Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 23

2

ambiguous distribution. While [:] generally patterns as a


THE TENSE-LAX DISTINCTION IN ENGLISH VOWELS AND THE ROLE OF
PAROCHIAL AND ANALOGICAL CONSTRAINTS tense vowel, it is allowed before [N] and tautomorphemic

[ft], which are otherwise lax-only environments. And while


Antony Dubach Green
[A] generally patterns as a lax vowel, it may stand in
University of Potsdam
tense-only environments in recent loanwords. More surpris-

ingly, lax [A] may stand before [N] and tautomorphemic [ft]
1. Introduction and theoretical background
only in recent loanwords, even though other lax vowels
The vast majority of the work that has been done in Opti- stand freely in these positions. There are also some va-
mality Theory (McCarthy & Prince 1993, Prince & Smolensky rieties of EGA that have the diphthong [E«] which patterns
1993) has focused, sometimes directly, sometimes indi- as a tense vowel; this can stand before [mp ft sk sp],
rectly, on the interaction between markedness (or well- although usually only lax vowels may stand there. I will
formedness) constraints and faithfulness constraints. The argue that these exceptions to well-formedness are attrib-
question of lexical exceptions to patterns of phonotactic utable to the influence of a network of connections between
well-formedness has been somewhat less often explored (but lexical items, concretely represented in the theory as a
see, for example, Tranel 1996, Inkelas et al. 1997, Inkelas web of conjoined output-output (OO) correspondence con-
1999), but in this paper I intend to investigate not only straints known as analogical constraints (Myers 1999).
lexical exceptions, but cases where phonotactic well- More isolated lexical exceptions are attributed to the
formedness is regularly violated by certain vowel + conso- influence of morpheme-specific parochial constraints.1 This
nant sequences in most words (including the most common theory will be developed further in Green (in prep.); below
ones), while it is obeyed only in a handful of rare (mostly

foreign) words.
1 Since many of the constraints discussed here refer specifically
As will be discussed in §2, the dichotomy between
to English lexical items, they can hardly be said to be universal.
tense and lax vowels is blurred in the low back region in Although the proposal that all constraints are universal belongs to the
founding tenets of Optimality Theory, some recent work (e.g. Boersma
Eastern General American English (henceforth EGA): While
2000, Ellison 2000) has argued against it. My personal belief is that
there are certain environments in which tense vowels are while constraints on phonological markedness are universal, constraints
referring to lexical and/or morphological properties are language-
prohibited, and other environments in which lax vowels are
specific. See Green (in prep.) for more on this issue.
prohibited, the low back vowels in EGA [:] and [A] show an

To appear in Linguistics in Potsdam No. 15, September 2001.


3 4

I give a brief sketch of the basic idea. the corresponding cardinal vowel: [hIt], [bEt], [sUt],

The role that analogical constraints and parochial [bÃt]. The tense vowel in each case is long, has a quality

constraints play in this analysis demonstrates an important more nearly that of the cardinal vowel, and may tend to
consequence for Optimality Theory: There is more to pho- diphthongization, this tendency being greater in some dia-

nology than just the interaction between markedness and lects than in others: [hi:t ~ hIit], [be:t ~ beIt], [su:t ~

faithfulness constraints, since constraints can also en- sUut], [bo:t ~ boUt ~ b«Ut]. Tenseness and length usually

courage the proliferation of a phonologically marked pat- co-occur in English: Lax vowels are short, while tense
tern, and can also require specific lexical items to have a vowels are long (in stressed syllables).

certain phonological shape. I shall not be concerned here with the articulatory or
The organization of the paper is as follows. In §2.1 acoustic differences between tense and lax vowels; for a

the distribution of tense and lax vowels in EGA is de- review of the debate the reader is referred to Halle

scribed and in §2.2 is given an OT-based analysis. In §2.3 (1977), and for arguments against the existence of the

lexical exceptions to the usual pattern are discussed and feature [tense] in English to chapter 1 of Lass (1976).

analyzed. In §3 the exceptional behavior of the two low Instead, I use purely distributional criteria to classify
back vowels, lax [A] and tense [:] is described and ana- vowels into the groups "tense" and "lax". An arbitrary

lyzed. In §4 the analysis is extended to the tense vowel labeling could have also been used, such as that of Wells
[E«] present in some varieties of EGA. §5 summarizes and (1982): His "part-system A" corresponds to the vowels

concludes the paper. usually called lax: [I], [E], [Q], [U], [Ã], [A] (= Brit-

ish []); his "part-system B" corresponds to those "tense"


2. Distribution of tense and lax vowels in English
vowels and diphthongs that end in the high front region:

[i:], [e:/eI], [aI], [I]; "part-system C" corresponds to


2.1 Description
those "tense" vowels and diphthongs that end in the high
English is generally described as having a distinction back region: [u:], [o:/oU], [aU]; and "part-system D"
between tense and lax vowels. Minimal pairs such as hit- corresponds to those "tense" vowels and diphthongs that
heat, bet-bait, soot-suit, butt-boat illustrate this con- have relatively open qualities: [:], British [Î:], Brit-
trast. In each pair, the lax vowel has a short, monoph- ish [A:], and the British centering diphthongs [I«], [E«],
thongal pronunciation rather centralized with respect to [U«]. For EGA I would consider that "part-system D" in-
5 6

cludes [:], [r`:], [E«] (in the dialects that have it; see Halle & Mohanan (1985), Borowsky (1986, 1989), Hammond

§4 for discussion), and the rhotacized diphthongs [ir], (1999). Tense vowels may stand in stressed final open

[Er], [ur], [r], [Ar]. (See Green 2001 for arguments that syllables and lax vowels may not, but otherwise the distri-
these "r-colored vowels" have the status of diphthongs in bution seems to have little to do with syllable structure

American English.) (as was argued for German by Féry 1997 and for French by

Using distributional rather than phonetic criteria for Féry, this volume). Another environment where tense but

this vowel classification allows us to avoid the problem not lax vowels may stand is in stressed final syllables
that some vowels and diphthongs behave like tense vowels closed by [D] or [Z]. Examples of (usually) tense-only

but are phonetically more similar to lax vowels. Thus, environments are shown in (1).3 For the moment, I abstract

there is phonetically nothing "tense" about the members of away from the low back vowels [A, :].

Wells's part-system D,2 but they have the same distribution

as the members of part-systems B and C, not the same as (1) Distribution of vowels in stressed final open sylla-
part-system A. Since I am not concerned here with the bles and before final [D, Z]
differences between part-systems B, C, and D, I will con- [i:] see, tree, be, wreathe, liege

tinue to use the traditional labels "lax" and "tense" in [e:] day, play, way, bathe, beige

this paper. [u:] too, do, who, soothe, rouge


The distribution of tense and lax vowels in English [o:] sew, know, toe, loathe, loge
has been discussed by a variety of authors, including Chom-
sky & Halle (1968), Kahn (1976), Lass (1976), Halle (1977),
3 My sources for the words illustrating the distribution of vow-
els, not counting my own introspection, are Wood (1936), Wells (1982),
Hammond (1999), and Muthmann (1999). EGA, the dialect I focus on here,
2 Including, of course, [:]. The symbol [] is used to represent has the following properties: It is rhotic (i.e. [r] may be present
a mid lax vowel in many languages (e.g. German, French), but in English outside syllable onsets); it has merged the [A:] and [] of British
[:] really does pattern as a tense vowel, namely the tense counterpart English into [A], so that balm and bomb are homophonous as [bAm], and
to British []/American [A]. For this reason, I consider [:] to be a father and bother rhyme as [fADr`, bADr`]. Unlike many other dialects of
low tense vowel. Wells (1982: 145) points out that American [:] is North American English, EGA as examined here has not merged the [:]
quite open, falling between cardinal [] and cardinal [], and in Wells and [] of British English, so that caught [k:t] and cot [kAt] are
(1990) he even uses the symbol [:] to stand for the EGA pronunciation distinct. Many varieties of EGA have an additional tense vowel [E«],
of the vowel in words like thought. which I discuss below in §4.
7 8

Note: The lax vowels [I], [E], [U], [Ã], [Q] are generally the surface representation of underlying /ng/, so it might

prohibited in these environments, but cf. [ED] (name be considered just another instance of a noncoronal clus-

of the letter "D") and the French loanword cortège ter; but in my opinion this view is untenable because of
which may be pronounced with final [EZ] or [e:Z].4 pairs like anger [QNgr`] vs. hangar [hQNr`], lingam [lINg«m]

vs. gingham [gIN«m], dingo [dINgo] vs. dinghy [dINi]6, so I


Note that the (nearly absolute) prohibition of lax vowels shall consider [N] an independent phoneme. The restriction
before [D] and [Z] applies only when these consonants are to lax vowels before noncoronal clusters and [N] holds both
in word-final position. Word internally, lax vowels may when the consonants are word-final and when they are inter-
stand before them, as in gather, azure; feather, measure; vocalic.
wither, vision; mother.

Conversely, lax vowels may stand before [N] and before (2) Distribution of vowels before noncoronal clusters and
certain consonant clusters containing one noncoronal conso- [N]
nant (henceforth referred to as a "noncoronal cluster"); [I] lisp, whisper; eclipse, gypsy; script, triptych; lift,
tense vowels may not stand in these environments. The nifty; risk, whisker; mix, pixie; strict, victim;
clusters in question are those of stop + fricative, frica- filbert; sylph, pilfer; silver; film; milk; pilgrim;
tive + stop, and sonorant + obstruent.5 (Most obstruent + limp, simple; limber; link, trinket; linger; sing,
sonorant clusters can be preceded by both types of vowel.) gingham
The velar nasal [N] has traditionally been assumed to be [E] vesper; biceps, epilepsy; accept, Neptune; left; desk,

rescue; sex, exit; sect, nectar; help; Melba; shelf,


4 In many British dialects, with is pronounced [wID], but the belfry; twelve, velvet; elm, helmet; elk, welcome;
usual American pronunciation is [wIT].
hemp, tempest; ember; ginseng
5 Included among the noncoronal clusters considered here are [ps
ks pt kt]; it must be pointed out that tense vowels are prohibited only [U] pulpit; wolf
before tautomorphemic clusters. Tense vowels freely appear here when
[Ã] cusp; abrupt; tuft, mufti; tusk, musket; crux, buxom;
the [s] or [t] in such clusters forms an inflectional ending (peeps,
peaks, peeped, peaked). I do not have space here to develop an analy- duct; pulp, culprit; bulb; gulf, sulfur; culminate;
sis of this fact, but presumably an output-output constraint requiring
peeps etc. to have the same vowel as peep etc. outranks the constraint
against tense vowels before noncoronal clusters. 6 Some people pronounce this [dINgi], however.
9 10

bulk; vulgar; pump, trumpet; number; hunk, bunkum; The rhotacized diphthongs [ir Er Ar r ur] do not occur

hunger; tongue before [N] or, usually, noncoronal clusters (some excep-

[Q] hasp, jasper; draft, after; lapse; rapt, captain; tions listed below); neither do usually they occur before
mask, basket; ax, taxi; act, practice; scalp; album; word-final [D], [Z]. But they can stand at the end of

Ralph; valve; talc, falcon; amalgam; camp, pamper; stressed final syllables, implying that they have the dis-

amber; sank, Yankee; anger; fang, hangar tribution of tense vowels.

Note: [i:], [e:], [o:], [u:] usually prohibited in such


words, but cf. chamber, cambric, Cambridge, traipse (4) Distribution of rhotacized diphthongs: tense envi-

with [e:], coax, hoax with [o:] and (for some ronments
speakers only) rumba with [u:] (normally [rÃmb«] or [ir] peer, tear (n.), beer

[rUmb«]). [Er] pear, tear (v.), bear; concierge [kAnsiErZ]

[Ar] par, tar, bar

The syllabic rhotic [r`:] and the diphthongs [aI], [I], [r] pour, tore, boar

[aU] pattern with the tense vowels, as shown in (3). [ur] poor, tour, boor
Exceptions: (ant)arctic, coarctation, harpsichord, in-

(3) Distribution of [r`:] and the diphthongs: tense envi- farct, Marx with [Ar]; corpse, (ab-,
ronments ad)sorption/-sorptive with [r].

[r`:] spur, fir, myrrh (no examples before final [D] and

[Z]) To sum up, although tense and lax vowels can contrast in
[aI] die, try, buy, lithe (no examples before [Z]) stressed syllables that are closed by a single consonant

[I] boy, joy, annoy (no examples before [D] and [Z]) (other than [D, Z, N]) or by a consonant cluster in which

[aU] cow, allow, bough, mouth (vb.) (no examples before all members are coronal, in other environments the two sets
[Z]) are in complementary distribution. In word-final stressed

Note: These are usually prohibited before [N] and noncoro- syllables that are either open or closed by [D] or [Z],

nal clusters, but cf. excerpt, Xerxes with [r`:] and only tense vowels may occur. Before noncoronal clusters or

deixis/deictic with [aI]. [N], only lax vowels may occur (with some exceptions, as

noted above).
11 12

2.2 The basic constraint interaction traipse, which will be discussed below), all of these con-

straints are unviolated, and assuming that all outrank


These facts can be submitted to an OT analysis by means of
IDENT(tense), requiring output vowels to have the same
several interacting constraints. First of all, Foot Bi-
specification for [tense] as their corresponding inputs, it
narity (FTBIN) requires that feet be at least bimoraic.
does not matter whether underlying vowels are marked as
Second, TNS<->µµ says that vowels are tense if and only if
[+tense] or [-tense] in the environments where there is no
they are bimoraic.7 The constraint *3µ bans trimoraic syl-
contrast.9
lables. The constraint *D,Z/µ prohibits these two voiced
So, in stressed open syllables, FTBIN and TNS<->µµ con-
coronal fricatives from being moraic; as we see below, this
spire to permit only tense vowels to surface, as shown for
has the result of banning short lax vowels before syllable-
see in (5).10 Since [I] and [i:] cannot contrast in this
final [D, Z]. The constraint *TNSCLUS prohibits tense vowels
environment, it does not matter which of them is in the
before noncoronal clusters.8 Finally, the constraint N/µ
input, as only the tense [i:] can surface in the output.
requires [N] to be moraic, a constraint which could also

play a role in the cross-linguistic tendency to disfavor


[N] in onset position. In the data we have seen so far

(abstracting away from the handful of exceptions like

7 This constraint is regularly violated when tense vowels are un-


stressed: pretty [prIRi], yellow [jElo], virtue [vr`:tSu], etc. I will
not be further concerned with unstressed tense vowels here.
8 I stipulate the constraints *D,Z/µ and *TNSCLUS in order to skirt
the issue of precisely why tense and lax vowels have the distribution 9 There is a large literature on English syllable structure. Some
they do in these contexts. Hammond (1999) bases his analysis of these representative examples of this work are: Kahn (1976), Selkirk (1982),
facts on syllable structure, but I find unconvincing his proposal that Clements & Keyser (1983), Borowsky (1986), and Lamontagne (1993).
[N] and [Z] contribute two moras to the syllable, and in the case of 10 Tracy Hall (p.c.) points out that full lax vowels are prohibited
[Z] the first of these two moras must be shared with the preceding from word-final position in unstressed syllables as well in American
vowel. I further disagree with many of his intuitions regarding syl- English, e.g. silly [sIli], *[sIlI], and suggests that it is a constraint
labification (e.g. ambisyllabic [t] in active, ambisyllabic [k] in banning full lax vowels from word-final position rather than FTBIN that
alcove, bulky syllabified [bÃlk.i], etc.), upon which his analysis excludes *[sI] in (5). If FTBIN is responsible for *[sI], however, then
crucially depends. See Hall (2001) for a full review of Hammond some other constraint must rule out *[sIlI] -- perhaps a constraint
(1999). against full lax vowels in unstressed open syllables.
13 14

(5) (α) /sI/


FTBIN TNS<->µµ IDENT(tense) illustrates this for tongue and the tableau in (8) for
(β) /si:/
(α)
cusp.
sI * !
(β) *
(α) (7) (α) /tÃN/
sI: * ! *3µ TNS<->µµ N/µ IDENT(tense)
(β) *
(β) /to:N/
(α) *
si * ! * (α)
(β) ☞ tõNµ
(β) *
(α) *
☞ si: (α) *
(β) toµNµ * !
(β)
(α) *
to:µµN * !
(β)
Adding *D,Z/µ to the high-ranking constraints ensures that
(α) *
to:µµNµ * !
only tense vowels appear before these two consonants. The (β)

tableau in (6) illustrates this for beige, and it would be

the same for bathe. (8) (α) /kÃsp/


*3µ TNS<->µµ *TNSCLUS IDENT(tense)
(β) /ko:sp/
(α)
☞ kõspµ
(6) (α) /bEZ/
FTBIN *D,Z/µ TNS<->µµ IDENT(tense) (β) *
(β) /be:Z/ (α) *
koµspµ * ! *
(α) (β)
bEµZµ * !
(β) * (α) *
ko:µµspµ * ! *
(α) (β)
bEµZ * !
(β) * (α) *
ko:µµsp * !
(α) (β)
bE:µµZ * !
(β) *
(α) *
beµZµ * ! *
(β) In environments where tense and lax vowels contrast, namely
(α) *
beµZ * ! * in syllables closed by a single consonant other than [D, Z,
(β)
(α) * N] and in position before coronal clusters, the inputs must
☞ be:µµZ
(β) not be as rich as they are in (5)-(8), because IDENT(tense)
will be crucial in determining the optimal form. This need
In the environments where only lax vowels are permitted, not be problematic, though: If we assume pest has only the
TNS<->µµ conspires with *3µ, *TNSCLUS, N/µ to prohibit tense input /pEst/ and paste has only the input /pe:st/, the
vowels from the relevant contexts. The tableau in (7) desired surface forms will be judged optimal in each case.
15 16

The remaining question is how to deal with lexical excep- (9) Turkish coda devoicing

tions to the prohibition of tense vowels before noncoronal a. kAnAt 'wing'

clusters, like chamber and coax. kAnAtlAr (plural)


kAnAdµ (accusative)
2.3 Lexical exceptions
b. devlet 'state'

According to Inkelas (1995, 1996) and Inkelas et al. devletler (plural)

(1997), lexical exceptions to otherwise robust well- devleti (accusative)


formedness principles within a language are best treated by c. etyd 'study'

allowing a three-way underlying contrast between [+F], etydler (plural)


[-F], and [0F] and ordering the relevant faithfulness con- etydy (accusative)

straint above the relevant markedness constraint. This

enables the fully specified forms always to surface faith- (10) The analysis of Inkelas et al.

fully, while the underspecified form, which cannot surface a. /kAnAD/ FAITH CODA DEVOICING
☞ kAnAt *
faithfully (all features being fully specified as either + kAnAd * * !
or - on the surface), is subject to the markedness con-

straint. So, for example, most Turkish words are subject b. /devlet/ FAITH CODA DEVOICING
☞ devlet
to coda devoicing, as shown by the contrast between (9)a
devled * ! *
and (9)b, but some words are exempt from coda devoicing, as

shown in (9)c.11 The tableaux illustrating the analysis of c. /etyd/ FAITH CODA DEVOICING
etyt * !
Inkelas et al. are given in (10). (/D/ represents a stop
☞ etyd *
underspecified for voice.)

Inkelas et al. argue that this analysis is superior to a

rule-based one that requires co-phonologies, but it comes

at the cost of allowing a three-way underlying contrast


among [+voice], [-voice], and [0voice]. This is in viola-
11 See Artstein (1998) for further discussion of this example.
tion not only of the Contrastive Underspecification hy-
17 18

pothesis (Calabrese 1988), according to which features that ogy outlined in Green (in prep.), however, allows an analy-

contrast (e.g. [voice] in Turkish) cannot be left unspeci- sis of lexical exceptions that relies neither on co-

fied underlyingly, but also of the convention of (both phonologies nor on underspecification. Instead, parochial
contrastive and radical) underspecification theory, going constraints requiring particular morphemes to surface with

back to Stanley (1967), that assumes "strict binarity of particular features outrank the relevant markedness con-

feature specifications in underlying lexical representa- straint, which in turn outranks the general faithfulness

tions. In each environment, we can have at most [0F] and constraint.


[αF], where [-αF] is the value assigned by the most spe- In (1)-(4) I listed words showing that, for the most

cific rule (language-particular or universal) which is part, only lax vowels are permitted before tautomorphemic
applicable in that environment" (Kiparsky 1993: 285). noncoronal clusters, but there were some exceptions to this

That alone is worrying enough, but when we consider tendency. I repeat some of those exceptions here for con-

the arguments that have been presented against input under- venience.

specification within OT in general, the analysis becomes

even weaker. For example, Smolensky (1993) argues that in (11) Exceptions to the prohibition of tense vowels before
an OT approach to markedness, unmarked features are phonol- tautomorphemic noncoronal clusters

ogically inert not because they are absent the input (in a. "D" ED
fact, they are present there), but because they are liter- b. cortège krtEZ

ally unmarked, i.e. engender no violation marks under har- c. chamber tSe:mbr`

mony evaluation (cf. also Golston 1996). Smolensky's ap- d. coax ko:ks
proach would then not predict any difference between e. deixis/deictic daIksIs/daIktIk

[-voice] and [0voice] in obstruents. Itô et al. (1995), f. excerpt Eksr`:pt

examining redundant feature specifications (in particular g. rumba (for some) ru:mb«
[voice] in nasals), show that there is no requirement of h. traipse tre:ps

underlying feature minimization, implying that underlying i. corpse krps

[0voice] -- whether on sonorants or on obstruents -- is an

unnecessary and therefore undesirable tool. In a-b, *D,Z/µ is apparently violated; in c-i, *TNSCLUS is.

The nonderivational approach to morphology and phonol- As far as I am aware, there are no exceptional words that
19 20

violate N/µ (except words with [:N] discussed below). I 3. Ambiguity in low back vowels

therefore propose that the words in (11) have parochial

constraints requiring them to have the vowel with which 3.1 The distribution of [A] and [:]

they surface. For example, the constraint "traipse[e:]"


EGA has two low back vowels, [A] and [:]. Since [:] is
requires the lexical item traipse to surface with the vowel
longer than [A], and because [A] but not [:] may appear
[e:]. This constraint, and the ones holding for the other
before most noncoronal clusters (as will be discussed pres-
words in (11), outrank *D,Z/µ or *TNSCLUS, but there are
ently), it is attractive to consider these vowels a
apparently no parochial constraints outranking N/µ.
lax/tense pair like the ones discussed in §2.1. As shown
in (13), [A] and [:] contrast in environments where both
(12) /tre:ps/ N/µ traipse[e:] *D,Z/µ *TNSCLUS IDENT(tense)
☞ tre:ps * tense and lax vowels are permitted.
trEps * ! *

(13) Minimal pairs illustrating lax [A] vs. tense [:]


The majority of words, however, either have no such paro- collar kAlr` caller k:lr`
chial constraint, or else it so low ranking that it plays cot kAt caught k:t
no role.12 In this case, *TNSCLUS determines that the opti- stock stAk stalk st:k
mal candidate must have a lax vowel before a noncoronal don dAn dawn d:n
cluster, as we saw above in (8). knotty nARi naughty n:Ri
We have now analyzed the distribution of most tense

and lax vowels in EGA, including the lexical exceptions. However, unlike the pairs seen above, [A] and [:] may
In the next section we move to low back vowels [A] and contrast also in stressed open final syllables and before
[:], which we have ignored up to now, and whose distribu- [N]. (In stressed open final syllables, [A] is pronounced
tion blurs the distinction between tense and lax vowels. long.)

12 Alcántara (1998) argues that only high-ranking parochial con-


straints ("specific" constraints in his terminology) are present in the
grammar.
21 22

(14) Contrast of [A] and [:] in stressed open final syl- (16) Distribution of [:]

lables and before [N] Tense environments: jaw, law, saw

Shah SA: Shaw S: Lax environments: soft, long, bauxite, auction,
la lA: law l: auxiliary, auspice, auscultation,

pa pA: paw p: palfrey, Balkan, (for some) donkey,

ma mA: maw m: (for some) falcon

Hong Kong hAN kAN long l:N Other environments: thought, hawk, daub, cloth, cross, off
dugong dugAN gong g:N

In the next two subsections we will look at this ambiguous


Thus we see that both [A] and [:] can occur in environ- distribution in more detail and begin to form an analysis.

ments where only tense vowels are allowed, as well as in


3.2 Lax [A] in tense-only environments
environments where only lax vowels are allowed. The dis-

tribution of [A] and [:] is illustrated in (15)-(16). Let's begin with the distribution of [A], which we are

Note that [A] is pronounced long in environments where lax assuming to be [-tense]. In words like bra, spa, Shah,
vowels are prohibited, otherwise it is pronounced short; mirage, the constraint against long lax vowels appears to

[:] is pronounced long everywhere. be violated. Take for example the word spa. Given the
constraint hierarchy shown above in (5), even the input

(15) Distribution of [A] /spA/ should give the output *[sp:].

Tense environments: bra, spa, Shah, mirage (with long [A:])


Lax environments: wasp, copse, mosque, ox, opt, concoct, (17) Constraint hierarchy falsely predicts spa to be *[sp:]
/spA/ FTBIN TNS<->µµ IDENT(tense)
pomp, somber, conquer, conger, Hong
spA * !
Kong (with short [A]) spA: * !
Other environments: father, bother, balm, bomb, Mali, Molly , sp: *

(with short [A])


But following the analysis of lexical exceptions outlined

in §2.3, we may propose a high-ranking parochial constraint

specific to the lexical items spa and mirage requiring them


23 24

(α) /lA/
to have lax vowel: spa([-tense]) and mirage([-tense]). The (19) FTBIN TNS<->µµ IDENT(tense)
(β) /l:/
tableaux illustrating this analysis, given in (18), also
(α)
lA * !
show that FTBIN outranks TNS<->µµ, which was not provable (β) *
(α)
before. lA: * !
(β) *
(α) *
☞ l:
(18) Parochial constraints force lax [A] to show up in tense (β)

contexts
a. /spA/ FTBIN 3.3 Tense [:] in lax-only environments
spa([-tense]) TNS<->µµ IDENT(tense)
spA * !
☞ spA: *
We can now move on to the [:] cases. Notice in (16) that
sp: * ! * [:] is not permitted in all lax environments: It occurs

before [N] and [ft], for some people before [Nk], and in a
IDENT
b. /mIrAZ/ FTBIN mirage([-tense]) *D,Z/µ TNS<->µµ few isolated words like bauxite and auction but otherwise
(tense)
mI(rAµZµ) * ! not before noncoronal clusters. Also, if we compare words
mI(rAµZ) * !
that have [:] before [N] with those that have [A] before
☞ mI(rA:µµZ) *
mI(r:µµZ) * ! * [N] we see that most words have [:], but some foreign
words can vary between [A] and [:] (i.e. some speakers use

It is especially interesting that all words in which [A] [A] and others use [:]). Before [Ng] plus vowel, however,

appears in contexts otherwise restricted to tense vowels [A] is more common than [:].

are either recent loanwords like spa and mirage or hypo-


coristics like ma and pa. Ordinary native words like law, (20) Distribution of [:] and [A] before [N]

on the other hand, need no parochial constraint and surface a. [:] before [N] in most words

with a tense vowel because of ordinary constraint interac- along, belong, ding-dong, (di-, mono-,

tion, regardless of whether the input provides /A/ or /:/. tri-)phthong, dong, furlong, gong, long, mah-

jongg, Mekong, oblong, oolong, prolong, prong, sa-

rong, scuppernong, song, strong, thong, throng,

tongs, wrong
25 26

b. Variability between [A] and [:] before [N] in (22) Parochial constraint forces long to be [l:N]
/l:N/ long([+tense]) *3µ TNS<->µµ N/µ IDENT(tense)
foreign words
☞ l:µµN *
bong, dugong, Hong Kong, Ping-Pong, Vietcong l:µµNµ * !
c. [A] fairly consistently before [Ng] + vowel lµNµ * !
lAµNµ * ! *
bongo (also [b:Ngo]), conga, conger, Congo,

congress, congruence, humongous (also


Under this analysis, the other words listed in (20)a would
[hjumÃNg«s]), jongleur, monger (also [mÃNgr`]),
also have parochial constraints requiring that they have a
Mongol (also [mAn-]), mongoose (also [mAngus]),
tense vowel, and these parochial constraints would be
mongrel (also [mÃNgr«l]), Rancho Cucamonga (also
ranked above N/µ. Other words, such as those in (20)c,
[-mÃNg«]), Songhai (also [s:NgaI]), Tonga
those in (20)b for speakers who use the variant [A] rather

than [:], and all words with any vowel besides a low back
We begin our analysis with common native words like long,
vowel before [N], would not have any parochial constraint
pronounced [l:N] in EGA. Given the constraint hierarchy
requiring them to have a certain kind of vowel, but would
given above in (7), even the input /l:N/ should give the
be taken care of solely by the usual phonotactic and faith-
output *[lAN].
fulness constraints, as shown in (23) for dugong with the

pronunciation [dugAN]. (For simplicity's sake I exclude


(21) Constraint hierarchy falsely predicts long to be *[lAN]
candidates that violate *3µ and TNS<->µµ.)
/l:N/ *3µ TNS<->µµ N/µ IDENT(tense)
l:µµN * !
l:µµNµ * ! (α) /dugAN/
(23) N/µ IDENT(tense)
lµNµ * ! (β) /dug:N/
, lAµNµ * (α)
☞ dugAµNµ
(β) *
(α) *
One conceivable solution (which we will later reject) would dug:µµN * !
(β)
be to follow the same route we took for spa and mirage and

propose parochial constraints requiring words like long to The idea, therefore, would be that the words in (20)a, i.e.
have tense vowels. the native words, form a class of lexical exceptions to the
generalization that tense vowels are prohibited before [N],
27 28

and that these lexical exceptions are accounted for by stated as the set of conjoined output-output (OO) con-

high-ranking parochial constraints. Newer words, such as straints relating the [N] in these words to the preceding

those in (20)b, follow the phonotactically expected pattern [:]. Assuming just these seven words, there are 7 × 6 ÷ 2
and thus are not subject to this kind of parochial con- = 21 OO constraints requiring that both members of any pair

straint. have the vowel [:] (as exemplified in (24)), 15 OO con-

One problem with this analysis is it doesn't explain straints requiring that both members of any pair have the

why only [:] behaves this way; the prohibition against all consonant [N] (as exemplified in (25)), and 21² = 441 con-
other tense vowels before [N] is absolute. Why is [:] straint conjunctions requiring pairs to have both [:] and

different? Furthermore, this analysis flies in the face of [N] (as exemplified in (26)).13
the usual treatment of exceptional loanword phonology,
(24) IDENT-OO(long, song; :), IDENT-OO(long, strong; :),
according to which native words conform to phonotactically
etc.
expected patterns, while loanwords can violate markedness

constraints that native words are subject to (Itô & Mester (25) IDENT-OO(long, song; N), IDENT-OO(long, strong; N), etc.

1995, 1999, Davidson & Noyer 1996, Fukazawa et al. 1998,


(26) OO(long, song; :) & OO(long, song; N), OO(long,
Féry to appear).
strong; :) & OO(long, strong; N), etc.
The problem then is to find a way to capture the in-
tuition that the native words in (20)a are less marked than
The constraint conjunctions in (26), acting together, are
the foreign words in (20)b. To do this, I turn to the

principle of lexical relatedness webs as outlined in Green


13 An issue I do not have space to go into here is how this pat-
(in prep.), which makes use of the analogical constraints tern got started. Briefly, I suspect that only a historical explana-
tion is possible: At some point in the history of the dialect(s) in
proposed by Myers (1999).
question there was a sound change lengthening [] (the ancestor sound
The first point to make is that the forms in (20)a of EGA [A] in lax environments) to [:] before [N] (also before voice-
less fricatives, as in cloth [kl:T], cross [kr:s], soft [s:ft]).
include the most commonly occurring (and probably earliest
The phonetic or phonological rationale for such a sound change is
acquired) words of all that contain a low back vowel fol- unclear to me, and it may not have originally applied to all words
simultaneously. Instead, it may have begun in just a few forms and
lowed by [N]: along, belong, ding-dong, long, song,
then spread by lexical diffusion. I plan to examine lexical diffusion
strong, wrong. These words establish a correlation between in future research.

[:] and [N] that overrides N/µ; this correlation can be


29 30

strong enough to attract the rest of the words in (20)a within OT, any one of which could successfully be applied

and, for many speakers, some or all of the words in (20)b here, e.g. Anttila (1997), Nagy & Reynolds (1997), Boersma

into it.14 Following Myers (1999) we may refer to this (1998). Variation in the pronunciation of Vietcong
influence as synchronic analogy. A representative tableau [vi:«tk:N ~ vi:«tkAN] is illustrated in (28), where a wavy
for diphthong [dIfT:N] is given in (27). In practice, line indicates variable ranking between two constraints.
there would not be just a single OO conjunction, but at

least seven, one pairing diphthong with each of the most (28) Variability in Vietcong
OO(long, Vietcong; :) &
common [:N] words. In the upper left hand corner, /Å/ /vi«tkÅN/ Vietcong([-tense]) N/µ
OO(long, Vietcong; N)
stands for "either /A/ or /:/". ☞ vi:«tkAµNµ *
☞ vi:«tk:µµN * *

(27) diphthong [dIfT:N] influenced by analogy with long


etc. This analysis now lets us mark foreign words like Congo and

/dIfTÅN/
OO(long, diphthong; :) &
N/µ
Vietcong as special and unusual, while native words like
OO(long, diphthong; N)
long, song, strong, and wrong obey the basic constraint
dIfTAµNµ * !
☞ dIfT:µµN * ranking of the language. Note, however, that the basic

constraint ranking of the language is not simply a matter


Words like those listed in (20)b, which vary between [A] of conflicting markedness and faithfulness constraints.
and [:] before [N], have parochial constraints requiring Rather, analogical constraints play a role as well, estab-
them to have lax vowels, but these constraints are not lishing strong patterns that violate otherwise robust
consistently ranked above the analogical constraints. phonotactic tendencies. This approach allows us to treat
There are a number of different approaches to variation the difference between foreign words and native words in a

much more intuitively satisfying way.

Now we can return to the words in (20)c, showing [A]


14 The remaining words in (20)b and those in (20)c do not have
before [Ng] + vowel. I suggest that these words show that
such a strong connection with those in (20)a, either because of their
low frequency (see Bybee 1995 on the importance of frequency in estab- [Ng], unlike [N] alone, is an ordinary noncoronal cluster
lishing lexical connections), their status as recent loanwords, or the
and thus the relevant markedness constraint for these words
presence of [g] + vowel after [N].
is not N/µ but rather *TNSCLUS. Thus conger [kANgr`] has a
31 32

lax vowel for precisely the same reason cusp in (8) does: lexical exceptions in any way, and should be able to be

high-ranking *TNSCLUS. For speakers who pronounce bongo accounted for directly. To do this, we need an OO con-
[b:Ngo], the parochial constraint bongo([+tense]) outranks straint requiring that vowels in the positive and compara-
*TNSCLUS, just as traipse([+tense]) does in (12). tive forms of an adjective agree for the feature [tense]:
Otherwise the only place where [:Ng] + vowel is found IDENT-OO(Apos, Acmp; [tense]). This does not require that

is in the derived forms15 longer, longest, stronger, strong- the comparative be derived from the positive, merely that
est, diphthongal. The analysis as described so far falsely the comparative and positive can be identified as forms of
predicts [A] rather than [:] in these words, because [Ng] the same word, presumably through their semantic proper-
patterns as a noncoronal cluster that is irrelevant for the ties. Ranking this constraint above *TNSCLUS achieves the
constraint N/µ. As shown in the tableaux in (29)-(30), the desired result, as shown in the tableau in (31). As dis-

theory predicts the same vowel in longer as in congress. cussed above, the [:] of long is an effect of analogical

constraints among the various words ending in -ong.


(29) /lÅNgr`/ N/µ *TNSCLUS
, lANgr`

IDENT-OO (Apos,
Acmp; [tense])
l:Ngr` * !
pos:/lÅN/ OO(long, song; :) &
(31) N/µ *TNSCLUS
cmp:/lÅNgr`/ OO(long, song; N)
(30) /kÅNgr«s/ N/µ *TNSCLUS
☞ kANgr«s
k:Ngr«s * !
pos:[l:N]
* ! *
cmp:[lANgr`]
Appealing to a high-ranking parochial constraint requiring ☞ pos:[l:N]
* *
longer etc. to contain a tense vowel is unsatisfying, for cmp:[l:Ngr`]
pos:[lAN]
* !
the same reasons that the similar constraint for long given cmp:[lANgr`]

in (22) was unsatisfying: These forms do not seem to be

The relationship between the vowels in diphth[:]ng and

diphth[:]ngal can presumably be analyzed in a similar way,


15 I use the term "derived form" for expository convenience;
within the word-based morphology I assume there is of course no actual although this is obviously not a positive-comparative ad-
process of derivation.
jective pair.
33 34

As mentioned briefly above, [:] occurs regularly not tributional restrictions on tense vowels are suspended for

only before [N] but also before the noncoronal cluster [ft] [:], such that [:] occurs nearly to the exclusion of [A]

in native words and names of English origin: aloft, Ash- before [N] (but not usually before [Nk] and [Ng]) and be-
croft, Bancroft, loft, oft, often (when pronounced with fore [ft], but there are foreign words like dugong and

[t]), soft. Here again, analogical constraints connecting zaftig that are exceptions to this exceptional behavior.

tense [:] with the cluster [ft] outrank *TNSCLUS, as shown


4. The low front tense vowel [E«]
in the tableau in (32). (As above, one analogical con-
straint is shown in the tableau, but this must be under- In many varieties of EGA there is a tense partner to lax

stood as standing for a whole host of them, one for each [Q]; its exact phonetic realization varies from region to

pair of words with [:ft].) region, but in general it is either a vowel slightly higher

and somewhat longer than [Q] (in IPA, [Q3>]) or else a diph-
(32) /lÅft/ OO(loft, soft; :) & OO(loft, soft; ft) *TNSCLUS
thong beginning with a front vowel and ending with [«], so
lAft * !
☞ l:ft * somewhere along the spectrum [Q« - E« - e« - I«]. For some

speakers this vowel may also be spontaneously nasalized

There is even a lexical exception to the pattern of having (i.e. even when not preceding a nasal consonant). I will

[:] rather than [A] before [ft]: the Yiddish loanword choose [E«] to indicate any variety of this "tense Q"; in

zaftig, which is usually pronounced [zAftIk]. For this the previous literature the most common symbol is [E].

word, there is presumably a high-ranking parochial con- Unlike the other tense vowels of English, [E«] does not

straint requiring a lax vowel that outranks the analogical occur in stressed open final syllables. This is because it

constraints establishing the [:ft] pattern, as illustrated is derived from lax [Q], which could not stand there;

in (33). therefore there are no words in which [E«] has the opportu-

nity to stand in a stressed open final syllable.16 Discus-


(33) /zÅftIk/
OO(zaftig, soft; :) &
zaftig([-tense]) *TNSCLUS
OO(zaftig, soft; ft)
16 The only exception I know of is yeah, pronounced [jE«]. In
☞ zAftIk *
nonrhotic accents, [E«] is also found in words like pair [pE«], tear
z:ftIk * ! *
(verb) [tE«], care [kE«]. For some speakers, then, scarce [skE«s]
rhymes with pass [pE«s].

So there are a number of instances where the usual dis-


35 36

sions of this vowel and its patterning can be found in g. shaft SE«ft

Trager (1930, 1934, 1940, 1941), Labov (1966, 1972, 1981), h. task tE«sk

Ferguson (1972), Kahn (1976), Wells (1982: 477-9 and 510- i. grasp grE«sp
2), Benua (1995), and Morén (1997) (who analyzes the vowel

in question as lax).17 There are some words that unexpectedly have [Q] in these
In most dialects that have [E«], it occurs in stressed environments, resulting in minimal pairs between [E«] and
final syllables before nasals (except [N]) and voiceless [Q], such as can 'tin container' [kE«n] vs. can 'be able'
fricatives (not all dialects allow it before [S]); some [kQn], or halve [hE«v] vs. have [hQv] and (for some people)
varieties allow it before voiced obstruents as well.18 bad [bE«d] vs. bade [bQd] in the varieties that allow [E«]
Interestingly, noncoronal clusters beginning with one of before voiced obstruents. [E«] does not occur in nonfinal
the permitted segments are not excluded. Some examples of syllables for all speakers, e.g. manage [mQn«dZ], tassel
words with [E«] are shown in (34). [tQsl`], with the proviso that while Class I suffixes cause

[E«]~[Q] alternations (class [klE«s] ~ classic [klQsIk]),


(34) Words with [E«] Class II suffixes do not (classy [klE«si]).19 Also, mono-
a. ram rE«m syllables that are truncations of longer words maintain the
b. ran rE«n vowel of the original, resulting in pairs like caf [kQf]
c. laugh lE«f (truncation of 'cafeteria') vs. calf [kE«f], path [pQT]
d. path pE«T (truncation of 'pathology') vs. path [pE«T] (as in 'foot-
e. pass pE«s path'), or Mass [mQs] (truncation of 'Massachusetts') vs.

f. camp kE«mp mass [mE«s] (Benua 1995). In some varieties, [E«] can also

occur (even in nonfinal syllables) before [r], as in Mary


17 Many thanks to my informants: Nate Brown (Schenectady, NY), [mE«ri], which is then distinct from both merry [mEri] and
Ellen DeSoto (Poughkeepsie, NY), Jeff Kaplan (Philadelphia), Cindy
marry [mQri]; in other varieties, Mary and merry (and some-
Schneider (Watchung, New Jersey), and Alan Stevens (New York City).
18 And within voiced obstruents, there is also variation. For ex-
ample, some people have [E«] before voiced fricatives and [d] but [Q] 19 Again, I am using the labels "Class I Suffix" and "Class II
before [b, g]. Other people have [E«] before voiced fricatives and [d, Suffix" for descriptive convenience; such constructs play no role in a
b], but [Q] before [g]. word-based morphology.
37 38

times marry as well) are homophonous as [mEri].20 The prohibition of [E«] in the environments in (35) can be
Like most tense vowels, [E«] is prohibited before [N] analyzed in the same way as the prohibition of other tense
and before most noncoronal clusters (except [mp ft sk sp]), vowels in these environments was analyzed in §2.2. Tab-
as shown in (35). leaux for fang and lapse are shown in (36)-(37). The sym-

bol Q stands for "either /E«/ or /Q/."


(35) Only [Q], not [E«]

a. lapse lQps *lE«ps (36) /fQN/ *3µ TNS<->µµ N/µ


☞ fQµNµ
b. rapt rQpt *rE«pt fE«µNµ * !
c. ax Qks *E«ks fE«µµN * !
fE«µµNµ * !
d. act Qkt *E«kt

e. scalp skQlp *skE«lp


(37) /lQps/ *3µ TNS<->µµ *TNSCLUS
f. Ralph rQlf *rE«lf ☞ lQµpsµ
lE«µpsµ * ! *
g. valve vQlv *vE«lv
lE«µµpsµ * ! *
h. talc tQlk *tE«lk lE«µµps * !

i. fang fQN *fE«N


j. sank sQNk *sE«Nk As for (34)f-i, the analysis is basically the same as it
was for words like long and soft: high-ranking analogical

constraints force the members of these classes to rhyme


20 For some people, the distribution of [Q] and [E«] is apparently
with each other. For example, each pair of words in the
in lexical diffusion (cf. Labov 1994). One of my informants has, for
example, [Q] in graph, half, and staff but [E«] in laugh and riff-raff; set {ask, bask, cask, flask, mask, task} establishes a
before a noncoronal cluster she has [Q] in Basque, cask, casket, flask,
correlation between the cluster [sk] and the preceding
paschal, rascal but [E«] in ask, bask, basket, mask, task. As often
seems to be the case with lexical diffusion, there is great variation: vowel [E«]. In the tableau in (38), just one of these
One informant has [Q] in clasp, grasp, hasp, rasp and [E«] in asp,
analogical constraints is illustrated, but it stands for
gasp; another informant has [Q] in asp, gasp, hasp and [E«] in clasp,
grasp, rasp; a third has [Q] in asp, clasp, grasp, hasp and rasp and all of them.
[E«] in gasp; a fourth has [Q] in asp and hasp and [E«] in clasp, gasp,
grasp, rasp. So all four have [Q] in hasp, but otherwise there is no
agreement. A fifth informant has [E«] in all these words.
39 40

(38) /tQsk/ OO(task, ask; E«) & OO(task, ask; sk) *TNSCLUS
final syllables that are either open or closed by [D] or
☞ tE«sk *
tQsk * ! [Z], there is the lexical exception [ED] 'name of the let-

ter D' and a fair number of exceptions involving [A] in

There are lexical exceptions to this pattern as well. For foreign words: spa, bra, mirage, etc. Furthermore, while

example, one of my informants reports that he generally has tense vowels (and diphthongs) usually cannot stand before

[E«] before [sk] in stressed penults: basket, casket, [N] or noncoronal clusters, there are a number of lexical

rascal all have [E«]. But paschal, which is a rather rare exceptions such as traipse and coax. The tense vowel [:]

word, is exceptional in having [Q]. Once again, a paro- is remarkable in that it usually stands before [N] and

chial constraint, this time requiring paschal to have a lax [ft], and (in the varieties of EGA that have this sound)

vowel, can take care of this, as shown in (39). the diphthong [E«] is remarkable in that it usually stands

before [sk, sp, ft, mp]. But each of these unexpected


OO(paschal, rascal; E«) & generalizations has lexical exceptions too, mostly involv-
(39) /pQskl/ paschal([-tense]) *TNSCLUS
OO(paschal, rascal; sk)
ing rare or foreign words: dugong with [A] rather than
pE«skl` * ! *
☞ pQskl` * [:] before [N], zaftig with [A] rather than [:] before
[ft], and paschal with [Q] rather than [E«] before [sk].
So, just as we saw with [:] in §3, there are circumstances I have argued that lexical exceptions are best ana-
under which the tense vowel [E«] occurs in environments lyzed as resulting from parochial constraints requiring
where normally only lax vowels are allowed. The facts can specific lexical items to contain specific phonological
be analyzed in a theory that assumes analogical constraints information (such as the feature [-tense]) which can out-
relating rhyming words, which outrank phonotactic con- rank general phonotactic well-formedness constraints; the
straints like *TNSCLUS. majority of lexical items will not have parochial con-

straints and will thus be subject to phonotactic marked-


5. Conclusions
ness. In cases like [:N], [:ft], [E«sk], etc., the fact
In this paper, I have discussed data from Eastern General that more words violate markedness than obey it, and the
American English that show regular exceptions to the dis- fact that the words that do obey it tend to be rare or
tribution of lax and tense vowels. Namely, while it is foreign words, make it unlikely that this is a simple case
usually the case that lax vowels cannot stand in stressed of parochial constraints outranking markedness. Rather,
41 42

banczyk (eds.), University of Massachusetts Occasional


the members of the set of words containing sequences like
Papers 18: Papers in Optimality Theory. Amherst:
[:N], [:ft], [E«sk], etc., reinforce each other by means GLSA, University of Massachusetts. 77-136.
of analogical constraints. These analogical constraints Boersma, P. (1998). Functional Phonology: Formalizing the
Interactions between Articulatory and Perceptual
then outrank markedness, and can be themselves outranked by
Drives. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Amsterdam.
parochial constraints governing rare and foreign words like Published by Holland Academic Graphics, The Hague.
dugong, zaftig, paschal. Boersma, P. (2000). Learning a grammar in Functional Pho-
nology. In Dekkers et al., 465-523.
These conclusions contribute to phonological theory by
Borowsky, T. J. (1986). Topics in the Lexical Phonology of
showing that constraint interaction is not always a matter
English. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Massachu-
of conflict between faithfulness constraints and markedness setts.
constraints; analogical constraints reinforcing exceptional Borowsky, T. J. (1989). Structure preservation and the
syllable coda in English. Natural Language and Lin-
patterns as well as parochial constraints governing spe-
guistic Theory 7, 145-166.
cific lexical items have roles to play as well.
Bybee, J. L. (1985). Morphology: A Study of the Relation
between Meaning and Form. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Acknowledgments
Bybee, J. L. (1988). Morphology as lexical organization.
In M. Hammond & M. Noonan (eds.), Theoretical Morphol-
Thanks to Tracy Hall and Ruben van de Vijver for helpful
ogy: Approaches in Modern Linguistics. San Diego:
comments and criticism. More comments are always welcome! Academic Press. 119-141.
Errors are of course my responsibility. Bybee, J. L. (1995). Regular morphology and the lexicon.
Language and Cognitive Processes 10, 425-455.
References Calabrese, A. (1988). Towards a Theory of Phonological
Alphabets. Ph.D. dissertation, MIT.
Alcántara, J. (1998). The Architecture of the English
Chomsky, N. & M. Halle (1968). The Sound Pattern of Eng-
Lexicon. Ph.D. dissertation, Cornell University.
lish. New York: Harper & Row. Reprinted 1991, Cam-
Anttila, A. (1997). Variation in Finnish Phonology and bridge, Mass.: The MIT Press.
Morphology. Ph.D. dissertation, Stanford University.
Clements, G. N. & S. J. Keyser (1983). CV Phonology: A
Artstein, R. (1998). The incompatibility of underspecifi- Generative Theory of the Syllable. Cambridge, Mass.:
cation and markedness in Optimality Theory. RuLing The MIT Press.
Papers: Working Papers from Rutgers University 1, 7-
Davidson, L. & R. Noyer (1996). Loan phonology in Huave:
13.
nativization and the ranking of faithfulness con-
Benua, L. (1995). Identity effects in morphological trun- straints. In B. Agbayani & S.-W. Tang (eds.), WCCFL
cation. In J. N. Beckman, L. Walsh Dickey & S. Ur-
43 44

15. Stanford: Stanford Linguistics Association. 65- Halle, M. & K. P. Mohanan (1985). Segmental phonology of
79. Modern English. Linguistic Inquiry 16, 57-116.

Dekkers, J., F. van der Leeuw & J. van de Weijer, eds. Hammond, M. (1999). The Phonology of English: A Prosodic
(2000). Optimality Theory: Phonology, Syntax, and Optimality-Theoretic Approach. Oxford: Oxford Uni-
Acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press. versity Press.
Ellison, T. M. (2000). The universal constraint set: Inkelas, S. (1995). The consequences of optimization for
convention, not fact. In Dekkers et al., 524-553. underspecification. In J. N. Beckman (ed.), Proceed-
ings of NELS 25, vol. 1. Amherst: GLSA, University
Ferguson, C. A. (1972). "Short a" in Philadelphia English.
of Massachusetts. 287-302.
In M. E. Smith (ed.), Studies in Linguistics in Honor
of George L. Trager. The Hague: Mouton. Inkelas, S. (1996). Archiphonemic underspecification: an
optimization approach to the phonological description
Féry, C. (1997). The mora as a measure of weight and a
of morphemes. Ms., UC Berkeley.
syllabic constituent. In P. M. Bertinetto, L. Gaeta,
G. Jetchev & D. Michaels (eds.), Certamen Phonologicum Inkelas, S. (1999). Exceptional stress-attracting suffixes
III: Papers from the Third Cortona Phonology Meeting, in Turkish: representations versus the grammar. In
April 1996. Turin: Rosenberg & Sellier. 91-110. R. Kager, H. van der Hulst & W. Zonneveld (eds.), The
Prosody-Morphology Interface. Cambridge: Cambridge
Féry, C. (to appear). Final devoicing and the stratifica-
University Press. 134-187.
tion of the lexicon in German. In V. van Heuven, H.
van der Hulst & J. van de Weijer (eds.), Proceedings Inkelas, S., O. Orgun & C. Zoll (1997). The implications
of HILP 4. of lexical exceptions for the nature of grammar. In
I. Roca (ed.), Derivations and Constraints in Phonol-
Fukazawa, H., M. Kitahara & M. Ota (1998). Lexical strati-
ogy. Oxford: Clarendon Press. 393-418.
fication and ranking invariance in constraint-based
grammars. In: M. C. Gruber, D. Higgins, K. S. Olson Itô, J. & A. Mester (1995). The core-periphery structure
& T. Wysocki (eds.), CLS 34, Part 2: Papers from the of the lexicon and constraints on reranking. In J. N.
Panels. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society. 47-62. Beckman, L. Walsh Dickey & S. Urbanczyk (eds.), UMOP
18: Papers in Optimality Theory. Amherst: GLSA,
Golston, C. (1996). Direct Optimality Theory: representa-
UMass.
tion as pure markedness. Language 72, 713-748.
Itô, J. & A. Mester (1999). The phonological lexicon. In
Green, A. D. (2001). American English "r-colored" vowels
N. Tsujimura (ed.), The Handbook of Japanese Linguis-
as complex nuclei. Ms., University of Potsdam.
tics. Oxford: Blackwell. 62-100.
Green, A. D. (in prep.). Relatedness Webs in Phonology and
Itô, J., A. Mester & J. Padgett (1995). Licensing and
Morphology. Ms., University of Potsdam.
underspecification in Optimality Theory. Linguistic
Hall, T. A. (2001). Review of Hammond (1999). Lingua 111, Inquiry 26, 571-613.
235-242.
Kahn, D. (1976). Syllable-based Generalizations in English
Halle, M. (1977). Tenseness, vowel shift, and the phonol- Phonology. Ph.D. dissertation, UCLA. Reproduced by
ogy of the back vowels in Modern English. Linguistic the Indiana University Linguistics Club.
Inquiry 8, 611-625.
45 46

Kiparsky, P. (1993). Blocking in nonderived environments. Prince, A. & P. Smolensky (1993). Optimality Theory:
In S. Hargus & E. M. Kaisse (eds.), Phonetics and Pho- constraint interaction in generative grammar. Rutgers
nology 4: Studies in Lexical Phonology. San Diego: University Center for Cognitive Science Technical Re-
Academic Press. 277-313. port #2.
Labov, W. (1966). The Social Stratification of English in Selkirk, E. O. (1982). The syllable. In H. van der Hulst
New York City. Washington: Center for Applied Lin- & N. Smith (eds.), The Structure of Phonological Rep-
guistics. resentations. Part II. Cinnaminson, NJ: Foris.
337-384.
Labov, W. (1972). Sociolinguistic Patterns. Philadelphia:
University of Pennsylvania Press. Smolensky, P. (1993). Harmony, markedness, and
phonological activity. Handout from Rutgers Optimal-
Labov, W. (1981). Resolving the Neogrammarian controversy.
ity Workshop 1. ROA-87-0000.
Language 57, 267-308.
Stanley, R. (1967). Redundancy rules in phonology. Lan-
Labov, W. (1994). Principles of Linguistic Change: Inter-
guage 43, 393-436.
nal Factors. Oxford: Blackwell.
Trager, G. L. (1930). The pronunciation of "short a" in
Lamontagne, G. (1993). Syllabification and Consonant Cooc-
American Standard English. American Speech 5, 396-
currence Conditions. Ph.D. dissertation, University
400.
of Massachusetts.
Trager, G. L. (1934). What conditions limit variants of a
Lass, R. (1976). English Phonology and Phonological The-
phoneme? American Speech 9, 313-315.
ory: Synchronic and Diachronic Studies. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press. Trager, G. L. (1940). One phonemic entity becomes two:
the case of "short a". American Speech 15, 255-258.
McCarthy, J. J. & A. S. Prince (1993). Prosodic Morphology
I: constraint interaction and satisfaction. Rutgers Trager, G. L. (1941). « Èn«wt on Q «nd Q﬩ > in «Èmerik«n
University Center for Cognitive Science Technical Re- ÈiNgliS. Maître Phonétique 17-19.
port #3.
Tranel, B. (1996). Exceptionality in Optimality Theory and
Morén, B. T. (1997). Markedness and Faithfulness Con- final consonants in French. In K. Zagona (ed.), Gram-
straints on the Association of Moras. M. A. thesis, matical Theory and Romance Languages. Amsterdam:
University of Maryland. Benjamins.

Muthmann, G. (1999). Reverse English Dictionary. Berlin: Wells, J. C. (1982). Accents of English. 3 vols. Cam-
Mouton de Gruyter. bridge: Cambridge University Press.

Myers, J. (1999). Lexical phonology and the lexicon. Ms., Wells, J. C. (1990). Longman Pronunciation Dictionary.
National Chung Cheng University. ROA-330-0699. Essex: Longman.

Nagy, N. & B. Reynolds (1997). Optimality Theory and vari- Wood, C., ed. (1936). The Complete Rhyming Dictionary and
able word-final deletion in Faetar. Language Varia- Poet's Craft Book. Garden City: Halcyon House.
tion and Change 9, 37-55.

Вам также может понравиться