Searle, 1998), but such effects are as likely to follow founders of new species.
from physically small chromosomal rearrangements It should be emphasized that such shenanigans in
as large ones. This incremental model seems to ac- no way represent an argument for reducing the hy-
count better for the facts of hybridization among the bridizing forms to congeneric status, let alone con-
catarrhine primates, both hominoid and cercopithe- specificity. Occasional, opportunistic hybridization
coid. Thus, successful hybridization can occur be- is consistent with separate species status under any
tween some species within Cercopithecus (Dutril- current definition. It is also completely compatible
laux et al., 1982) and Hylobates (Van Tuinen et al., with maintenance, and even reinforcement, of diver-
1999) that differ in gross chromosomal morphology gent adaptive trends in the parental species. Papio
and number, as well as karyotypically uniform pap- and Theropithecus populations, for instance, have
ionins, while karyotypically similar chimpanzees probably been sympatric, and occasionally exchang-
and gorillas do not interbreed. Among the papionins, ing genes, for several million years, but have re-
with their highly conserved, 42-chromosome karyo- mained separate entities with their own distinct and
192 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 44, 2001
diverging evolutionary trajectories, and have also does this fact significantly affect our understanding
themselves speciated internally (Delson, 1993; Jab- of early hominin evolution? Certainly, as Tattersall
lonski, 1993), attributes that systematists generally and Eldredge (1977) have long argued, establishing
recognize by separation at the genus or subgenus the cladistic relationships among a cluster of related
level. taxa is a logical and highly desirable first step to-
The analogy of hybridizing papionins does not, of wards understanding their evolutionary history: not
course, prove that hominins did hybridize, but it only their phylogeny, but also causal factors such as
does suggest that crossing might have been possible, dispersal, vicariance, and adaptation. The preceding
even between forms as distinct morphologically (and discussion, for example, includes several interpreta-
presumably ecologically) as Paranthropus boisei and tions that would not have suggested themselves be-
Homo (now Kenyanthropus) rudolfensis, or Austra- fore (((Homo, Pan) Gorilla) Pongo) replaced ((Homo
lopithecus afarensis and K. platyops, or Ardipithecus (Pan, Gorilla)) Pongo) or even (Homo, ((Pan, Gorilla)
ramidus kadabba and the proto-chimpanzee. As Pongo)) as the accepted cladogram of extant Homi-
long as they remained ecologically distinct, occa- nidae. However, it could also be argued that the
sional gene flow would not necessarily undermine importance of determining the correct cladogram, as
their distinct adaptations, because selection among well as the practical possibility of doing so, de-
backcrosses would effectively prevent maladaptive creases in proportion to the lengths of its internodes.
genes moving between them. But by the same token, For example, two “robust” hominin species derived
advantageous genes could be rapidly incorporated, in quick succession from the same, or closely similar,
without disrupting the balanced, divergently adap- “gracile” stocks might well have been very similar as
tive genotypes of the parental species. Contrary to living animals, as well as unrecognizable as sepa-
the standard model, the fuzzy zone in which evolu- rate clades by any methods we can bring to bear on
tion is somewhat reticulate as well as divergent can their fossil remains. Moreover, for a considerable
be prolonged well beyond the point of adaptive, and time after they acquired their distinctive, derived,
paleontologically documented, morphological diver- adaptive traits, they would probably have been in-
gence. terfertile both with each other and with their respec-
But could limited gene-flow of this kind cause tive “gracile” sister taxa. While a reliable cladogram
paleontologically recognizable anomalies in the dis- of early hominins remains a worthwhile goal, some
tribution of morphological character states? To an- of its details may be inherently insoluble. (This
swer this question, we need to know much more doubt seems to be increasingly shared; recent com-
about the genetic basis for such traits (Weiss, 1994; mentaries (Wood and Brooks, 1999; Collard and Ai-
Weiss and Buchanan, 2000). Those dependent on an ello, 2000) tend to omit lines of descent from their
integrated complex of genes at unlinked loci may not depictions of hominin phylogeny).
survive the recombination inherent in backcrossing Fortunately, the early hominins present plenty of
long enough to be selected, whereas traits with a other important questions: numerous problems of
simple genetic base should be more visible, both alpha taxonomy, for example, and paleobiology at
paleontologically and to natural selection. The pos- the local deme level (e.g., Lee-Thorp and van de
sibility of significant intertaxon gene-flow should Merwe, 1993; Teaford and Ungar, 2000; Grine and
certainly be entertained. Increase in relative brain Kay, 1988). which (again on a papionin analogy;
size, highly homoplastic in hominins by any cladistic Benefit, 2000) was probably more diverse than we
permutation (Strait et al., 1997), would be an obvi- presently perceive.
ous candidate. The papionin analogy (supported, in this case, by
From the cladist’s perspective, opportunistic in- analogies from other, similarly divergent mamma-
terlineage gene-flow represents yet another way lian clades) suggests that the hominin “morphs” that
that species might acquire similarities that do not we recognize as synchronous, often sympatric, and
reflect their phylogeny, at least as that term is gen- ecologically distinct species and genera, were prob-
erally used. If the assumption of speciation by clean ably capable of limited interbreeding, even after sev-
breaks without subsequent reticulation is relaxed to eral million years of divergent adaptation.
allow some hybridization-driven reticulation, one By extension, the papionins suggest that hominin
can often reduce significantly the number of steps in lineages were even more capable of interbreeding at
the most parsimonious cladogram (Haszprunar, an earlier stage of differentiation, after newly emer-
1998), but presumably the probability of the hybrid- gent stocks had become widespread, and when pop-
ization itself should be factored into any parsimony ulations were diverging by drift, adaptation to local
comparison, and it is difficult to see how this might conditions, and econiche specialization. At this
be done. stage, gene flow between them would depend less on
Considering the possibility of occasional reticula- their inherent reproductive and genetic compatibil-
tion, along with the adaptive homoplasy and cryptic ity (their “crossability”) than on extrinsic factors:
symplesiomorphy that were probably rife in basal accidents of biogeography that created or removed
hominin evolution, Alexander Papyoe feels com- physical barriers to contact between them, and the
pelled to ask whether a reliable cladogram of the intensity of disruptive selection that maintained the
group may be a practical impossibility. And, if so, distinctness of their gene-pools.
Jolly] PAPIONIN ANALOGIES FOR HOMININ EVOLUTION 193
Several papionin taxa (especially species within emphasis to put on the distribution of probabilities
the genus Papio and within Macaca species-groups) of zygote formation (“zygostructure;” Jolly, 1993), or
have presently attained this stage of differentiation the distribution of traits within and among popula-
and exemplify in their contemporary population tions (“phenostructure”). The BSC emphasizes zygo-
structure the complexities that occur among partial structure (species as reproductive isolates), while
isolates (e.g., Hoelzer et al., 1994). There seems no the PSC emphasizes phenostructure (species as con-
reason to doubt that the history of hominins from sistently diagnosable clades). Justification for the
the late Miocene onwards was at least as complex, BSC tends to look forward in time, emphasizing that
and equally intractable to adequate description in if genes can flow across populational boundaries, all
terms of a simple, ramifying phylogeny and taxon- populations so linked can at least in theory evolve as
omy. We can hope that the paleogeographic picture a single unit, and that gene-flow might even homog-
and early hominin fossil record will eventually be- enize them in the future. Adherents of the PSC tend
come sufficiently continuous and fine-grained in to look backwards in time, emphasizing species as
time and space to allow some specific comparisons terminal taxa in a cladogram defined by their his-
and predictions. This remains a distant goal for torical relationships. The PSC and the BSC are
early hominin history, and meanwhile, Alexander equally valid descriptions of aspects of population
Papyoe suggests that early hominin phylogenies and structure resulting from evolutionary processes.
taxonomies should be regarded as disposable ap- Both are compatible with the traditional, ontologi-
proximations. cally appealing but epistemologically weak concept
The same principle applies to the most recent of a species as a cluster of populations with a com-
radiations within Homo and Papio, but here the mon and distinct evolutionary trajectory. Neither
level of documentation has reached the point where kind of species is more “real” than the other; both
the complexity of the problems, if not their solution, are abstractions from the observable attributes of
can be discerned. organisms. My nominalist bias suggests that, since
HYBRID ZONES AND POPULATION both species definitions use the same basic at-
REPLACEMENTS IN BABOONS AND HUMANS tributes of populations (pheno- and zygostructure),
merely differing in how to weight them, we should
The “Neandertal problem” has had different con- focus on describing these attributes, and shelve in-
notations for every generation of human evolution- definitely the largely bogus “species problem” (Jolly,
ists. One constant theme, however, has been the 1993).
degree of distinctness between Neandertals and an- In taxonomic practice, the BSC-PSC distinction is
atomically modern people. In contemporary discus- most significant where phenotypically distinct,
sions, this question tends to be couched in taxonomic parapatric populations interbreed at their bound-
terms: whether or not the Neandertals should be aries. The PSC calls such populations “species” (in
classified within Homo sapiens, or, more generally, spite of the fact that hybrid individuals can be as-
how many species of the genus Homo existed con- signed only arbitrarily, and thus all individuals will
temporaneously during the later Pleistocene, say, not be strictly assignable). The BSC calls them semi-
from ca. 150 –30 ka. Answers to the second question species of a superspecies, or subspecies of a Rassen-
from contemporary paleoanthropologists range from kreis or polytypic species, since they are potentially
a firm “one” to an equally positive “unknown, but linked by gene-flow (even though in most cases no
certainly several.” Even a cursory review of the “Ne- gene-flow has been demonstrated). Because such sit-
andertal problem,” which has produced several large uations are very common in nature, the seemingly
volumes and innumerable papers over the past few minor difference in species definitions greatly affects
years, is far beyond the present scope. However, the number, and size, of recognized species. More-
Alexander Papyoe suggests that a new angle or two over, within the BSC, many closely related “species”
may emerge by comparing it to an analogous prob- are very similar in diversity and scale to “subspe-
lem in Papionini: the question of how many species cies,” differing from the latter only in that marginal
should be recognized among contemporary forms of gene-flow has not been shown to occur. If such gene-
the genus Papio, or at a more particular level, flow is later demonstrated, the discovery mandates a
whether the olive baboon, for example, should be wholesale taxonomic revision (for the baboon case,
considered a different species from the hamadryas, cf. Thorington and Groves, 1970; Groves 2001).
or the yellow baboon, or both. The population structure and dynamics of taxa at
In both cases, real biological issues are often ob- this level exemplify crucial evolutionary processes
scured by the perennial but inevitably unproductive gathered under the rubric of “speciation” (Barton,
discussions about what a species “really is.” A recent 2001; Turelli et al., 2001): the evolution of pheno-
review (Hey, 2001) counted 24 active species defini- typic distinctiveness or reproductive isolation, or
tions. Those seemingly most widely used among zo- both. Investigation of these processes is too biologi-
ologists and paleontologists (the “biological” and the cally important to be sidetracked by the empty se-
“phylogenetic” species concepts; BSC and PSC, re- mantics of the “species question,” and to avoid this
spectively) are both population-based. The disagree- pitfall, Grubb (1999) introduced the concept of the
ment between them seems to hinge on how much allotaxon. Allotaxa are phylogenetically close, but
194 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 44, 2001
well-differentiated and diagnosable, geographically This spatial restriction may indicate that contact is
replacing forms whose ranges do not overlap, but are too recent for the interpopulational clines to have
either disjunct, adjoining, or separated by compara- reached equilibrium, or that habitat-specific or in-
tively narrow zones in which characters are clinally trinsic hybrid disadvantage is balancing outward
distributed. Related allotaxa typically exhibit dis- and inward gene-flow. Field studies of particular
tinct adaptations to their respective habitats, but hybrid zones have revealed a great variety of struc-
their defining allo- or parapatry suggests that they tures and dynamics (Barton and Hewitt, 1985; Har-
are close enough ecologically and behaviorally to rison et al., 1987).
preclude actual coexistence within the same ecosys- Like many other terrestrial organisms, extant Pa-
tem. Where the ranges of allotaxa meet, often at an pio baboons (and Macaca species-groups) show a
ecotone, a zone of intermediate phenotypes ( a “hy- typical “patchwork quilt” internal phenostructure of
brid zone”) frequently occurs. Such zones have been geographically replacing, parapatric allotaxa (Fig.
aptly called “natural laboratories of the evolutionary 1). Though many details and some crucial areas
process” (Harrison, 1993), and a lively biological spe- remain to be investigated, the distribution of baboon
cialty has grown up around their theoretical and allotaxa (Jolly, 1993; Groves, 2001) is better known
empirical study (Barton and Hewitt, 1985, 1989; than implied by their chronically confused formal
Harrison, 1993), the latter greatly facilitated by the taxonomy. Five “forms” are customarily recognized,
growing availability of genetic markers (serological, either as PSC species (Groves, 2001) or BSC subspe-
allozymic, and most recently genomic). The use of cies (Williams-Blangero et al., 1990; Jolly, 1993), or
genetic markers, combined with advances in paleo- as a species of a “superspecies.” At least 2 of the 5
climatology, has added a fourth dimension to the include two or more allotaxa, which, strictly, should
picture by documenting genetic patterns that can be disqualify them as single PSC species. The geo-
related to the late Neogene history of repeated, often graphically circumscribed allotaxa are distin-
very rapid, environmental change. The dynamics of guished most readily by characters of pelage texture
such processes have been most thoroughly investi- and color. Dental and cranial features also differ
gated in Europe (Hewitt, 1996, 2001) and North among allotaxa (Jolly, 1965, 1970b; Phillips-Conroy,
America (Avise, 1994), but there is ample evidence 1978), but are less diagnostic. The precise number of
that environmental fluctuations were equally influ-
allotaxa that can usefully be recognized is still un-
ential in the tropics and subtropics, and in the
certain; some “forms” that have been described and
southern hemisphere, though the nature and distri-
named, especially in the east African corridor from
bution of barrier zones in these areas is less obvious.
Tanzania to Sudan, may turn out to be hybrids or
The model developed by Hewitt (1996, 1999, 2001)
segments of a continuously varying cline (Groves,
describes a plausible process by which late Neogene
paleoclimatic oscillations converted local popula- 2001). The number of allotaxa recognized also de-
tions within species into fully diagnosable allotaxa. pends upon how broad an intergradation zone the
Responding to rapid habitat shifts, the ranges of systematist is prepared to tolerate between them.
geographical populations would have expanded and For example, the small yellow baboon (Papio kindae
contracted, repeatedly dividing and reuniting, forc- or P. cynocephaus kindae) has a wide range that
ing equally rapid fluctuations in population size. stretches from Angola to the Luangwa Valley in
The core area of each population, habitable during Zambia. It is quite distinct from the large, “typical”
adverse climatic periods, tends to preserve ancient yellow baboon of Tanzania and Malawi (P. cynoceph-
genetic variation, both adaptive and random. Colo- alus, sensu stricto), but the cline that links them is
nizing subpopulations at the fringe individually lose relatively broad. Conservatively, 8 or 9 allotaxa are
variation, but local adaptation, drift, and founder- recognized here.
flush effects may cause them to become diverse and In the cohesion species concept of Templeton
genetically idiosyncratic. The descendants of “colo- (1989), a crucial criterion of conspecificity is that
nists” dominate numerically during expansions, and conspecific organisms should be ecologically inter-
as populations expand it is the demes at their changeable. Like other species criteria, this one sit-
fringes, which are the most genetically derived, that uates the allotaxa of Papio on the cusp of species
meet their neighbors. Repeated contraction and ex- status. On the one hand, the ranges of some Papio
pansion from separate refugia thus tend to generate allotaxa roughly correspond to broad vegetational
and accentuate the kind of genetic differences that zones, and the lines of contact of parapatric allotaxa
define adjacent allotaxa, and sometimes push the often fall close to an ecotone (Jolly, 1993; Kingdon,
process to the point of partial or complete reproduc- 1997). In at least one case (the hamadryas), we can
tive incompatibility (Hewitt, 2001). demonstrate a pervasive pattern of subtle, physio-
When such differentiated allotaxa make second- logical (Kaplan et al., 1999), behavioral (Kummer,
ary contact, initially at a sharp boundary, interac- 1968), and anatomical (Jolly and Phillips-Conroy,
tions between them may fall anywhere between 2001 and unpublished data) adaptations to the
complete interfertility and total reproductive isola- semidesert habitat in which most populations are
tion. Secondary hybrid zones generated by marginal found. Other habitat-specific adaptations (e.g., yel-
interbreeding are often quite restricted in breadth. low baboons to the woodland vegetation of the Afri-
Jolly] PAPIONIN ANALOGIES FOR HOMININ EVOLUTION 195

Fig. 1. Geographical distribution of some of the recognizable allotaxa of Papio baboons.

can southern seasonal tropics) have been suggested the Ethiopian highlands, are occupied by anubis
(Kingdon, 1997), but have yet to be demonstrated. baboons (Zinner and Hapke, 2001). Conversely, it is
On the other hand, ecological similarity is indi- anubis, not hamadryas, that occur in the semidesert
cated by the fact that Papio allotaxa are parapatric, Saharan massifs of Tibesti and Air (Jolly, 1965). All
with ranges that meet but do not overlap, presum- Papio baboons are ecological generalists, a trait that
ably because groups from either side of the allotaxon perhaps makes them especially relevant to human
boundary interact ecologically and socially as com- evolution. Their modest, habitat-specific ecological
petitors when they meet. At allotaxon interfaces, adaptations have not become specializations pre-
individuals are in fact literally interchangeable, be- cluding expansion into neighboring habitats. Conse-
cause they migrate across allotaxon lines and suc- quently, the distribution of each allotaxon seems to
cessfully take up residence in a “foreign” group (Al- result not so much from ecological determinants or
berts and Altmann, 2001; Samuels and Altmann, preferences, as from population history, some of it
1986; Phillips-Conroy et al., 1992), where their eco- quite recent. For instance, the ecologically anoma-
logical behavior and preferences are indistinguish- lous distribution of anubis and hamadryas in Eri-
able from those of their hosts (Nystrom, 1992). trea may be due to the fact that hamadryas reached
Moreover, a closer examination of the range of the and colonized the moist highlands first, and have yet
various allotaxa reveals many exceptions to the to be displaced by anubis, which are still confined, in
broad ecological associations. For example, hamadr- low numbers, to the western savanna lowlands (Zin-
yas baboons in Eritrea occupy comparatively moist ner and Hapke, 2001). This hypothetical scenario is
montane habitats that immediately to the south, in supported by observations from western Tanzania
196 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 44, 2001
(J. Moore, personal communication), Ethiopia (Phil- typic (Jolly, 1965, 1993) evidence all suggests that
lips-Conroy et al., 1992), and southeastern Kenya the primary division among stocks leading to extant
(Maples and McKern, 1967; Alberts and Altmann, Papio was between a “southern” branch, ancestral to
2001). In each of these areas, anubis baboons seem extant chacma baboons, and a “northern” one, an-
to be expanding their range, at the expense of neigh- cestral to all the others, and that this split occurred
boring yellow baboon and hamadryas populations, about 1.7 ma. Genetically and phenotypically (as
partly by displacement, and partly by genetic infil- well as geographically), anubis baboons are “north-
tration. ern,” but have some “southern” traits such as tail
In captivity, all allotaxa appear to hybridize indis- shape (Jolly, 1965). As Kingdon (1997) suggested,
criminately (Jolly, unpublished data), and there is the anubis phenotype may have originated as a sta-
no evidence for hybrid breakdown, behavioral in- bilized hybrid, in a small, isolated “northern” popu-
compatibility, or intrinsic sterility. Similarly, there lation, resembling Guinea baboons, that received
is no evidence that Papio baboon allotaxa ever avoid and incorporated “southern” immigrants, resem-
interbreeding when they meet in the wild, though bling chacmas, via a glacial-period corridor through
many boundary areas have yet to be investigated. the central African rainforest. This scenario will
The fact that documented baboon hybrid zones are require testing against extensive genetic informa-
narrow, in spite of the lack of obvious, intrinsic tion from anubis baboons of west-central Africa,
barriers to gene-flow, strongly suggests that they which at present are totally unknown.3
are the result of secondary contact following range The complex populational history of Papio is not
oscillations (Barton and Hewitt, 1985; Hewitt, 2001; unique, or probably even unusual, among taxa of
Harrison, 1993). comparable time-depth. Among the papionin mon-
In East Africa, where genetic sampling has been keys, observational and genetic investigation of spe-
more dense than elsewhere, the distribution of cies-groups of macaques has revealed a phylogeog-
mtDNA haplotypes across baboon allotaxa strongly raphy that is at least equally complex, involving all
hints at previous cycles of hybridization. In partic- degrees of hybridization, sex-specific gene-flow, and
ular, all haplotypes so far found in anubis baboons secondary fusion between differentiated taxa
in Ethiopia form a clade that is distinct from, but (Fooden, 1963; Bynum et al., 1997; Tosi et al., 2000;
related to, haplotypes of Ethiopian hamadryas ba- Hoelzer et al., 1993; Melnick and Hoelzer, 1992;
boons, while haplotypes of anubis baboons from Ke- Evans et al., 1999).
nya are closer to those of Kenyan and north Tanza- How can these speculative scenarios about popu-
nian yellow baboons (Wildman, 2000 and personal lation structure and phylogeographic history in Pa-
communication; Newman et al., 2001). In external pio baboons be translated into useful analogies for
phenotype, all anubis baboons are (by definition) understanding evolution within the genus Homo? A
quite different from either yellow or hamadryas, and comparison of chronologies indicates that the initial
moreover, Kenyan and Ethiopian anubis baboons diversification of stocks leading to extant Papio
are externally indistinguishable. This combination forms (Wildman, 2000; Newman et al., 2001) is com-
of phenotypes and haplotypes is difficult to explain parable in age (at about 1.7 ma) to the origin and
except by a scenario involving a cycle of hybridiza- rapid deployment of Homo, sensu stricto (i.e., the
tion previous to the present one, with sex-specific clade stemming from African Homo erectus a.k.a. H.
introgression, and probably also radical fluctuations ergaster, but excluding “H.” habilis and “H.” ru-
in allotaxon ranges (Wildman, 2000 and personal dolfensis) (Wood and Collard, 1999). If we can in-
communication; Newman et al., 2001). With benefit deed assume a common timescale of average, intrin-
of hindsight, we can see vestiges of this earlier hy- sic reproductive isolation for all catarrhines, as
bridization cycle in the skull structure of Ethiopian argued above, this suggests that all human lineages
and Kenyan anubis baboons, which in some aspects stemming from the H. ergaster stock were probably
of size and shape resemble those of neighboring as fully interfertile as are extant Papio populations.
hamadryas and yellow baboons, respectively (Jolly, On these grounds, they could be regarded as mem-
1965 and unpublished data). If this scenario is ap- bers of a single, polytypic (BSC) species (cf. Wolpoff
proximately correct (and much more genetic work is et al., 1993; Hawks et al., 2000). Several caveats
needed to test it fully), it suggests that the pelage should be observed, however.
phenotype defining anubis baboons, which is very First, the postulated “common catarrhine cross-
stable not only in Ethiopia and Kenya but across the ability scale” is at best a stochastically driven ap-
whole northern savanna belt as far as Sierra Leone, proximation that is necessarily less predictive for
was already characteristic of the baboons contribut- particular cases and over shorter timescales. Muta-
ing the anubis genes to the mix in Kenya and Ethi- tions contributing to reproductive isolation, perhaps
opia. If this is so, it must have originated well before
this round of hybridization. 3
The anubis phenotype itself may have originated The phyletic position of yellow baboons is ambiguous. Their genet-
ics seem to ally them closely with anubis baboons (Newman et al.,
in a still earlier round of hybridization. MtDNA 2001; Williams-Blangero et al., 1990), but they have been sampled
(Wildman, 2000; Newman et al., 2001), nuclear ge- only from East Africa, close to a zone of hybridization with anubis.
netic (Williams-Blangero et al., 1990), and pheno- Morphologically they are certainly “southern.”
Jolly] PAPIONIN ANALOGIES FOR HOMININ EVOLUTION 197
initiating a rapid “cascade” to full intersterility with markers would reveal a comparable, underlying cla-
neighboring populations (Barton, 2001; Rieseberg, distic history, probably greatly complicated by suc-
2001), could arise at any time in any geographically cessive events of marginal gene-flow and hybridiza-
isolated lineage. Such an event in a human lineage tion. Some human allotaxa (e.g., the one represented
would be difficult, if not impossible, to detect from by the Ngandong specimens), analogous to Guinea
fossil evidence. or chacma baboons, might have been more-or-less
Second, it is important to stress that if we reduce isolated and little changed since the initial deploy-
recognizable “forms” of Homo (currently, often ment of the genus Homo (strictu senso). Others,
named as PSC species such as H. neanderthalensis analogous to contemporary hamadryas, anubis, and
and H. erectus) to (BSC) subspecific status because yellow baboons of the East African corridor, proba-
of the possibility of some marginal gene flow be- bly had a more recent and complex history of inter-
tween them, this would not imply that they were mittent genetic interaction.
“ephemeral” or “evolutionarily unimportant,” any We can also assume that where they met, the
more than these terms could be applied to, say, boundaries between human allotaxa showed the
anubis baboons. This point is worth emphasizing, kind of complex dynamics seen in other vertebrate
because a suggestion to the contrary seems to have and invertebrate contact zones. Unfortunately, in
crept into the debate over the implications of the most cases the meager fossil evidence limits us to
Lagar Velho “hybrid child” (Duarte et al., 1999). It observing that there was appreciable regional vari-
seems to represent a semantic confusion. If indeed ation in cranial form among humans of this period
the Lagar Velho child was the result of marginal (Wolpoff, 1999). The analogy of Papio (and other
gene-flow between Neandertals and “moderns,” it catarrhines) suggests that early human allotaxa
would prove them to be conspecific by the BSC, but were probably much more distinct in the flesh than
not by the PSC. But it is only PSC taxa that are from the kind of evidence available in the fossil
defined so closely that intraspecific variation is, al- record. There are significant average skeletal and
most by definition, unimportant. Large, polytypic dental distinctions among the baboon allotaxa, but
BSC species can and often do include persistent, they are not easily seen without large samples and
diagnosable, “important” allotaxa, named as subspe- prior allotaxon assignment, based on soft-part (pel-
cies. age) characteristics. On the other hand, Papio does
Nor would the assumption of universal interfertil- not altogether fit the predictions of “Tattersall’s law”
ity within the genus Homo (strictu senso) conflict (Tattersall, 1993), which suggests that any taxa di-
with evidence pointing to long-term, consistently di- agnosable on hard-part criteria can be assumed to
agnosable human lineages, such as a pre-Neander- be even more distinct in the flesh. For example, the
tal-to-Neandertal lineage persisting for ⬃300,000 “kindae” and “typical” yellow baboon allotaxa have
years in Europe (Trinkaus, 1991, 1993; Rosas, 2001), very similar coloration (and are classified together
or Homo erectus populations locally surviving the in 1 of the traditional 5 species), but are easily
origins and spread of “modern” humans (Swisher et distinguishable on cranial and dental size, and are
al., 1996). Even if they were not surrounded by in- connected by a phenocline in the wild. The only
trinsic barriers to interbreeding with their neigh- generalization that we can make on the analogy of
bors, the genetic integrity of such populations could the baboons is that we should expect our diagnosis of
be effectively maintained by a combination of peri- individual fossil human specimens to be unreliable,
odic geographical barriers, selection for habitat-spe- even if the existence of significant phenostructuring
cific ecological adaptations, behavioral differences can be inferred from divergent central tendencies in
(which themselves could have a combined genetic geographically defined clusters of specimens. At
and environmental basis), and simple, outward pop- best, a much denser record will be required to dis-
ulation-pressure. A PSC advocate could happily cern what the human allotaxa were, where their
name each one a full species, while a BSC enthusiast geographical limits lay, and how they interacted
could describe the same phenomenon as polytypy with each other.
and regional continuity within a single species. In fact, in the densest fossil record that we have
From the point of view of hybrid-zone theory, such (of the Neandertals and their neighbors to the
cases just represent some of the many possible in- south), the pattern on one corner of the Late Pleis-
teractions between neighboring allotaxa. tocene human patchwork is dimly discernible. Leav-
As for the “Neandertal question,” it is a reason- ing aside the largely semantic question of Neander-
able working hypothesis that, like baboons today, tal species status, and the issue of their genetic
the genus Homo by the beginning of the Late Pleis- contribution to later human populations, we find
tocene presented a patchwork of allotaxa, each with substantial agreement. It is generally accepted that
its own history of successive glacial (cold-dry period) Neandertals were a localized, recognizable human
retreats and interglacial (warm-humid period) ex- population of Europe and southwestern Asia, com-
pansions. As with the baboons, we can assume that prising an allotaxon distinct from pene-contempo-
all human allotaxa alive at that time were not rary Afro-Arabian populations. The latter are repre-
equally related to each other: that if a time-travel- sented by specimens such as Skhul V, Kafzeh, and
ling geneticist could sample them in depth, genetic Omo II, which are widely believed either to repre-
198 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 44, 2001
sent the ancestral modern human stock itself, or at cussion now focuses on whether replacement was a
least to be more closely related to it than the Nean- matter of gene-flow between static populations, or
dertals were. If Neandertals and Africans came into an expanding population replacing a shrinking one,
periodic contact, this probably occurred at an eco- or something in between. The problem is thus
tone in the eastern Mediterranean, where an alter- brought into the realm of general hybrid-zone the-
nation of faunas and human allotaxa seems to have ory, in which newts (Arntzen and Wallis, 1991),
occurred in synchrony with glacial-driven oscilla- crows (Saino et al., 1992), and baboons are at least
tions (Tchernov, 1989). This extended, marginal in- as relevant as human beings.
teraction between Neandertal and Afro-Arabian al- The fragments of Neandertal mtDNA sequence
lotaxa, apparently similar in culture, technology, (Krings et al., 1997; Hoss, 2000; Ovchinnikov et al.,
and ecological role in their respective habitats, 2000) suggest the point at which the Neandertal
would have been quite distinct from subsequent “re- story can be linked to the analogous history of ba-
placement” events. boons. Discussion of the Neandertal mtDNA se-
During a period that was recently narrowed to quence has focused mainly on its relatively ancient
36 –30 ka, Neandertal morphology was replaced in separation from the root of all extant human se-
Europe by a more “modern” type of human quences, and its implications for a Neandertal ge-
(Churchill and Smith, 2001), a local manifestation of netic contribution to modern human populations.
the general replacement of “archaic” by “modern” From the baboon (or chimpanzee, or gorilla) perspec-
humans that had begun some time before in the tive, however, the separation is not very ancient. It
tropics. The interface between them is believed to is comparable to ⬃600 ka divergences between olive
have moved quite rapidly from the southeast west- and hamadryas baboon mtDNA haplotypes, and
wards, reaching northwest Europe and Iberia last. much more recent than, e.g., the Guinea-hamadryas
The disagreement concerns dynamics at the inter- split. Mitochondrial diversity in Papio may be anal-
face. ogous to the condition in Homo before the “event”
When considering Neandertals’ interactions with (generally interpreted as an “out-of-Africa” expan-
neighboring populations, most paleoanthropologists sion of a relatively small subpopulation) that elimi-
have drawn analogies from the behavior of extant nated most of the diversity from its collective mito-
Homo sapiens, assuming for instance that cultural chondrial (and Y-linked, and autosomal) gene-pool.
factors such as language (rather than “hard-wired” Unfortunately, investigation of continent-wide ge-
differences in mate recognition systems, believed to netic phenostructure in Papio is still in its earliest
be characteristic of other mammals) were the major stages, so we cannot pursue the analogy further in
determinant. An early, extreme form of the multire- this direction. We can, however, make some sugges-
gional hypothesis saw only cultural elements flow- tions based on work in contemporary zones of hy-
ing between populations, and physical changes as bridization, especially the Awash anubis-hamadryas
indigenous adaptations to the resultant technologi- hybrid zone. For example, we can conclude that un-
cal shifts. Certainly, only modern humans approach less an undocumented, radical genetic event oc-
the cultural sophistication inferred for both Nean- curred in the 600 ka since they shared mtDNA an-
dertals and their contemporaries, but they present a cestry with the Neandertals, premodern humans
poor analogy in that no two modern human popula- were certainly able to interbreed with them and
tions are as distantly related as were Neandertals produce viable, fertile, offspring, as hamadryas and
and the precursors of modern humans, or have had anubis baboons do.
as long a period of separation in which to acquire The baboon analogy does not, however, inspire
distinct mate-recognition systems. Whatever the confidence that the detailed dynamics of “archaic”-
reason, it is now clear that the genetic structure of “modern” interactions will ever be determined. Cer-
the extant human species is very unusual among tainly, finding any fossil documentation is quite un-
widespread primate taxa. It is distinguished by a likely, as obvious evidence of intergradation (or its
paucity of ancient genetic lineages, except in loci absence) is likely to be seen only in a spatially (or,
where diversity is likely to be maintained by selec- with a moving zone, chronologically) very restricted
tion (Disotell, 1999), and in general is dominated by zone at the allotaxon interface. In Ethiopian and
the effects of massive population expansions, most of Kenyan baboon hybrid zones, as in many zones in-
which occurred in the past 20 ka (e.g., Harpending volving other taxa, there is a very narrow region in
et al., 1998). At the very least, this suggests that which most individuals are phenotypically obvious
additional analogies should be sought outside the hybrids, yet such hybrids are found only within one
genus Homo. Another reason for doing this is that or two dispersal distance units (about 30 km) of the
the uniquely human, culture-driven, in situ conver- zone’s center, although genetic evidence of hybrid-
sion of Neandertals to “moderns” (Hawks and Wol- ization is much more widespread. For the human
poff, 2001) without any appreciable population case, this has an important implication: demonstrat-
movement or “gene-flow” is now hard to reconcile ing phenotypic distinctness (lack of overlap) of Ne-
with the rather short timescale of replacement andertal and “modern” samples drawn from areas
(Churchill and Smith, 2001), and has been aban- remote in time and space from the zone of contact
doned by its original formulators. As a result, dis- does not disprove the occurrence of interbreeding at
Jolly] PAPIONIN ANALOGIES FOR HOMININ EVOLUTION 199
the interface. It also means that the Lagar Velho investigation of the DNA of post-Neandertal fossil
child, if indeed it is a hybrid, is a rare and valuable humans (cf. Hawks and Wolpoff, 2001); the second
find, even though it is irrelevant to the Neandertal by trawling the extant human gene-pool itself.
“species question,” and does not tell us whether Ne- So far, almost all genetic systems investigated in
andertals (or other “archaic” humans) contributed extant humans show no signs of a Neandertal inher-
genes to the Upper Paleolithic, or the extant, human itance, but perhaps we need to be more selective in
gene-pool. Not that these are equivalent, as is often our search. A moving hybrid zone may leave in its
implied; there was ample opportunity for the loss of wake a few neutral markers derived from the re-
a few stray Neandertal genes from European Upper treating population (Arntzen and Wallis, 1991), but
Paleolithic populations when the latter shrank and these are likely later to be eliminated by drift. Most
were replaced by food-producing peoples. likely to survive and be incorporated are genes for
If interbreeding did occur at the Neandertal-mod- traits strongly favored by local conditions (and
ern interface, the number of different possible sce- “hitch-hiking” markers linked to these). Some years
narios of hybrid zone dynamics is enormous. Any ago, a popular work (Kurtén, 1971) plausibly sug-
number of factors besides intrinsic hybrid disadvan- gested that Neandertals were blond and blue-eyed
tage could have restricted gene-flow out of the con- in adaptation to cloudy, periglacial Europe, while
tact zone, or filtered it, or directed it asymmetrically incoming “moderns” had the darker pigmentation of
toward one population or the other. Only one such a subtropical people. Perhaps we should survey nor-
zone in primates (between anubis and hamadryas dic Europeans for unusually “deep” diversity in non-
baboons in the Awash National Park, Ethiopia) has coding genetic elements closely linked to loci deter-
been the subject of both detailed behavioral and mining pigmentation. Less fancifully, Parham et al.
genetic work, and this has revealed a complex situ- (1994; and Parham, personal communication) spec-
ation in which gene-flow into and out of the hybrid ulatively identified a possible Neandertal legacy: an
zone is directed and limited largely by idiosyncratic allele of the human MHC system that is found at low
patterns of social behavior distinctive of the two frequency in the old Neandertal range. It is remark-
parental taxa and their hybrids (Sugawara,1979; able for its inferred ancient separation from other
Nystrom, 1992; Bergman, 2000; Beyene, 1998). alleles, which themselves form a tight, young clade.
In the Neandertal case, the fact that the interface MHC alleles are among the likeliest genes to pass
moved historically from east to west indicates that through a semipermeable hybrid zone, since selec-
the pressure of gene-flow was greater in that direc- tion favors immunological diversity per se, so if the
tion; if a hybrid zone existed, the genes in it were interpretation is confirmed it would set a likely up-
contributed disproportionately by “moderns.” “Ne- per limit on the Neandertal genetic contribution to
andertal morphological genes” may have been re- extant Europeans.
moved by natural selection from a narrow zone of The message from A. Papyoe is, once again, to
hybridization, or been swamped by differential ge- concentrate on biology, avoid semantic traps, and
netic inflow, or perhaps they simply died out with realize that any species-level taxonomy based on
their carriers without any hybridization at all. Any fossil material is going to be only an approximate
combination of these factors could have contributed reflection of real-world complexities. With extant
to their disappearance. A much more fine-grained taxa such as Papio baboons, we can document soft-
temporal record of the transition would be necessary part as well as dental and skeletal anatomy, and
to decide between these alternatives, and the precise sample enough individuals to document inter- and
scenario is immaterial both for the eventual out- intrapopulational diversity (Fig. 2). Even then, we
come, and for the so-called “species question.” cannot reach a consensus about the “number of ba-
What is important, and hotly contested, is boon species.” At a less global level, our research
whether Neandertals (and other archaics) contrib- group has studied gene-flow on the ground in the
uted any genes to the gene-pool of the human pop- best-known of the intergrade zones, and has typed
ulation who succeeded them. This would imply a mitochondrial haplotypes and 10 microsatellite loci
flow of genes from the marginal hybrid zone into the in nearly 1,000 individuals, as well as examining
expanding modern population: swimming, as it their external phenotype and features of their den-
were, against the tide. The important question is not tition, and observing and quantifying mating pref-
whether Neandertals could have passed some genes erences, social barriers to interbreeding, and the
by hybridization to incoming Afro-Arabians; they behavior of hybrids. Yet we still have only a very
almost certainly could. It is certainly not the neoes- general notion of the amount of gene-flow between
sentialist (Cartmill, personal communication) red these populations, the breadth of the genetic hybrid
herring of whether or not they were “really” differ- zone, and the factors that determine its structure.
ent species. The important questions are purely em- This suggests that it is hardly worth getting too
pirical: first, whether they actually did contribute exercised about similar problems with fossil Homo,
any distinctive alleles to the incoming population, or with any other fossil taxon for that matter. We
and second, whether any of these have survived can, of course, define convenient paleospecies by
post-Pleistocene upheavals in the human gene-pool. carving the continuum of spatial and temporal vari-
The first question can only be answered by genetic ation into chunks that roughly match the diversity
200 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 44, 2001
of whatever living taxa one considers to provide the DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS
best analogy. There is no harm in this, provided that
I have argued that paleoanthropology could take
we recognize that we have not thereby answered
greater advantage of analogies drawn from across
many biological questions (about degree of phenetic
biological science, to supplement the insights that it
overlap or separation; about the position, breadth,
has always drawn from the anthropocentric sci-
and permeability of hybrid zones; and about the
ences. I have also tried to illustrate with a few ex-
existence of population structure defined by mating
amples that the papionin monkeys (especially the
preferences and probabilities) that complicate the
baboons) are an unusually valuable source of such
definition and diagnosis of extant species.
analogies, especially for the earlier, less “human”
periods in hominin evolution. Apart from particular
analogies, the major lesson to be drawn from the
papionins (in fact, much of it could be derived from
any widespread, diverse group of actively speciating,
terrestrial vertebrates) is that some areas of inquiry
are likely to be difficult to resolve for hominins,
simply because most hominin lineages have no liv-
ing representative. The areas that will challenge
paleontologists most severely include the detailed
cladistics of closely related extinct forms, the popu-
lation structure of the genus Homo for much of its
history, the dynamics of most interpopulational hy-
brid zones and replacement scenarios, and, of
course, to the extent that it depends on such infor-
mation, the species-level taxonomy of the hominins.
The use of analogy in the ways suggested here is
routine in zoology. The value of phylogenetically
distant analogues seems to be much less appreciated
by paleoanthropologists working on early hominins,
who seem to reach almost reflexively for the nearest
collection of Pan and Gorilla material, often without
considering whether alternative analogues might be
more appropriate. If they justify the ape analogue, it
is usually in terms of phylogenetic proximity: a sure
sign, as I have attempted to show, that the true
value of analogy has been misunderstood.
The practice of drawing analogies from wherever
they can be found in nature is an application of a
very broad scientific principle, that particular cases
are to be explained wherever possible in terms of
general “laws,” and that such explanations are to be

Fig. 2. Distribution of ecologically related and presumably


adaptive traits in neighboring baboon allotaxa, chosen to be anal-
ogous to cranial and postcranial features believed to be diagnostic
of Neandertal and “modern” Homo sapiens. The pattern common
to both is the highly significant difference between mean values
for comparable age and sex categories in the two allotaxa, but
overlapping ranges of variation in which bimodality is hardly
observable, and where individuals would not be readily assigned
to the correct taxon. All data are from Awash National Park.
A: Relative toe length in male hamadryas and anubis baboons.
Mean and 95% confidence intervals for age categories. Within
each taxon cluster, age categories are (left to right): young juve-
niles, older juveniles and subadults, and adults. Note that there
are clearly significant age and intertaxon differences, concordant
with the ecology of the two forms: anubis, more arboreal, hamadr-
yas, more of a rock-climber. B: Same variables as a bivariate plot
of individuals. The mean difference is apparent, but only when
individuals are identified by taxon. C: Relative molar row length
in adult anubis and hamadryas baboons, both sexes. The index is
an approximation of the relative importance of molars in the
dentition. D: Same variables as a bivariate plot; sex-asociated
size clusters are apparent, but taxon clusters are not separate.
Jolly] PAPIONIN ANALOGIES FOR HOMININ EVOLUTION 201
preferred over particularistic, ad hoc explanations. LITERATURE CITED
It is not surprising, then, that analogies and com-
Alberts SC, Altmann J. 2001. Immigration and hybridization
parative studies that range widely across taxa, patterns of yellow and anubis baboons in and around Amboseli,
searching for broadly applicable generalizations, are Kenya. Am J Primatol 53:139 –154.
commonplace in vertebrate socioecology and paleon- Albrecht GH. 1977. Craniofacial morphology of the Sulawesi ma-
tology, as in biological science in general. What is caques. Basel: S. Karger, AG.
surprising is that many paleoanthropologists, espe- Arntzen JW, Wallis GP. 1991. Restricted gene-flow in a moving
hybrid zone of the newts Triturus cristatus and T. marmoratus
cially those whose subjects are human and prehu- in western France. Evolution 45:805– 826.
man hominins, seem suspicious of them. Perhaps Asfaw B, White TD, Lovejoy O, Latimer B, Simpson S, Suwa G.
the reason is historical rather than scientific or ra- 1999. Australopithecus garhi: a new species of early hominid
tional. Paleoanthropology, whose major roots lie in from Ethiopia. Science 284:629 – 635.
the anthropocentric disciplines of anthropology, ar- Avise JC. 1994. Molecular markers, natural history and evolu-
tion. London: Chapman and Hall.
chaeology, medicine, and human anatomy, rather Barton NH. 2001. Speciation. Trends Ecol Evol 16:325.
than in general biology (Spencer, 1982), sometimes Barton NH, Hewitt GM. 1985. Analysis of hybrid zones. Annu
seems to treat human (and by extension, prehuman) Rev Ecol Syst 16:113–148.
biological evolution as a phenomenon to which other Barton NH, Hewitt GM. 1989. Adaptation, speciation and hybrid
organisms have relevance only in proportion to their zones. Nature 341:497–503.
Benefit BR. 2000. Old World monkey origins and diversification:
phylogenetic proximity to humans. Chimpanzees an evolutionary study of diet and dentition. In: Whitehead PF,
and gorillas are not human, but they are the next Jolly CJ, editors. Old World monkeys. Cambridge: Cambridge
best thing, and other primates come a distant third. University Press. p 133–179.
Using more analogies drawn from nonhominid Bernstein IS. 1966. Naturally occurring primate hybrid. Science
sources would, I believe, open up the anthropological 154:1559 –1560.
Bernstein IS. 1968. Social status of two hybrids in a wild troop of
imagination, suggesting new and alternative direc- Macaca irus. Folia Primatol (Basel) 8:121–131.
tions to take, and new hypothetical scenarios and Bromage TG, Schrenk F, editors. 1999. African biogeography,
explanations to test. But analogies are not them- climate change, and human evolution. New York: Oxford Uni-
selves hypotheses; they are not “refuted” by showing versity Press.
that the correspondence between analogues is not Brunet M, Beauvilain A, Coppens Y, Heintz E, Moutaye AHE,
Pilbeam D. 1995. The first australopithecine 2,500 kilometers
exact—which, of course, it can never be. In fact, west of the Rift Valley (Chad). Nature 378:273–275.
divergences between analogues may be as informa- Bynum EL, Bynum DZ, Froehlich JW, Supriatna J. 1997. Revised
tive as resemblances, especially when viewed geographic ranges and hybridization in Macaca tonkeana and
against the backdrop of the parallel resemblances: Macaca hecki. Trop Biodivers 4:275–283.
the molar crowns and brain size of geladas are a case Carr SM, Ballinger SW, Derr JN, Blankenship LH, Bickham JW.
1986. Mitochondrial DNA analysis of hybridization between
in point. One of the most difficult aspects of the sympatric whitetailed and mule deer in West Texas. Proc Natl
study of human evolution has always been to recon- Acad Sci USA 83:9576 –9580.
cile the fact that early hominins were more like Cartmill M. 1994. A critique of homology as a morphological
living humans than are any living species, with the concept. Am J Phys Anthropol 94:115–123.
concept that they were not themselves human. Churchill SE, Smith FH. 2000. Makers of the Early Aurignacian
of Europe. Yrbk Phys Anthropol 43:61–115.
Moreover, the “mix” of human and nonhuman itself Collard M, Aiello LC. 2000. From forelimbs to two legs. Nature
evolved in the lineage from which H. sapiens is de- 404:339 –340.
rived: by the beginning of the late Pleistocene, hu- Collard M, Wood B. 2000. How relaible are human phylogenetic
mans can be considered mentally “like us,” yet closer hypotheses? Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 97:5003–5006.
to many nonhuman species in terms of population Cronin JE, Sarich VM. 1976. Molecular evidence for dual origin of
mangabeys among Old World monkeys. Nature 260:700 –702.
structure and diversity. Though no living species Crook JH, Aldrich-Blake P. 1968 Ecological and behavioural con-
can provide an exact analogy for these intermediate trast between sympatric ground dwelling primates in Ethiopia.
stages, the baboons can, I think, often provide in- Folia primatol. 8:192–227.
sights into the nonhuman element. Darwin CD. 1871. The descent of man. London: Charles Murray
(3rd ed, 1877).
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS De Heinzelin J, Clark JD, White T, Hart W, Renne P, WoldeGa-
briel G, Beyene Y, Vrba E. 1999. Environment and behavior of
To be given the opportunity to parade one’s hobby 2.5-million-year-old Bouri hominids. Science 284:625– 629.
horses before a captive audience is a rare treat. I am Delson E. 1993. The fossils from Africa and India and the taxon-
most grateful to Mark Teaford for inviting me to give omy of the genus. In: Jablonski NG, editor. Theropithecus: rise
the AAPA lunchtime talk, to the colleagues who sat and fall of a primate genus. Cambridge: Cambridge University
through it, to Chris Ruff who invited this version Press. p 157–190.
Disotell TR. 1996. The phylogeny of Old World monkeys. Evol
and endured the editorial process, to three anony- Anthropol 5:18 –24.
mous reviewers who provided invaluable sugges- Disotell TR. 1999. Origins of modern humans still look recent.
tions, and to the many colleagues from whose work, Curr Biol 9:647– 650.
over the years, the ideas contained in this paper Disotell TR. 2000. Molecular systematics of the Cercopithecidae.
were derived. Particular thanks are due to Todd In: Whitehead PF, Jolly CJ, editors. Old World monkeys. Cam-
bridge: Cambridge University Press. p 29 –56.
Disotell, Tim Newman, Jane Phillips-Conroy, Ryan Duarte C, Mauricio J, Pettitt PB, Souto P, Trinkaus E, van der
Raaum, Jeff Rogers, and Derek Wildman for gener- Plicht H, Zilhao J. 1999. The early Upper Paleolithic human
ously sharing unpublished data. skeleton from the Abrigo do Lagar Velho (Portugal) and mod-
202 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 44, 2001
ern human emergence in Iberia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96: Hewitt GM. 1996. Some genetic consequences of ice ages, and
7604 –7609. their role in divergence and speciation. Biol J Linn Soc 58:247–
Dunbar RIM, Dunbar P. 1974. On hybridization between Thero- 276.
pithecus gelada and Papio anubis in the wild. J Hum Evol Hewitt GM. 1999. Post-glacial recolonization of European biota.
3:187–192. Biol J Linn Soc 68:87–112.
Dutrillaux B. 1980. Chromosomal evolution of the great apes and Hewitt GM. 2001. Speciation, hybrid zones and phylogeogra-
man. J Reprod Fertil [Suppl] 2. phy— or seeing genes in space and time. Mol Ecol 10:537–549.
Dutrillaux B, Couturier J, Muleris M, Lombard M, Chauvier G. Hewitt-Emmett D, Cook CN, Barnicot NA. 1976. Old World mon-
1982. Chromosomal phylogeny of forty-two species or subspe- key hemoglobins: deciphering phylogeny from complex pat-
cies of Cercopithecoids (Primates, Catarrhini). Ann Genet 25: terns of molecular evolution. In: Goodman M, Tashian RE,
96 –109. editors. Molecular anthropology. New York: Plenum. p 257–
Evans BJ, Morales JC, Supriatna J, Melnick DJ. 1999. Origin of 275.
the Sulawesi macaques (Cercopithecidae: Macaca) as sug- Hey J. 2001. The mind of the species problem. Trends Ecol Evol
gested by mitochondrial DNA phylogeny. Biol J Linn Soc 66: 16:326 –329.
539 –560. Hill WCO. 1970. Primates: comparative anatomy and taxonomy:
Fleagle JG, McGraw WS. 1999. Skeletal and dental morphology a monograph. Volume VIII: Cynopithecinae: Papio, Mandrillus,
supports diphyletic origin of baboons and mandrills. Proc Natl Theropithecus. Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh Press.
Acad Sci USA 96:1157–1161. 982 p.
Fooden J. 1963. Rhesus and crab-eating macaques: intergrada- Hoelzer GA, Hoelzer MA, Melnick DJ. 1993. The evolutionary
tion in Thailand. Science 143:363–365. history of the sinica group of macaque monkeys as revealed by
Fooden J. 1976. Provisional classification and key to living spe- mtDNA restriction site analysis. Mol Phylogenet Evol 1:215–
cies of macaques (Primates: Macaca). Folia Primatol (Basel) 222.
25:225–236. Hoss M. 2000. Neanderthal population genetics. Nature 404:453–
Fooden J. 1980. Classification and distribution of living macaques 454.
(Macaca lacepede, 1799). In: Lindburg DG, editor. The ma- Hunt KD. 1994. The evolution of human bipedality: ecology and
caques: studies in ecology, behavior and evolution. New York: functional morphology. J Hum Evol 26:183–202.
Van Nostrand Reinhold Co. p 1–9. Huxley JS. 1932. Problems of relative growth. New York: Dial
Gee H. 1999. Hybrid camel bridges 11 million years. Nature Press.
Science Update, 990506:1. Jablonski NG, editor. 1993. Theropithecus: rise and fall of a
Goodall J. 1986. The chimpanzees of Gombe: patterns of behavior. primate genus. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Jauch A, Wienberg J, Stanyon R, Arnold N, Tofanelli S, Ishida T,
Goodman SJ, Barton NH, Swanson G, Abernethy K, Pemberton Cremer T. 1992 Reconstruction of genomic rearrangements in
great apes and gibbons by chromosome painting. Proc Natl
JM. 1999. Introgression through rare hybridization: a genetic
Acad Sci USA 89:8611– 8615.
study of a hybrid zone between red and sika deer (genus Cer-
Jolly CJ. 1966. Introduction to the Cercopithecoidea, with notes
vus) in Argyll, Scotland. Genetics 152:355–371.
on their use as laboratory animals. Symp Zool Soc Lond 17:
Gray AP. 1954 Mammalian hybrids: a check-list with bibliogra-
427– 457.
phy. Farnham Royal: Commonwealth Agricultural Bureaux.
Jolly CJ. 1965. The origins and specialisations of the long-faced
Grine FE, editor. 1988. Evolutionary history of the “robust” aus-
Cercopithecoidea. Ph.D. thesis, University of London.
tralopithecines. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Jolly CJ. 1967. The evolution of the baboons. In: Vagtborg H,
Grine FE, Kay RF. 1988. Early hominid diets from quantitative
editor. The baboon in medical research, volume 2. Austin: Uni-
image analysis of dental microwear. Nature 333:765–768.
versity of Texas Press. p 23–50
Groves CP. 1978. Phylogenetic and population systematics of the Jolly CJ. 1970a. The seed-eaters: a new model of hominid differ-
mangabeys (Primates: Cercopithecoidea). Primates 19:l–34. entiation based on a baboon analogy. Man 5:1–26.
Groves CP. 2001. Primate taxonomy. Jolly CJ. 1970b. The large African monkeys as an adaptive array.
Grubb P. 1999. Evolutionary processes implicit in distribution In: Napier J, Napier P, editors. Old World monkeys— evolution,
patterns of modern African mammals. In: Bromage TG, systematics, and behavior. New York: Academic Press. p 227–
Schrenk F, editors. African biogeography, climate change, and 262.
human evolution. New York: Oxford University Press. p 150 – Jolly CJ. 1972a. The classification and natural history of Thero-
164. pithecus (Simopithecus) (Andrews, 1916), baboons of the Afri-
Gundling T, Hill A. 2000. Geological context of fossil Cercopithe- can Plio-Pleistocene. Bull Br Mus Nat Hist (Geol) 22:1–123.
coidea from eastern Africa. In: Whitehead PF, Jolly CJ, editors. Jolly CJ. 1972b. Changing views of hominid origins Yrbk Phys
Old World monkeys. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Anthropol 16:1–17.
p 180 –213. Jolly CJ. 1993. Species, subspecies, and baboon systematics. In:
Haile-Selassie Y. 2001. Late Miocene hominids from the Middle Kimbel WH, Martin LB, editors. Species, species concepts, and
Awash, Ethiopia. Nature 412:178 –181. primate evolution. New York: Plenum Press. p 67–107.
Harpending HC, Batzer MA, Gurven M, Jorde LB, Rogers AR, Jolly CJ, Phillips-Conroy JE. 2001. Testicular size, mating sys-
Sherry ST. 1998. Genetic traces of ancient demography. Proc tem, and maturation schedules in wild anubis and hamadryas
Natl Acad Sci USA 95:1161–1167. baboons. Int J Primatol (in press).
Harris EE, Disotell TR. 1998. Nuclear gene trees and the phylog- Jolly CJ, Plog F. 1987. Physial anthropology and archeology, 3rd
eny of mangabeys (Primates: Papionini). Mol Biol Evol 15:892– ed. New York: Random House.
900. Jolly CJ, Woolley-Barker T, Beyene S, Disotell TR Phillips-Con-
Harrison RG. 1993. Hybrid zones and the evolutionary process. roy JE. 1997. Intergeneric hybrid baboons. Int J Primatol 18:
Oxford: Oxford University Press. 597– 627.
Haszpruner G. 1998. Parsimony analysis as a specific kind of Kalb JE, Jolly CJ, Tebedge S, Mebrate A, Smart C, Oswald EB,
homology estimation and the implications for character weight- Whitehead PF, Wood CB, Adefris T, Rawn-Schatzinger V. 1982.
ing. Mol Phylogenet Evol 9:333–339. Vertebrate faunas from the Awash Group, Middle Awash Val-
Hawks J, Wolpoff MH. 2001. Paleoanthropology and the popula- ley, Afar, Ethiopia. J Vertebr Paleontol 2:237–258.
tion genetics of ancient genes. Am J Phys Anthropol 114:269 – Kaplan JR, Phillips-Conroy J, Fontenot MB, Jolly CJ, Fairbanks
272. LA, Mann JJ. 1999. Cerebrospinal fluid monoaminergic metab-
Hawks J, Hunley K, Lee S-H, Wolpoff M. 2000. Population bot- olites differ in wild anubis and hybrid (anubis/hamadryas) ba-
tlenecks and Pleistocene human evolution. Mol Biol Evol 17:2– boons: possible relationships to life history and behavior. Neu-
22. ropsychopharmacology 20:517–524.
Jolly] PAPIONIN ANALOGIES FOR HOMININ EVOLUTION 203
Kingdon J. 1997. The Kingdon field guide to African mammals. Parham P, Arnett KL, Adams EJ, Barber LD, Domena JD, Stew-
San Diego: Academic Press. art D, Hildebrand WH, Little AM. 1994. The HLA-B73 antigen
Kluge A. 1989. A concern for evidence and a phylogenetic hypoth- has a most unusual structure that defines a second lineage of
esis of relationships among Epicrates (Boidae, Serpentes). Syst HLA-B alleles. Tissue Antigens 43:302–313.
Zool 38:17–25. Peters CH. 1982. Nut-like oil seeds: food for monkeys, apes, and
Krings M, Stone A., Schmitz RW, Krainitzki H, Stoneking M, probably ape-men. Am J Phys Anthropol 57:283–301.
Paabo S. 1997. Neandertal DNA sequences and the origin of Phillips-Conroy JE. 1978. Dental variability in Ethiopian ba-
modern humans. Cell 90:19 –30. boons: an examination of the anubis hamadryas hybrid zone in
Kummer H. 1968. Social organization of hamadryas baboons. the Awash National Park, Ethiopia. Ph.D. thesis, New York
Chicago: Aldine. University.
Kurtén B. 1978. Dance of the tiger: a novel of the Ice Age. Phillips-Conroy JE, Jolly CJ, Nystrom P, Hemmalin HA. 1992.
Leakey LSB, Whitworth T. 1958. Notes on the genus Simopithe- Migration of male hamadryas baboons into anubis groups in
cus with description of a new species from Olduvai Occ. Pap the Awash National Park, Ethiopia. Int J Primatol 13:455– 476.
Coryndon Mem Mus 6:1–14. Rieseberg LH. 2001. Chromosomal rearrangements and specia-
Leakey MG, Feibel CS, McDougall I, Walker AC. 1995. New tion Trends Ecol Evol 16:351–358.
four-million-year-old hominid species from Kanapoi and Allia Rosas A. 2001. Occurrence of Neanderthal features in mandibles
Bay, Kenya. Nature 376:565–571. from the Atapuerca-SH site. Am J Phys Anthropol 114:74 –91.
Leakey MG, Spoor F, Brown FH, Gathogo PN, Kiarie C, Leakey Ruvolo M, Disotell TR, Allard MW, Brown WM, Honeycutt RL.
LN, McDougall I. 2001. New hominin genus from eastern Af- 1991. Resolution of the African hominoid trichotomy by use of
rica shows diverse middle Pliocene lineages. Nature 410:433– a mitochondrial gene sequence. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 88:
440. 1570 –1574.
Lee-Thorp JA, van der Merwe NJ. 1993. Stable carbon isotope Saino N, Lorenzini R, Fusco G, Randi E. 1992. Genetic variability
studies of Swartkrans fossils. In: Brain CK, editor. Swartkrans. in a hybrid zone between carrion and hooded crows (Corvus
A cave’s chronicle of early man. Pretoria: Transvaal Museum. p corone corone and C. c. cornix Passeriformes, Aves) in north-
251–256. western Italy. Biochem Syst Ecol 20:605– 613.
Le Gros Clark WE. 1955. The fossil evidence for human evolution. Samuels A, Altmann J. 1986. Immigration of a Papio anubis male
Chicago. into a group of cynocephalus baboons and evidence for an
Lehman N, Eisenhawer A, Hansen K, Mech LD, Peterson RO, anubis-cynocephalus hybrid zone in Amboseli, Kenya. Int J
Gogan PJP, Wayne RK. 1991. Introgression of coyote mitochon- Primatol 7:131–138.
drial DNA into sympatric North American gray wolf popula- Schaller GB, Lowther GR. 1969. The relevance of social carnivore
tions. Evolution 45:104 –119. behavior to the study of early hominids. Southwest J Anthropol
Lockwood CA, Fleagle JG. 1999. The recognition and evaluation 25:307–341.
of homoplasy in primate and human evolution Yrbk Phys An- Schluter D. 2001. Ecology and the origin of species. Trends Ecol
thropol 42:189 –232. Evol 16:372–380.
Maples WR, McKern TW. 1967. A preliminary report on classifi- Schluter D, Nagel LM. 1995. Parallel speciation by natural selec-
cation of the Kenya baboon. In: Vagtborg H, editor. The baboon tion. Am Nat 146:292–301.
in medical research, volume 2. San Antonio: University of Searle JB. 1998. Speciation, chromosomes and genomes Genome
Texas Press. p 1322. Res 8:1–3.
Markarjan DS, Isakov EP, Kondakov GI. 1974. Intergeneric hy- Skelton RR, McHenry HM. 1992. Evolutionary relationships
brids of the lower (42chromosomal) monkey species of the among early hominids. J Hum Evol 23:309 –349.
Sukhumi Monkey Colony. J Hum Evol 3:247–255. Spencer F, editor. 1982. A history of American physical anthro-
Martin RD. 1993. Allometric aspects of skull morphology in pology, 1930 –1980. New York: Academic Press.
Theropithecus. In: Jablonski NG, editor. Theropithecus: rise Spenser LM. 1997. Dietary adaptations of Plio-Pleistocene Bovi-
and fall of a primate genus. Cambridge: Cambridge University dae: implications for hominid habitat use. J Hum Evol 32:201–
Press. p 273–298. 228.
McCollum MA. 1999. The robust australopithecine face: a mor- Strait DS. 2001. Integration, phylogeny, and the hominid cranial
phogenetic perspective. Science 284:301–305. base. Am J Phys Anthropol 114:273–297.
Melnick DJ, Hoelzer GA. 1992. Differences in male and female Strait DS, Grine FE, Moniz MA. 1997. A reappraisal of early
macaque dispersal lead to contrasting distributions of nuclear hominid phylogeny. J Hum Evol 32:17– 82.
and mitochondrial DNA variation. Int J Primatol 13:379 –393. Strasser E, Delson E. 1987. Cladistic analysis of cercopithecid
Moore CM, Jaquish C, Eddy CA, Hubbard GB, Leland MM, relationships. J Hum Evol 16:8l–99.
Rogers J. 1999. Cytogenetic and fertility studies of a rheboon, Struhsaker TT, Butynski TM, Lwanga JS. 1988. Hybridization
rhesus macaque (Macaca mulatta) ⫻ baboon (Papio hamadr- between redtail (Cercopithecus ascanius schmidti) and blue (C.
yas) cross: further support for a single karyotype nomenclature. mitis stuhlmanni) monkeys in the Kibale Forest, Uganda. In:
Am J Phys Anthropol 110:119 –127. Gautier Hion A, Bourlière F, Gautier JP, editors. A primate
Napier JR. 1980. Hands. London: Allen & Unwin. radiation: evolutionary biology of the African guenons. Cam-
Newman TK, Jolly CJ, Rogers J. 2001. Mitochondrial DNA se- bridge: Cambridge University Press.
quence variation among baboons (Papio hamadryas): phyloge- Sugawara K. 1979. Sociological study of a wild group of hybrid
netic and phylogeographic analysis (submitted). baboons between Papio anubis and Papio hamadryas in the
Nickerson E, Nelson DL. 1998. Molecular definition of pericentric Awash Valley, Ethiopia. Primates 20:21–56.
inversion breakpoints occurring during the evolution of hu- Swisher CC III, Rink WJ, Antón SC, Schwarcz HP, Curtis GH,
mans and chimpanzees. Genomics 50:368 –372. Suprijo H, Widiasmoro. 1996. Latest Homo erectus of Java:
Nystrom PD. 1992. Mating success of hamadryas, anubis and potential contemporaneity with Homo sapiens in Southeast
hybrid male baboons in a “mixed” social group in the Awash Asia. Science 274:1870 –1874.
National Park, Ethiopia. Ph.D. dissertation, Washington Uni- Tattersall I. 1993. Speciation and morphological differentiation in
versity. the genus Lemur. In: Kimbel W, Martin L, editors. Species,
O’Neill RJW, Eldridge MDB, Graves JAM. 2001. Chromosome species concepts, and primate evolution. New York: Wiley.
heterozygosity and de novo chromosome rearrangements in Tattersall I, Eldredge N. 1977. Fact, theory and fantasy in human
mammalain interspecies hybrids. Mammalian Genome evolution Am Sci 65:204 –211.
12:256 –259. Taylor EB, McPhail JD. 1999. Evolutionary history of an adaptive
Ovchinnikov IV, Gotherstrom A, Romanova GP, Kharitonov VM, radiation in species pairs of threespine sticklebacks (Gasteros-
Linden K, Goodwin W. 2000. Molecular analysis of Neander- teus): insights from mitochondrial DNA. Biol J Linn Soc 66:
thal DNA from the northern Caucasus. Nature 404:490 – 493. 271–291.
204 YEARBOOK OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY [Vol. 44, 2001
Tchernov E. 1989. The Middle Paleolithic mammalian sequence Walker AC. 1981. Dietary hypotheses and human evolution. Phi-
and its bearing on the origin of Homo sapiens in the Southern los Trans R Soc Lond [Biol] 292:57– 64.
Levant. In: BarYosef O, Vandermeersch B, editors. Investiga- Weiss KM, Buchanan A. 2000. Rediscovering Darwin after a
tions in South Levantine prehistory. Préhistoire du Sud-Le- Darwinian century. Evol Anthropol.
vant. BAR Int Ser 497:25– 42. Weiss KM. 1994. A toe, a tooth, and a vertebra: the genetic
Teaford MF, Ungar PS. 2000. Diet and the evolution of the ear- dimensions of complex morphological traits Evol Anthropol
liest human ancestors. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 97:13506 – 2:121–137.
13511. White TD, Suwa G, Asfaw B. 1994. Australopithecus ramidus, a
Templeton AR. 1989. The meaning of species and speciation: a new species of early hominid from Aramis, Ethiopia. Nature
genetic perspective. In: Otte D, Endler J, editors. Speciation 371:306 –312.
and its consequences. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates. Wildman D. 2000. Ph.D. dissertation, New York University.
Templeton AR. 1998. Nested clade analyses of phylogeographic Williams-Blangero S, Vanderberg J, Blangero J, Konigsberg L,
data and testing hypotheses about gene flow and population Dyke B. 1990. Genetic differentiation between baboon subspe-
history. Mol Ecol 7:381–397. cies: relevance for biomedical research. Am J Primatol 20:67–
Thorington RW, Groves CP. 1970. An annotated classification of
81.
the Cercopithecoidea. In: Napier JR, Napier P, editors. The Old
Wolpoff MH. 1973. Posterior tooth size, body size, and diet in
World monkeys. New York: Academic Press. p 629 – 647.
South African gracile australopithecines. Am J Phys An-
Tosi AJ, Morales JC, Melnick DJ. 2000. Comparison of Y-chromo-
some and mtDNA phylogenies leads to unque inferences of ma- thropol. 39:375–394.
caque evolutionary history. Mol Phylogenet Evol 17:133–144. Wolpoff MH. 1999. Paleoanthropology, 2nd ed. Boston: McGraw-
Trinkaus E. 1991. Les hommes fossiles de la Grotte de Shanidar, Hill.
Irak: evolution et continuité parmi les hommes archaiques Wolpoff MH, Thorne AG, Jelinek J, Zhang Y. 1993. The case for
tardifs du Proche-Orient. Anthropologie 95:535–572. sinking Homo erectus: 100 years of Pithecanthropus is enough!
Trinkaus E. 1993. Variability in the position of the mandibular Courier Forschungsinstitut Senckberg 171:772–773.
mental foramen and the identification of Neandertal apomor- Wolpoff MH, Hawks J, Caspari R. 2000. Multiregional, not mul-
phies. Riv Antropol 71:259 –274. tiple origins. Am J Phys Anthropol 112:129 –136.
Turelli M, Barton NH, Coyne JA. 2001. Theory and speciation. Wood BA, Brooks A. 1999. We are what we ate. Nature 400:219 –
Trends Ecol Evol 16:330 –343. 220.
Van Tuinen P, Mootnick AR, Kingswood SC, Hale DW, Kuamoto Wood BA, Collard M. 1999. The genus Homo. Science 284:65–71.
AT. 1999. Complex, compound inversion/translocation poly- Wrangham RW, Peterson D. 1996. Demonic males: apes and the
morphism in an ape: presumptive intermediate stage in the origins of human violence. New York: Mariner Books.
karyotypic evolution of the agile gibbon Hylobates agilis. Am J Zinner D, Hapke A. 2000. Ecological comparisons of primates in
Phys Anthropol 110:129 –142. Eritrea. Poster, at www.biozentrum.uni-wuerzburg.de.

Вам также может понравиться