Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 10

ORIGINAL RESEARCH

published: 20 July 2017


doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01239

Identifying Gifted Children:


Congruence among Different IQ
Measures
Estrella Fernández 1*, Trinidad García 1 , Olga Arias-Gundín 2 , Almudena Vázquez 3 and
Celestino Rodríguez 1
1
Faculty of Psychology, Oviedo University, Oviedo, Spain, 2 Department of Psychology, Sociology and Philosophy, Faculty of
Education, León University, León, Spain, 3 Asunción León-Primary and Secondary School, León, Spain

This study has two main aims: (1) analysing the relationship between intellectual
capacities and levels of creativity in a sample of Spanish students from the third and
sixth grades; and (2) examining the discrimination capacities and degree of congruence
among different tests of intellectual ability that are commonly used to identify high-
ability students. The study sample comprised 236 primary school students. Participants
completed different tests of intellectual ability, which were based on both fluid and
crystallized intelligence, as well as creativity. Results indicated that it is advisable to use
varying tests in the assessment process, and a complementary measure (i.e., creativity)
Edited by: in order to create a multi-criteria means of detection that can more efficiently distinguish
José Jesús Gázquez,
University of Almería, Spain this population of students.
Reviewed by: Keywords: intellectual ability, creativity, primary school, high ability, assessment methods
Juan Luis Castejon,
University of Alicante, Spain
Faye Antoniou,
National and Kapodistrian University
INTRODUCTION
of Athens, Greece
Identifying students with higher abilities has become a subject of great interest for researchers,
*Correspondence: education administrators, teachers and families alike. However, it is also a controversial issue
Estrella Fernández
because there is still no agreement on which variables must be taken into account to determine
fernandezestrella@uniovi.es
whether a student has higher abilities, or how these variables should be measured in these cases.
Specialty section:
The different conceptualizations of higher intellectual abilities, either from educational, socio-
This article was submitted to political or psychometric perspectives, have traditionally tried to identify those children who are
Educational Psychology, exceptional (Pfeiffer, 2015). One of the models that has received more attention is the Three-
a section of the journal Ring Conception of Giftedness by Renzulli (1978). This model has helped establish some of the
Frontiers in Psychology general criteria being used to classify students with higher abilities today. This author defined high
Received: 31 January 2017 intellectual ability as a consistent interaction between three basic human traits that characterize
Accepted: 06 July 2017 high-ability people: (a) above-average general intelligence; (b) creativity (defined as “that cluster of
Published: 20 July 2017 traits that encompasses curiosity, originality, ingenuity, and a willingness to challenge convention
Citation: and tradition”; and (c) task commitment (which “represents a non-intellective cluster of traits
Fernández E, García T, found consistently in creative and productive individuals, including perseverance, determination,
Arias-Gundín O, Vázquez A and will power or positive energy”) (Renzulli, 2012). This model has been used as a reference in Spanish
Rodríguez C (2017) Identifying Gifted
schools to determine which students are gifted and which students are not gifted. In which the
Children: Congruence among
Different IQ Measures.
creativity acquiring, at a practical level, great protagonism, above-average commitment. Moreover,
Front. Psychol. 8:1239. some studies show that gifted learners are more creative than average learners, for example, when
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01239 evaluating divergent thinking or amount of original ideas (Ferrando et al., 2008; Jauk et al., 2013).

Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 1 July 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1239


Fernández et al. Identifying Gifted Children

However, this is not the only model to be considered. Other remains that standardized tests have been accepted as reliable
authors such as Jeltova and Grigorenko (2005), Calero et al. measures of identifying students with higher abilities to date
(2007), and Pfeiffer (2012) consider high-ability children as those (Lovett and Lewandowski, 2006; Lovett and Sparks, 2011; Erwin
who demonstrate a higher likelihood of attaining significant and Worrell, 2012) and as Carman (2013) suggests “no matter
achievements in culturally valued domains. These authors how often researchers suggest that an IQ score is not the only way
take into account a student’s intellectual abilities, while also of determining giftedness, it is still the most common method
emphasizing the relevance of certain personality traits and the of identifying gifted participants for research, either alone or in
role of stimulating social environments that can effectively favor combination with other criteria.” At a practical level, in Spain the
an individual’s learning in specific fields. However, regardless information obtained from standardized tests is the first criterion
of the theoretical model, there is agreement today that higher used to determine if a student may have higher abilities, and is
intellectual ability is a multi-dimensional construct, and that essential for continuation of the evaluation process. This measure
more human and material resources are needed to identify this is used as a baseline analysis of the students’ capacities and offers
often-latent potential in order to provide appropriate educational a starting point for the detection of higher intellectual abilities
support to such students (Tourón et al., 1998; Pfeiffer, 2015). (Renzulli, 2012; Wellisch and Brown, 2012).
It is therefore fundamental that schools and professionals are Accepting this condition as necessary, a new problem arises
provided with the right tools to identify high-ability students as concerning which standardized tests to choose and the degree of
early as possible (Reis and Renzulli, 2010). congruence required between different measures. This difficulty
Traditionally, intellectual ability was the central variable is associated, in part, with the definition of intelligence itself
used to discriminate high-ability individuals from the average and with the variables that are considered relevant to measure
population. Nowadays, however, various authors agree that this construct (e.g., abstract reasoning, vocabulary, numerical
intellectual quotient (IQ) cannot be used as a single variable in knowledge). Standardized tests designed to evaluate the IQ are
the conceptualization of high abilities (Calero and García-Martín, based on different conceptualizations of intelligence and this is
2011; Pfeiffer, 2015). For example, as discussed by Wellisch and an important aspect to consider when deciding which measure
Brown (2012) in their study, some authors suggest that the most should be used. Some authors recommend the use of non-
reliable information would be based on the perception of teachers verbal tests to avoid cultural and linguistic biases (Naglieri and
and families. Nevertheless, IQ remains an important factor to be Ford, 2003) such as the Factor “g” test (Cattell and Cattell,
assessed and, when used in conjunction with other variables, it 1994) or “Matrices” (Sánchez-Sánchez et al., 2015), both of
can provide essential information concerning the identification which are considered good estimators of fluid intelligence
of students with exceptional abilities (Sternberg, 2010; Renzulli and general intellectual ability (or “g” factor). Other authors,
and Gaesser, 2015). Moreover, many educational policies in order to provide a more contextual perspective to the
establish that, in order to implement effective identification and conceptualization of the intelligence, give greater weight to the
intervention processes, a non-negotiable criterion is to evaluate evaluation of psychological variables relevant to the execution of
the student’s intellectual capacity by means of standardized tests school tasks, thus estimating intellectual ability by focusing on
(Wet and Gubbins, 2011). Although other criteria may be used, school competences rather than on purely intellectual capacities
there are currently authors who consider that these criteria (Thurstone and Thurstone, 2005). Finally, some authors state
cannot equal the objectivity and reliability of IQ measurements that appropriate testing should take the form of batteries of tests
and tasks, especially for students with learning difficulties (Lovett that also collect information on a wide range of variables that,
and Lewandowski, 2006). This broader approach to assessment in the last decades, have demonstrated they are good indicators
is important, since the responsibility of detecting high-ability of intelligence, such as students’ verbal competence, together
students often falls to schools, which commonly only pay with components such as working memory, processing speed,
attention to the more traditional signals related to high-ability, comprehension, analytical capacity, and so forth (Sternberg,
such as high levels of academic achievement. Evaluation and 2010; Pierson et al., 2012).
intervention recommendations come from teachers in most cases At this point it is worth noting the current interest in the
(Renzulli and Gaesser, 2015); however, most teachers do not have research community in hierarchical models of intelligence and
a vast knowledge in the identification of high-ability students. their tests, and specifically in the Cattell–Horn–Carroll Theory
This may lead to mistakes during the assessment process (Tourón of Cognitive Abilities (CHC) (McGrew, 2005). This theory
et al., 2006; Reis and Renzulli, 2010) and under-identification establishes three strata in the conceptualization of intelligence:
of some students, especially those from lower socio-economic stratum III – general or global intelligence; stratum II (broad) –
backgrounds (Moon and Brighton, 2008; Baker, 2011; Freeman, 10 general intelligence abilities which are the main focus of
2011; Wellisch and Brown, 2012), and/or those who have socio- interest in the assessment of intellectual ability and are fluid
emotional problems and may appear to have low levels of and crystallized intelligence, short-term or immediate memory,
competence in basic learning processes (emulating students with long-term memory storage and retrieval, processing speed,
learning difficulties) (Silverman, 2009; Wellisch and Brown, quantitative reasoning, reacting or decision making speed, visual
2012). processing, auditory processing, reading ability, and writing
Therefore, although the exclusive use of standardized tests to ability; and stratum I (narrow) – made up of more specific
assess intellectual ability has its detractors (Pfeiffer, 2012) and components such as inductive processes, vocabulary, visual
these tests are not the only measures available nowadays, the fact memory, spatial relations, and general sequential reasoning,

Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 2 July 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1239


Fernández et al. Identifying Gifted Children

and which would conform to the general cognitive factors of SD = 0.55), with 47 (39.5%) being female and 72 (60.5%) being
stratum II. male. There were statistically significant differences between
Although this theory is gradually having an impact on the the proportion of boys and girls in this group (χ2 = 5.25;
evaluation and identification of higher ability students at the p = 0.022).
international level (Pfeiffer, 2015), and new assessment tools are
being designed or adapted based on this model (e.g., WISC-V; Measures
Wechsler, 2014), at a practical level, at least in Spain, it has not The following instruments were administered:
yet become established as a specific assessment protocol adjusted
to this perspective. Therefore, both the detection model and the Intellectual Abilities
tests used ultimately depend on the experience and knowledge of Three measures traditionally used in the assessment of
the professionals in charge of the evaluation, and the assessment intelligence were used. The Test of Educational Aptitudes
measures available in each case. (TEA-1) is a test of academic competences based on a selection
The present study had two objectives. First, following of the most relevant factors from the “Primary Mental Abilities”
Renzulli’s (1978) model, it aimed to describe intellectual by Thurstone (1938). The Battery of Differential and General
capacities and creativity levels of a sample of primary school Skills (Badyg) is consistent with the Cattell–Horn–Carroll
students from northern Spain, with the aim of detecting and theory (CHC) as the test is based on a hierarchical model of
analysing potential cases of high ability where IQ is 130 or intelligence with three different levels. Lastly, the Factor “g” test
above – or two typical deviations above the average. Students is a non-verbal test which provides a measure of fluid intelligence
from grades 3 and 6 were chosen as representative of this stage, (Gf) and general intellectual ability, or g factor. Due to the age
and two variables of measures, intellectual capacity and creativity, of the students, two different versions of the Badyg were used.
were measured. Second, taking into account that depending on Specifically, students in grade 3 completed the Badyg-2, while
the tests used the students identified as gifted children may students in grade 6 completed the Badyg-3. A more detailed
be different, this study aimed to establish the congruence and description of these tests follows.
efficacies of different types of intellectual ability measures in Test of Educational Aptitudes (adapted to Spanish by
order to determine if they concur, with respect to distinguishing Department I+D of TEA Editions, S.A.) (Thurstone and
students with higher abilities from average students. In schools Thurstone, 2005) test provides an estimation of general
it is common to use only a test of intellectual capacity in the intelligence and its factors. It consists of five parts that measure
processes of identification. Therefore, it is necessary to determine three different components or abilities (i.e., factors): verbal
if these results in incorrect identification, either by over- or (different words and vocabulary), numerical (calculation), and
under-identification, due to inconsistencies between different reasoning (drawing and series). It also offers the possibility to
type tests results. measure verbal and non-verbal abilities separately. It is available
In this analysis, although they are important variables in three different versions for different age groups. The TEA-
in Renzulli’s (1978) model, task involvement and academic 1 version was used in the present study and was administered
performance are not included as discriminating criteria because according to the age range of the sample. Reliability coefficients
previous literature suggests that many students with high ability by mean of Cronbach’s alpha ranged between 0.61 and 0.95 for
fail in the academic environment due to related factors, such the different subtests, with an alpha of 0.89 for the full scale. The
as lack of motivation, and poor recognition by teachers of their manual reports adequate internal validity, although correlations
real educational needs, both of which can also arise due to between different variables are mostly low to moderate. High
“teacher-bias” (Reis and Renzulli, 2004, 2009). correlations are only reported between verbal reasoning and
academic aptitude (r = 0.89), and between academic aptitude and
numerical reasoning (r = 0.85).
MATERIALS AND METHODS Battery of Differential and General Skills (Badyg) (Yuste
et al., 2005) provides an estimation of IQ and presents
Participants different versions for different age groups. Students in sixth
A sample of 236 primary school students from northern Spain grade completed the Badyg-E3, which consists of six subtests:
took part in this study. The students were recruited from the (1) analog relations (verbal intelligence), (2) numerical series
third grade (n = 117; 49.6%) and the sixth grade (n = 119; (inductive reasoning), (3) matrices (fluid intelligence), (4)
50.4%). Their ages ranged from 8 to 13 years (M = 9.96; sentence completion (inductive reasoning), (5) numerical
SD = 1.65). The ratio of males to females in the total sample problems (verbal intelligence), and (6) figure matching (visual
was not ideal (χ2 = 4.90; p =0.027). There were no statistically processing). An overall full-scale IQ index score is also provided.
significant differences in the percentage of students in the Students in third grade completed the Badyg-E2. It is made up
different grades (p = 0.90). The ages of the third grade students of the same subtests as the Badyg-E3 but varies in difficulty level
ranged from 9 to 10 years (M = 8.38; SD = 0.51), with 63 and application time. Cronbach’s alpha was from 0.77 to 0.84 for
(53.8%) of the sample being female, and 54 (46.2%) being the different subtests, and 0.95 for the full scale. The Cronbach’s
male. There were no statistically significant differences regarding alpha obtained in the present study, for the full scale, was 0.72.
gender distribution (p = 0.405). In the case of the sixth grade While there are more powerful assessment tools to evaluate
students, their ages ranged from 11 to 13 years (M = 11.50; this component and with better psychometric properties, this

Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 3 July 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1239


Fernández et al. Identifying Gifted Children

instrument was chosen for the following reasons: (a) it can be Data Analysis
used to predict academic performance in a reliable way; (b) it has A descriptive design was used. Due to the objectives of this
been used in previous studies which demonstrated a relationship study, statistical analyses were performed in different steps. First,
between intellectual ability and academic performance; and (c) the sample was described in terms of age, gender, IQ (based
factorial analysis showed high correlations between the different on the three measures of intelligence previously described), and
sub-scales that compose the Badyg battery. Criterion validity was creativity. This analysis was conducted separately for students
moderate to high (Pearson’s r from 0.39 to 0.58). This scale also in grade 3 and 6, as different versions of the Badyg were
shows a well-adjusted factorial structure making it possible to used. The normality of the dependent variables (i.e., global
carry out additional broad-scoped comparisons (e.g., Sabiston scores in the CREA, Badyg, TEA-1, and Factor “g” test) was
et al., 2013). analyzed, paying special attention to skewness and kurtosis
The Factor “g” test (Cattell and Cattell, 1994 – adapted values. Following Finney and Di Stefano’s (2006) criterion, the
to Spanish by Associated Specialized Technicians) evaluates adequacy of these values was demonstrated (Table 1). Secondly,
intelligence conceived as a general mental ability. It uses non- to estimate the correspondence between the different measures of
verbal tasks to eliminate the influence of those abilities that intellectual ability, Pearson correlation between global IQ scores
have been acquired through education, such as vocabulary or were conducted.
numerical knowledge. This test has three versions, each with Additionally, student’s t-test was also performed to analyze
different difficulty levels. The selection of the level depends upon within-subject differences in IQ estimated with the different
the age of the participant. Level 2 (suitable for children from 8 to tests. To analyze the discriminatory capacity of each test in the
14 years) was used in the present study. It includes four subtests: detection of students with high abilities, the absolute frequency
series, classification, conditions, and matrices. Individual scores of students with an IQ of 130 or higher (as determined by the
are combined to obtain a global IQ score. The participant is different tests) was then calculated. The congruence among the
asked to establish logical relationships between abstract figures three intelligence tests was estimated by recording the number
and forms. of students who were found to have an IQ of 130 or above in all
Cronbach’s alpha ranged between 0.76 and 0.85 for the the tests. Congruence between pairs of tests in the detection of
different subtests (alpha = 0.86 for the full scale), with high-ability students was also established. Although considering
a complementary index adequate stability of 2.59 (typical an IQ of 130 or above – or two typical deviations above the
measurement errors). Criterion validity was high, finding average – seems to be an arbitrary criterion, in both research and
statistically significant correlations between the different sub- educational practice this criterion is still used, in most cases, as a
scales and the Test of Educational Aptitudes-TEA 1 and 2 cut-off point to determine which students have higher intellectual
(Pearson’s r from 0.53 to 0.81; p < 0.001). abilities (Moon and Brighton, 2008; Carman, 2013; Guignard
et al., 2016; Peyre et al., 2016).
Creativity
The Creative Intelligence Test (CREA) (Corbalán et al., 2003)
presents participants with an image (commonly representing a RESULTS
social scene) and they have a limited time frame to formulate all
the questions that the situation evokes in them. Version C, which Intellectual IQ Results of the Students
is aimed at children, was used. In addition to providing a global and Correspondence between Measures
measure of creativity, it offers the possibility to analyze the results Table 1 shows descriptive statistics for the sample, while Table 2
qualitatively. Three levels of creativity can be established based on presents correlations between IQ scores measured using the
percentages (low = below the 25th percentile; medium = 26th– different tests of intellectual ability described. Analyses for
74th percentiles; and high = 75th percentile and above). students in grade 3 and 6 are presented separately.

Procedure Third Grade Students


Students were recruited from different schools in Northern Spain. As Table 1 shows, 59% of the students in grade 3 had a
Once the schools were selected, principals and head teachers of medium level of creativity, while only 19% reached high levels of
the participating schools were contacted. They were informed creativity. However, the mean in this variable suggests low levels
about the aims of the study, its voluntary nature and anonymity, of creativity in general (values in this variable can range from 0
and the ethical treatment of the data recorded. The study was to 25).
conducted in accordance with The Code of Ethics of the World Results from the intelligence tests administered placed the
Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki), which reflects intellectual ability of the group around the average, regardless
the ethical principles for research involving humans (Williams, of the test used. Scores were slightly higher in the case of the
2008). Informed consent from families was also obtained. Factor “g” test (i.e., fluid intelligence). Standard deviations were
Researchers who were trained in psychology administered the high, suggesting the presence of large inter-subject variability. IQ
above tests, all of which were conducted using counter-balanced values ranged from 68 to 149 points in the case of the Factor
methodology over the course of the testing, in three different “g” test, between 65 and 135 in the TEA-1, and between 64
testing sessions. Students with severe learning difficulties or and 139 in the Badyg-2. The correlations between the various
special educational needs were excluded from the analyses. measures of intellectual ability were positive and statistically

Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 4 July 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1239


Fernández et al. Identifying Gifted Children

TABLE 1 | Descriptive statistics for the sample (third and sixth grade students).

Third grade students (N = 117) Sixth grade students (N = 119)

M SD M SD

CREA-Q Low n = 29 (24.8%) Low n = 5 (4.2%)


Medium n = 69 (59%) Medium n = 66 (55.5%)
High n = 19 (16.2%) High n = 48 (40.3%)
CREA-RS 7.94 3.81 10.85 3.60
Factor g test 109.28 15.90 94.27 20.87
BADYG 102.03 16.96 99.94 16.71
TEA-1 101.79 12.94 104.35 16.17

CREA-Q, CREA qualitative: low, medium, and high creativity; CREA-RS, CREA raw score; n, number of students by level; M, Mean; SD, Standard Deviation.

TABLE 2 | Bivariate correlations between IQ scores in the different tests (third and sixth grade students).

Third grade students (N = 117) Sixth grade students (N = 119)

Factor g test Badyg-2 TEA-1 Factor g test Badyg-3 TEA-1

Factor g test 0.605∗ 0.373∗ Factor g test 0.159 0.031


Badyg-2 0.502∗ Badyg-3 0.746∗
TEA-1 TEA-1
∗p < 0.001.

significant between all pairs of tests (see Table 2). Statistically Discriminatory Values of the Measures in
significant differences between IQ scores estimated with Factor the Detection of Students with High
“g” test and Badyg-2 (t = 5.369; p < 0.001), and between Factor
“g” test and TEA-1 (t = 4.964; p < 0.001) were found, but not Abilities, and the Intellectual Measures of
between the Badyg-2 and TEA-1 (p = 0.866). Thus, statistically the Students Detected
significant differences were found when the crystallized and fluid To detect students that could be considered high-ability and
intelligence measures were compared, with students’ IQ scores determine the congruence between the tests, a selection of cases
being higher when using the latter measure. in which a student scored 130 or above in the different IQ
tests was made. Results are presented according to school grade
(Tables 3, 4).
Sixth Grade Students
Results show that students in this group obtained higher scores
in CREA than the younger students. However, the scores varied Third Grade Students
from a minimum of 4 to a maximum of 20 in this variable. None of the students in this group obtained an IQ score
Again, the proportion of students with medium creativity was of 130 or above in all three of the tests. However, scores
greater than the proportion of students with low and high from the Factor “g” test and Badyg-2 converged in two cases.
creativity. However, the percentage of students with high levels With respect to the other possible paired-comparisons of the
of creativity was greater than in the third grade students group tests, there were no instances of converging results (Table 3).
(see Table 1). These students showed a medium-to-high percentile in the
Regarding the variable IQ, sixth grade students showed creativity test and a mean IQ of 139 points in the Factor
average levels of intelligence, although a large within-subject “g” test, with values ranging from 132 to 146 points. They
variability was observed. IQ scores ranged from 30 to 139 points also exhibited a mean of 137.5 points in the Badyg-2, with
when the Factor “g” test was used, from 55 to 136 in the case of the values between 136 and 139. Regarding IQ assessed by the
Badyg-3, and from 65 to 135 when the TEA-1 was administered. TEA-1, values were close to 130, ranging from 119 to 128
Correlations between the different measures were positive, but points.
only statistically significant when using the Badyg-3 and TEA- For students who had an IQ of 130 or greater in only one of
1 (see Table 2). At a within-subject level, statistically significant the tests, it can be observed that the Factor “g” test identified
differences in IQ scores were observed when the Factor “g” test the highest number of students who met this criterion (13
and Badyg-3 were compared (t = −2.529; p = 0.013), as well as students), while the TEA-1 was the most restrictive test with
between the Factor “g” test and TEA-1 (t = −4.237; p < 0.001), only one student identified. The Badyg-2, however, detected
and between the Badyg-3 and TEA-1 (t = −4.092; p < 0.001). six students who met the above-mentioned criterion. It should
Students in grade 6 obtained better results in the TEA-1 than in be noted that, in the majority of cases, students identified
the other tests. as having high-abilities showed a medium level of creativity.

Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 5 July 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1239


Fernández et al. Identifying Gifted Children

TABLE 3 | Descriptive statistic of participants with an IQ equal or above 130 in the different tests (third grade students).

Age Gender CREA-Q CREA-RS Factor g test Badyg-2 TEA 1


M (SD) M/F Low Medium High M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)

Convergence between Factor g test and Badyg-2 (n = 2)

8.5 (0.707) 1M/1F – n = 1 (50%) n = 1 (50%) 10.501 (2.242) 139 (9.898) 137.5 (2.123) 125.5 (6.363)
IQ above or equal to 130 in Factor g test (n = 13)

8.38 (0.506) 7M/6F n = 2 (15.4%) n = 10 (76.9%) n = 1 (7.7%) 8.461 (2.781) 136.232 (6.300) 116.846 (14.512) 107.676 (14.332)
IQ above or equal to 130 in Badyg-2 (n = 6)

8.667 (0.516) 3M/3F n = 1 (16.7%) n = 4 (66.6%) n = 1 (16.7%) 8.667 (3.265) 128 (1.714) 132 (3.982) 118.16 (7.935)
IQ above or equal to 130 in TEA 1 (n = 1)

9 1F – n = 1 (100%) – 8 122 127 135

M/F, male/female; CREA-Q, CREA qualitative: low, medium, and high creativity; CREA-RS, CREA raw score; M, Mean; SD, Standard Deviation.

TABLE 4 | Descriptive statistic of participants with an IQ equal or above 130 in the different tests (sixth grade students).

Age Gender CREA-Q CREA-RS Factor g test Badyg-3 TEA-1


M (SD) M/F Low Medium High M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)

Convergence between Factor g test and TEA 1 (n = 1)

12 1M – – n = 1 (100%) 19 139 127 135


IQ above or equal to 130 in Factor g test (n = 4)

11.5 (0.577) 2M/2F – n = 1 (25%) n = 3 (75%) 14.750 (3.862) 132.750 (4.193) 117.250 (7.500) 110.250 (17.967)
IQ above or equal to 130 in Badyg-3 (n = 1)

12 1F – n = 1 (100%) – 9 122 136 119


IQ above or equal to 130 in TEA 1 (n = 6)

11.5 (0.547) 4M/2F – n = 3 (50%) n = 3 (50%) 12.833 (3.656) 101 (22.172) 116.500 (9.995) 135

M/F, male/female; CREA-C, CREA qualitative: low, medium, and high creativity; CREA-RS, CREA raw score; M, Mean; SD, Standard Deviation.

Finally, IQ scores ranged between 130 and 149 points when levels in this variable. IQ values ranged from 130 to 139
measured by the Factor “g” test, and between 130 and 139 in in the case of the students identified by the Factor “g” test,
the case of the Badyg-2. A unique value of 135 was found in the whereas all the students identified by the TEA-1 showed an
TEA-1. IQ of 135. The student identified by the Badyg-3 had an IQ
of 136.
In summary, out of the total of 236 students, 31
Sixth Grade Students
students (20 from third grade and 11 from sixth grade)
Again, none of the students met the criteria of having an IQ
were identified as having an IQ equal to or greater
equal or above 130 points in all three of the tests. Regarding
than 130, considering the different tests separately. This
the convergence between pairs of tests, the Factor “g” test and
corresponds to 13.13% of the sample. There were only
the TEA-1 converged, but only in a single case. The results of
three cases in which two tests produced converging results,
this student can be seen in Table 4. He showed a high level of
which equates to only 1.27% of all students evaluated. No
creativity and his IQ was close to 130 when the Badyg-3 was
convergence of results was found among the three measures of
administered.
intelligence.
In relation to students scoring 130 or above in each of
the tests, results indicated that the TEA-1 was the test that
identified the greatest number of students that met this criterion DISCUSSION
followed by the Factor “g” test. The Badyg-3 was the most
restrictive test in this sense, as none of the students showed This study has two main objectives: analysing the relationship
an IQ score equal to or higher than 130 in this test. Table 4 between intellectual capacities of a group of third and sixth grade
presents the results corresponding to each group. In this students from Northern Spain; and to analyze the discriminatory
case, 50% of the children identified as high-ability students value and congruence between different tests of intelligence
in the different tests displayed a high level of creativity. traditionally used in the identification of high-ability students.
This pattern was different from that found in the group In general, results point to the need to use different tests in
of third grade students, where only 2 out of 20 (10%) of the identification process, as well as to include complementary
the students identified as having high-abilities showed high measures (i.e., creativity) to create a multi-criterial system for

Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 6 July 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1239


Fernández et al. Identifying Gifted Children

the detection of students who fall into this category (Renzulli, Discriminatory Value of the Measures
2012). Identifying High-Ability Students and
Intellectual Results Differences of the
Intellectual Capacities Results of the Students Detected
Students and Congruence among It is necessary to highlight that a reliable evaluation is the basis for
Measures an early detection and tailored intervention, and that currently
In general, results indicated that both third and sixth grade one of the most important concerns regarding higher abilities is
students showed an average intellectual ability (close to 100 in that these students often do not receive recognition, and thus
most of the cases). Regarding congruence among the different appropriate intellectual stimulation, at least in Spain (Calero
intelligence measures used, it is important to note that all and García-Martín, 2014). This can lead to a lack of interest,
the tests administered to third grade students showed positive frustration, and failure at school, as well as have a negative
and significant correlations to one another. A moderate to effect on the development of self-worth and social acceptance
high association between the Factor “g” test and the tests of (Kroesbergen et al., 2016) or result in behavioral problems in
educational and intellectual aptitudes, more related to academic some cases. On the other hand, a false positive may push students
performance (TEA-1 and Badyg), was found. However, for toward overly demanding and frustrating processes that may
sixth grade students, significant correlations were only found exceed the limits of their capacity. A total of 31 students in
between the Badyg-3 and TEA-1 (both assess general intelligence the current study presented an IQ equal or above 130 when
through those abilities related to learning and academic the different tests were used separately, which corresponds to
performance – or crystallized intelligence). Thus, when the 13.13% of the sample. This infers a clear over-estimation of
same tests were administered to older students, the correlation high-ability students, if the acknowledged distribution of IQ
between crystallized intelligence measures increased, while the in the general population is to be taken into account. When
association between crystallized and fluid intelligence measures convergence between any two tests was considered, only three
decreased, or even disappeared. These results are consistent students were identified as being high-ability children, which
with those reported by Pérez and González (2007), who noted corresponds to only 1.27% of the total sample.
that the subscales with a greater cultural basis (and containing In terms of creativity, students in sixth grade showed higher
more elements of the school curriculum) functioned differently scores in this variable in comparison to third grade students. This
according to age, and showed more congruence as children grow result suggests that creativity may increase as students progress
up (and presumably as their knowledge increases). through the different stages of schooling, and draws attention to
In addition, regarding the accuracy of the tests detecting the need for researchers to conduct more comprehensive studies
high-ability students, it should be noted that the congruence on what type of teaching methods favor or hinder creativity in the
among the various measures examined was disturbingly low. classroom.
In this sense, none of the students met the criterion of It is also worth noting some differences in the functioning of
showing an IQ equal to or above 130 in all of the three the tests according to grade level. Regarding third grade students,
measures that were administered in a concurrently. On the other results suggest that the Factor “g” test may be less restrictive than
hand, considering the different tests separately, 13.13% of the the other tests when it comes to detecting potentially higher-
total sample corresponds with students who were identified as ability students, whereas the TEA-1 may be the most liberal
having an IQ equal to or greater than 130, when theoretical in this sense, identifying the greatest number of higher-ability
percentage expectation would be around 2%. Differences in students. However, the discriminating power of the tests in
the estimations provided by the different tests for a same the case of sixth grade students was different. Specifically, the
student were high. This may point to important constraints Factor “g” test and TEA-1 tests were the most and the least
regarding the validity of the tests that are being currently being restrictive tests, respectively. Again it seems that the tests that
used. measure fluid intelligence and those which measure crystallized
It could thus be assumed that, at earlier stages of development, intelligence operate differently at different developmental stages
the different types of intelligence tests can converge, with respect (see Figure 1).
to findings. However, this convergence tends to decrease with With respect to the students’ intellectual variables, results
age, and congruence only stays present in cases in which indicated that a high IQ is not necessarily accompanied by high
those abilities have been facilitated and boosted by on-going creativity, which has already been demonstrated in previous
learning. These findings have some implications for practice. research (Kim, 2005; Marugán et al., 2010; Guignard et al., 2016).
Specifically, the lack of congruence among intelligence measures In the case of third grade students, 20 participants were identified
(such as that identified in this study) may lead to misdiagnosis, as high-ability children by at least one of the tests. However,
preventing some students from receiving adequate support only two of them demonstrated high levels of creativity. Among
for their exceptional needs. Likewise, it is appropriate to the sixth grade students, only six of the 11 who were identified
highlight the need to use different tests of fluid and crystallized as high-ability students also displayed high levels of creativity.
intelligence in the identification of high-ability students, always Studies carried out with large samples of Spanish students, such as
taking into consideration the students’ cognitive developmental that of Castejón et al. (2016), show how in classrooms, although
stages. gifted students are equally categorized, not all of them show

Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 7 July 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1239


Fernández et al. Identifying Gifted Children

FIGURE 1 | Results of the different measurements for groups with IQ higher and lower than 130 (depending on whether the IQ selection was made using g Factor,
Badyg or TEA-1).

the same cognitive-motivational profiles. In this way, there are establish a more detailed profile of the students and thereby
students who exhibit higher scores on creativity and lower scores assist in identifying their additional strengths and weaknesses.
on general mental ability or self-regulation learning strategies This is even more important in countries such as Spain,
(the group called by these authors as “creative gifted”) and there where most of the detection protocols available today, although
are student profiles that do not show special ability in this multidisciplinary, still use a single measure of intellectual ability
variable; for example, students called “gifted achievers,” who show as a starting point to identify those students at a higher level of
high scores in self-regulation learning variables and academic ability (Hernández and Gutiérrez, 2014). In this sense, it would
achievement, and lower scores in creativity; or students called be necessary to continue analysing the correspondence between
“cognitive gifted” who get high scores in general mental ability different assessment tests, as well as between different measures
only. of creativity, in order to better delimitate to what extent the tests
In summary, and as Heller (2004), Ziegler and Stoeger (2010), provide a coherent and comprehensive profile of the students’
and Wellisch and Brown (2012) have pointed out, the use of intellectual abilities.
different tests of intellectual ability in the identification of high- Finally, some limitations should be acknowledged in relation
ability students is necessary. Otherwise, this process may be to the present study. Firstly, the sample size was somewhat
biased. Furthermore, including additional measures not directly limited and also geographically localized, which may pose some
related to intellectual ability, such as creativity, would help constraints concerning generalization of the results. It would

Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 8 July 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1239


Fernández et al. Identifying Gifted Children

be necessary to expand the study sample to include a large ETHICS STATEMENT


number of gifted children and determine if the results obtained
on the lack of congruence between the tests are maintained. This study was carried out in accordance with the
In the current study, the percentage of students with scores recommendations of University of Oviedo with written informed
above 130 IQ points appears biased toward the distribution consent from the parents of all participants. All parents gave
or congruence between the measures. Secondly, future studies written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of
may consider the benefits of including additional variables in Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the University of Oviedo.
research of this kind, such as motivation, personality, learning
styles, socio-cultural conditions, and/or students’ affective-
emotional states. These additions to the methodology utilized
in the present study would undoubtedly enhance the results AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
of any future investigations of the multidimensional construct
EF, TG, and CR have participated in the design, analysis and
widely known as “higher ability” (Reis and Renzulli, 2010;
drafting of the paper. OA-G, and AV have participated in the
Sternberg, 2010; Hernández and Gutiérrez, 2014). Finally,
application of the measures and drafting of the paper.
although through different tests, the same construct (IQ) has
been evaluated. Thus, the possibility of an average regression
effect or profiles with cluster latent analysis, which is common
when evaluating students in a short period of time, has to ACKNOWLEDGMENT
be considered. It would be interesting to extend the time
between evaluations in order to control for this effect in future This work has been supported by a project of the Principality of
research. Asturias (FC-15-GRUPIN14-053).

REFERENCES Jauk, E., Benedek, M., Dunst, B., and Neubauer, A. C. (2013). The relationship
between intelligence and creativity: new support for the threshold hypothesis
Baker, J. (2011). Stability of racial differences in gifted education: the case for by means of empirical breakpoint detection. Intelligence 41, 212–221. doi: 10.
stereotype threat. Talent Dev. Excell. 3, 27–28. 1016/j.intell.2013.03.003
Calero, M. D., García, M. B., Jiménez, M. I., Kazén, M., and Araque, A. (2007). Jeltova, I., and Grigorenko, E. (2005). “Systemic approaches to giftedness:
Self-regulation advantage for high-IQ children: findings from a research study. contributions of russian psychology,” in Conceptions of Giftedness, eds R. J.
Learn. Individ. Differ. 17, 328–343. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2007.03.012 Sternberg and J. E. Davidson (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press),
Calero, M. D., and García-Martín, M. B. (2011). The Evaluation of Gifted Children: 171–187.
When, Why and How? Available at: http://www.infocop.es/view_article.asp?id= Kim, K. H. (2005). Can only intelligent people be creative? A meta-analysis.
3304 J. Second. Gifted Educ. 16, 57–66. doi: 10.4219/jsge-2005-473
Calero, M. D., and García-Martín, M. B. (2014). Temporal stability of IQ and Kroesbergen, E. H., Van Hooidonk, M., Van Viersen, S., Middel-Lalleman,
learning potential in gifted children: diagnostic implications. Anal. Psicol. 30, M. M. N., and Reijnders, J. J. W. (2016). The psychological well-being of
512–521. doi: 10.6018/analesps.30.2.163801 early identified gifted children. Gifted Child Quart. 60, 16–30. doi: 10.1177/
Carman, C. A. (2013). Comparing apples and oranges. fifteen years of definitions 0016986215609113
of giftedness in research. J. Adv. Acad. 24, 52–70. doi: 10.1177/1932202X124 Lovett, B. J., and Lewandowski, L. J. (2006). Gifted students with learning
72602 disabilities: who are they? J. Learn. Disabil. 39, 515–527. doi: 10.1177/
Castejón, J. L., Gilar, R., Miñano, P., and González, M. (2016). Latent class cluster 00222194060390060401
analysis in exploring different profiles of gifted and talented students. Learn. Lovett, B. J., and Sparks, R. (2011). The identification and performance of gifted
Individ. Differ. 50, 166–174. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2016.08.003 students with learning disability diagnoses: a quantitative synthesis. J. Learn.
Cattell, R. B., and Cattell, A. K. S. (1994). Factor “g” manual. Scales 2 &3. Madrid: Disabil. 46, 304–316. doi: 10.1177/0022219411421810
TEA Ediciones. Marugán, M., Carbonero, M. A., Torres, M. H., and León, B. (2010). Analysis
Corbalán, F. J., Martínez, F., Donolo, D. S., Alonso, C., Tejerina, M., and Limiñana, of connections between creativity and high capacities. Electron. J. Res. Educ.
R. M. (2003). Creative Intelligence CREA, Manual. Madrid: TEA Ediciones. Psychol. 10, 1081–1098.
Erwin, J. O., and Worrell, F. C. (2012). Assessment practices and the McGrew, K. S. (2005). “The Cattell–Horn–Carroll theory of cognitive abilities,”
underrepresentation of minority students in gifted and talented education. in Contemporary Intellectual Assessment: Theories, Tests, and Issues, 2nd Edn,
J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 30, 74–87. doi: 10.1177/0734282911428197 eds D. P. Flanagan and P. L. Harrison (New York, NY: Guilford Press),
Ferrando, M., Ferrándiz, C., Prieto, M. D., Bermejo, M. R., and Sáinz, M. (2008). 136–181.
Creativity in gifted & talented children. Int. J. Creat. Problem Solv. 18, 35–47. Moon, T. R., and Brighton, C. M. (2008). Primary teachers’ conceptions
Finney, S. J., and Di Stefano, C. (2006). “Non-normal and categorical data in of giftedness. J. Educ. Gifted 31, 447–480. doi: 10.4219/jeg-
structural equation modeling,” in Structural Equation Modeling: A second 2008-793
course, eds G. R. Hancock and R. O. Muller (Greenwich, CT: Information Age), Naglieri, J. A., and Ford, D. (2003). Addressing underrepresentation of gifted
269–314. minority children using the Naglieri Nonverbal Ability Test (NNAT). Gifted
Freeman, J. (2011). A wish for the gifted and talented. Talent Dev. Excell. 3, 57–58. Child Q. 47, 155–160. doi: 10.1177/001698620304700206
Guignard, J. H., Kermarrec, S., and Tordjman, S. (2016). Relationships between Pérez, L., and González, C. (2007). Differences in development and intellectual
intelligence and creativity in gifted and non-gifted children. Learn. Individ. variability among students with high capacity. Faisca 12, 106–117.
Differ. 52, 209–215. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2015.07.006 Peyre, H., Ramus, F., Melchior, M., Forhan, A., Heude, B., and Gauvrit, N. (2016).
Heller, K. A. (2004). Identification of gifted and talented students. Psychol. Sci. 46, Emotional, behavioral and social difficulties among high-IQ children during the
302–323. preschool period: results of the EDEN mother–child cohort. Person. Ind. Diff.
Hernández, D., and Gutiérrez, M. (2014). The study of high intellectual ability in 94, 366–371. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.02.014
Spain: analysis of the current situation. Rev. Educ. 364, 251–272. doi: 10.4438/ Pfeiffer, S. I. (2012). Current perspectives on the indentification and assessment of
1988-592X-RE-2014-364-261 gifted students. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 30, 3–9. doi: 10.1177/0734282911428192

Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 9 July 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1239


Fernández et al. Identifying Gifted Children

Pfeiffer, S. I. (2015). The tripartite model on high capacity and best practices in the Thurstone, L. L. (1938). Primary Mental Abilities. Chicago, IL: University of
evaluation of the ablest. Rev. Educ. 368, 66–95. doi: 10.4438/1988-592X-RE- Chicago Press.
2015-368-293 Thurstone, L. L., and Thurstone, T. H. (2005). Test of Educational Aptitudes.
Pierson, E. E., Kilmer, L. M., Rothlisberg, B. A., and McIntosh, D. E. (2012). Use of Madrid: TEA Ediciones S.A.
brief intelligence tests in the identification of giftedness. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. Tourón, J., Peralta, F., and Repáraz, C. (1998). Intellectual Giftedness. Models,
30, 10–24. doi: 10.1177/0734282911428193 Identification and Educational Strategies. Pamplona: EUNSA.
Reis, S. M., and Renzulli, J. S. (2004). Current research on the social and emotional Tourón, J., Repáraz, C., and Peralta, F. (2006). Nominations of teachers
development of gifted and talented students: good news and future possibilities. in identifying students of high intellectual capacity. Sobredotação 7,
Psychol. Schools 41, 119–130. doi: 10.1002/pits.10144 7–25.
Reis, S. M., and Renzulli, J. S. (2009). Myth 1: the gifted and talented constitute Wechsler, D. (2014). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Spanish, 5th Edn.
one single homogenous group and giftedness is a way of being that stays in the Madrid: Pearson Education.
person over time and experience. Gifted Child Q. 53, 233–235. doi: 10.1177/ Wellisch, M., and Brown, J. (2012). An integrated identification and intervention
0016986209346824 model for intellectually gifted children. J. Adv. Acad. 23, 145–167. doi: 10.1177/
Reis, S. M., and Renzulli, J. S. (2010). Is there still a need for gifted education? 1932202X12438877
An examination of current research. Learn. Individ. Differ. 20, 308–317. Wet, C. F., and Gubbins, E. J. (2011). Teachers’ beliefs about culturally,
doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2009.10.012 linguistically, and economically diverse gifted students: a quantitative study.
Renzulli, J. S. (1978). What makes giftedness? Reexamination of definition. Phi Roeper Rev. 33, 97–108. doi: 10.1080/02783193.2011.554157
Delta Kappan 60, 180–184. Williams, J. R. (2008). Revising the declaration of Helsinki. World Med. J. 54,
Renzulli, J. S. (2012). Reexamining the role of gifted education and talent 120–125.
development for the 21st Century: a four-part theoretical approach. Gifted Child Yuste, C., Martínez, R., and Galve, J. L. (2005). Battery of Differential and General
Q. 56, 150–159. doi: 10.1177/0016986212444901 Skills (BAD&G). Madrid: CEPE.
Renzulli, J. S., and Gaesser, A. H. (2015). A multi criteria system for the Ziegler, A., and Stoeger, H. (2010). How fine motor skills influence the
identification of high achieving and creative/productive giftedness. Rev. Educ. assessment of high abilities and underachievement in math. J. Educ. Gifted 34,
368, 93–131. doi: 10.4438/1988-592X-RE-2015-368-290 195–219.
Sabiston, C. M., O’Loughlin, E., Brunet, J., Chaiton, M., Low, N. C., Barnett, T.,
et al. (2013). Linking depression symptom trajectories in adolescence to physical Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare that the research was
activity and team sports participation in young adults. Prev. Med. 56, 95–98. conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could
doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2012.11.013 be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Sánchez-Sánchez, F., Santamaría, P., and Abad, F. J. (2015). Matrices. General
Intelligence Test. Madrid: TEA Ediciones S.A. Copyright © 2017 Fernández, García, Arias-Gundín, Vázquez and Rodríguez. This
Silverman, L. K. (2009). The two-edged sword of compensation: how the gifted is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons
cope with learning disabilities. Gifted Educ. Int. 25, 115–130. doi: 10.1177/ Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums
026142940902500203 is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the
Sternberg, R. J. (2010). Assessment of gifted students for identification purposes: original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic
New techniques for a new millennium. Learn. Individ. Differ. 20, 327–336. practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply
doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2009.08.003 with these terms.

Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 10 July 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 1239

Вам также может понравиться