Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 26

INT J LANG COMMUN DISORD, MAY–JUNE 2015,

VOL. 50, NO. 3, 273–297

Research Report
‘MetaTaal’: enhancing complex syntax in children with specific language
impairment—a metalinguistic and multimodal approach
Rob Zwitserlood†§, Frank Wijnen†, Marjolijn van Weerdenburg‡ and Ludo Verhoeven‡
†Utrecht Institure of Linguistics (OTS), Utrecht University, Utrecht, the Netherlands
‡Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, the Netherlands
§Royal Auris Group, Gouda, the Netherlands
(Received October 2013; accepted July 2014)

Abstract
Background: Currently, most research on the effective treatment of morphosyntax in children with specific language
impairment (SLI) pertains to younger children. In the last two decades, several studies have provided evidence that
intervention for older school-age children with SLI can be effective. These metalinguistic intervention approaches
teach grammatical rules explicitly and use shapes and colours as two-dimensional visual support. Reading or
writing activities form a substantial part of these interventions. However, some children with SLI are poor readers
and might benefit more from an approach that is less dependent on literacy skills.
Aims: To examine the effectiveness of a combined metalinguistic and multimodal approach in older school-age
children with SLI. The intervention was adapted to suit poor readers and targeted the improvement of relative
clause production, because relative clauses still pose difficulties for older children with SLI.
Methods & Procedures: Participants were 12 monolingual Dutch children with SLI (mean age 11;2). All children
visited a special school for children with speech and language disorders in the Netherlands. A quasi-experimental
multiple-baseline design was chosen to evaluate the effectiveness of the intervention. A set of tasks was constructed
to test relative clause production and comprehension. Two balanced versions were alternated in order to suppress
a possible learning effect from multiple presentations of the tasks. After 3 monthly baseline measurements,
the children received individual treatment with a protocolled intervention programme twice a week during
5 weeks. The tests were repeated directly post-therapy and at a retention measurement 3 months later. During the
intervention programme, the speech therapist delivering the treatment remained blind to the test results.
Outcomes & Results: No significant changes were found during the baseline measurements. However, measurement
directly post-therapy showed that 5 h of intervention produced significant improvement on the relative clause
production tasks, but not on the relative clause comprehension task. The gains were also maintained 3 months
later.
Conclusions & Implications: The motor and tactile/kinesthetic dimensions of the ‘MetaTaal’ metalinguistic in-
tervention approach are a valuable addition to the existing metalinguistic approaches. This study supports the
evidence that grammatical skills in older school-age children with SLI can be remediated with direct intervention
using a metalinguistic approach. The current tendency to diminish direct intervention for older children with SLI
should be reconsidered.

Keywords: specific language impairment, intervention, metalinguistic, Dutch, multimodal, school-age children with
SLI.

What this paper adds?


What is already known on this subject?
There is evidence that older school-age children with SLI can benefit from metalinguistic intervention. Previous
studies targeted various grammatical constructions (e.g. passives, verb argument structure, wh-questions and past
tense) and all used shapes, colours and written language as two-dimensional visual support. Relative clauses are
notably difficult for older school-age children with SLI, and therefore were chosen as the intervention target for this
group study.

Address correspondence to: Rob Zwitserlood, Royal Auris Group, Bachstraat 9, 2807 HZ, Gouda, the Netherlands; e-mail: r.zwitserlood@
gmail.com
International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders
ISSN 1368-2822 print/ISSN 1460-6984 online  C 2015 Royal College of Speech and Language Therapists

DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12131
274 Rob Zwitserlood et al.

What this study adds?


This study reports positive results of a metalinguistic intervention approach using three-dimensional material.
Children improved significantly on relative clause production tasks. The motor and tactile/kinesthetic dimensions of
the programme can be regarded as a valuable addition to the metalinguistic approaches studied thus far.

Introduction 2005) may be related to a lack of efficacy studies (Ebbels


2007).
Specific language impairment (SLI) is a neurodevel-
Although there are no official clinical guidelines in
opmental language disorder that affects approximately
the Netherlands for the treatment of children with lan-
7% of kindergarten children (Tomblin et al. 1997) and
guage disorders (Gerrits 2011), standard clinical practice
which can persist into adolescence and adulthood (Aram
in Dutch special education settings is that treatment for
et al. 1984, Beitchman et al. 1996, Stothard et al. 1998).
children with SLI older than 9 years focuses on mini-
Children with SLI can experience difficulties in many
mizing the impact of disability, rather than remediation
areas of language, but problems with verb morphology
of grammar or vocabulary. Most therapy resources are
and complex syntax are generally considered to be core
also allocated to the younger children between the ages
symptoms of the disorder (Leonard 1998). Persisting
4 and 8 years (Fortgens 2009, Willemsen 2011).
language impairment has severe effects both on academic
The policy to limit language interventions for older
achievements (Simkin and Conti-Ramsden 2006) and
children with SLI can be questioned. Impoverished
psychosocial development (Clegg et al. 2005).
grammatical abilities impede a variety of crucial domains
The general idea is that speech and language prob-
in the development of children and young adolescents
lems should be identified and treated as early as possible
with SLI. For example, grammar plays an important role
to prevent future difficulties. Early language-orientated
in narrative skills (Norbury and Bishop 2003) and these
interventions can prevent the development of other
skills are central to daily social interactions and academic
learning difficulties (Gillon 2000) and social–emotional
performance (Conti-Ramsden et al. 2009, Tannock et al.
problems (Robertson and Ellis Weismer 1999). Evi-
1993). Poor language skills are reported to be related to
dently, to date, intervention for young children has re-
a lower social and behavioural status. Children with SLI
ceived most attention from researchers. For instance,
also have a higher risk of becoming regular victims of
all 25 studies used in the meta-analysis by Law et al.
bullying (Conti-Ramsden and Botting 2004). Further-
(2004) on the efficacy of treatment for children with
more, the level of independence of adolescents with SLI
developmental speech and language delay or disorders
is negatively impacted by poor language and literacy
concerned children age between 2;01 and 6;01 years.
skills (Conti-Ramsden and Durkin 2008).
Furthermore, only two out of the 36 papers reviewed in
In summary, intervention programmes to enhance
Law et al. (2003) reported results on children older than
grammar for children with SLI between 9 and 12 years
8 years. However, results of early intervention studies
are sparse, whereas the impact of poor grammatical skills
have to be interpreted with caution, as language de-
may be large in later adolescence. The current situation
lays appear to resolve spontaneously in a considerable
can only change if more evidence for the effectiveness of
proportion of 2-year-old children (Bishop 2008, Wake
intervention for older children is provided. The present
et al. 2011). On the other hand, it has also been found
study aims to contribute to the body of evidence by
that language difficulties can persist in children with
investigating a multimodal and metalinguistic interven-
SLI (Law et al. 2000, Johnson et al. 1999). The effect
tion approach in Dutch children with SLI from 9 years
of speech and language therapy appears to depend on
upwards.
the type of problems: clear effects are found for children
with phonological or vocabulary difficulties, but the ef-
fects of interventions for grammatical problems are less Metalinguistic intervention: rationale and
robust (Law et al. 2003).
effectiveness
At present, research on the efficacy of treatments for
older children (i.e. between 9 and 12 years) with per- Therapy approaches for young (pre-school) children
sistent language difficulties has remained scarce. More- mainly train morphosyntactic skills implicitly, using
over, the existing evidence on effective interventions in techniques like recasting, elicited imitation and mod-
older children has not been disseminated widely (Bishop elling (Fey and Proctor-Williams 2000). The idea be-
2009). The fact that many services (in the UK at least) hind these interventions is that young children with
provide very little and often no therapy to children over SLI are able to learn the selected grammatical targets
11 years of age (Dockrell et al. 2006, Lindsay et al. 2002, when they are presented with an increased frequency
‘MetaTaal’ and SLI 275
and salience. Nonetheless, difficulties with morphosyn- independence, as they can correct their own mistakes
tax and complex syntax often persist in older school- and better understand why they are wrong (Ebbels
age children with SLI (Bishop et al. 2000, Rosen et al. 2007).
2009, van der Lely 1997, van der Lely and Stollwerck The literature on metalinguistic intervention ranges
1997, van Weerdenburg et al. 2006). This suggests that from uncontrolled case studies to randomized controlled
they have difficulties with learning language implicitly group studies in various clinical settings and reports
and, therefore, a different approach might be required on varied research designs (see Ebbels 2014, for a re-
(Ebbels 2007). As children grow older, they develop the view). Most studies used colour codes to help children
ability to reflect on the nature and properties of language in structuring sentences and identifying specific syntac-
enabling them to make explicit judgements about lin- tic elements. These approaches are based on ‘Colourful
guistic form (Menyuk and Chesnick 1997, van Kleeck Semantics’ (Bryan 1997) that was developed to highlight
1982). Metalinguistic interventions appeal to this abil- the grammatical structure of sentences.
ity by explicit instruction on language structures, often Several uncontrolled case studies using metalinguis-
supported with specific visual cues (Ebbels 2014). In the tic intervention can be found in the literature. For exam-
past two decades, several studies have provided evidence ple, one case study using the Colourful Semantics system
for the effectiveness of metalinguistic interventions that reported significant progress in content and structure of
focused on the remediation of morphosyntax and com- expressive language of two children with receptive and
plex syntax for older children and adolescents with SLI expressive language difficulties (Spooner 2002). How-
(Ebbels 2007, Ebbels and van der Lely 2001, Ebbels et al. ever, in another study using this system, only one of two
2007, Hirschman 2000, Levy and Friedman 2009). children with SLI made some progress after a 1-year
The rationale for metalinguistic intervention is intervention (Guendouzi 2003).
based on the hypothesis that children with SLI have dif- Levy and Friedman (2009) studied the effectiveness
ficulties learning grammar implicitly and benefit from of an intervention combining elements of Colourful Se-
explicit teaching of grammatical rules (Ebbels 2014, mantics (Bryan 1997) and the Shape Coding Scheme
Ebbels et al. 2014). This hypothesis is in line with developed by Ebbels (2007) on the comprehension and
results suggesting that (implicit) procedural memory production of structures involving syntactic movement
is impaired in children with SLI (Lum et al. 2012, in a 12-year-old boy with severe SLI. Written and oral ex-
Ullman and Pierpont 2005), whereas declarative mem- ercises were used to teach syntactic movement explicitly
ory (which supports explicit learning) appears to re- in various syntactic structures. Improvement was seen
main largely intact and can therefore play a compen- directly after treatment and was maintained 10 months
satory role in learning grammatical rules (Ullman and after.
Pierpont 2005). Results showing that grammatical abil- In a multiple case study involving four 11–12-year-
ities were correlated with declarative memory skills in old children with severe receptive and expressive SLI,
children with SLI, but with procedural memory in typ- Ebbels and van der Lely (2001) investigated the effi-
ically developing children (Lum et al. 2012) are in line cacy of Shape Coding for improving production and
with this hypothesis. These findings indicate that chil- comprehension of passives and wh-questions. Results
dren with SLI can engage their largely intact declara- showed that the first objective, i.e. improving the use
tive memory system in order to learn grammatical rules of passives, was reached for three out of four children.
explicitly instead of having to rely on their impaired Furthermore, progress was seen on production and com-
procedural memory system (Ebbels et al. 2014). prehension of wh-questions post-therapy, but gains were
A typical feature of existing metalinguistic interven- not maintained for most of the children at follow-up
tion practices is the use of visual support, such as writ- 10 weeks after therapy (Ebbels and van der Lely 2001).
ing, or the encoding of linguistic structure by shapes or In the study of Kulkarni et al. (2013) two 8-year-old
colours. Spoken language is transitory and a visual rep- children were treated with Shape Coding to improve
resentation offers children with SLI compensation for their use of past tense in both structured tasks and con-
their limited capacity to process and store verbal infor- versation. One child improved on the structured tasks,
mation (Bishop 1992, 1994, Ellis Weismer and Evans but needed a generalization intervention before gains in
2002, Montgomery 2000, 2003, Quail et al. 2009). conversation could be observed. The other child made
Provision of visual cues can also help a child to de- modest gains on both structured tasks and conversation
velop a concrete explicit representation of grammati- without recourse to the generalization therapy.
cal structures, which may support comprehension and A limited number of studies on the effects of met-
production (Ebbels and van der Lely 2001, Hirschman alinguistic approaches used (quasi-)experimental group
2000). Furthermore, a visual coding system can be used designs. Bolderson et al. (2011) found progress in both
to correct the errors children have made, making their comprehension and production after 8 weeks of inter-
mistakes visible, which is expected to increase their vention based on Colourful Semantics (Bryan 1997) in
276 Rob Zwitserlood et al.
six 5–6-year-old children with expressive language dif- metalinguistic therapy also works without deploying
ficulties, which contrasted with absence of change dur- written language.
ing a preceding baseline period. Another group study
(Hirschman 2000) involved a total of 67 children with
Multimodal learning
SLI in two groups (with mean ages of 9;4 and 10;6 years)
who received 55 group therapy sessions of metalinguistic The metalinguistic approaches discussed so far all used
training in a classroom setting during a 9-month period. two-dimensional visual support systems consisting of
These children were compared with a non-treated ref- different shapes and colours. Written language also
erence group of children with SLI matched on grade formed an important component of the interventions
level. The metalinguistic training aimed at improving used with older children with SLI (e.g. Hirschman
the use of complex sentences, utilizing written language 2000, Levy and Friedman 2009). However, a substan-
and illustrations. A comparison of pre- and post-therapy tial proportion of children with SLI experience prob-
test scores showed that the use of complex sentences im- lems in reading and spelling (McArthur et al. 2000, van
proved in oral and written language in the experimental Weerdenburg et al. 2011). It is likely that for such chil-
group (Hirschman 2000). dren, employing reading activities in therapy may be
In another study, Shape Coding was used to enhance more of a hindrance than a help.
the comprehension of dative forms and comparative For the present study, we searched for a dynamic,
questions, as well as the use of past tense in written multimodal approach that would not only make use
work. Nine pupils (aged between 11 and 13 years) took of the visual and auditory channels, but also employ
part; six of them made progress, but the method was not the tactile/kinesthetic and motor systems. According to
effective for all children for all goals (Ebbels 2007). Shams and Seitz (2008), the human brain has evolved to
To date, two randomized controlled trials (RCT) develop, learn and operate optimally in multimodal en-
have been published that target individual metalinguis- vironments. Sensori-motor engagement raises the level
tic therapy approaches. The first RCT investigated the of active participation in the children and may also
production of verb argument structure in 27 children enhance their enjoyment and motivation for therapy
with SLI (aged 11;0–16;1 years) with a pre-therapy, (Sankey et al. 2010). Therefore, an intervention ap-
post-therapy and follow-up assessment (Ebbels et al. proach encompassing a tactile/kinesthetic and motor
2007). The children were randomly assigned to three component was expected to support language learn-
conditions, namely Shape Coding, verb semantic ther- ing effectively. In education, multimodal training ap-
apy or a usual care control therapy. Both the Shape proaches are not new at all (e.g. Montessori 1912, 1949).
Coding approach and the semantic therapy yielded In fact, what Montessori achieved through careful ob-
significant progress, which was also maintained after servation of child development is now substantiated by
3 months (Ebbels et al. 2007). research. The processing of information entering the
The second RCT study used a waiting list condition cognitive system through multiple channels helps to cir-
and focused on comprehension of coordinating con- cumvent the limited processing capabilities of the indi-
junctions (Ebbels et al. 2014). Fourteen children (aged vidual channels (Clark and Paivio 1991). Consequently,
11;3–16;1 years) with receptive and expressive language a greater total of information can be processed when
impairment received eight 30-min individual sessions of shared between multiple senses (Birsh 2005).
explicit teaching with visual support of the Shape Cod- A multimodal and metalinguistic intervention ap-
ing system. Results showed Shape Coding therapy led proach in which language is represented by manipu-
to a significant improvement in comprehension of the lable items was found in the ‘Grammar in Form and
targeted conjunctions as well as on the Test of Reception Colour’ programme, originally developed for Danish
of Grammar (TROG-2; Bishop 2003), but not on a test by K. Thyme to remedy grammatical problems in chil-
of passives (Ebbels et al. 2014). dren with severe hearing impairment (K. Thyme, per-
In conclusion, studies on metalinguistic interven- sonal communication, 8 December 2010). Grammar in
tion for children with SLI aged 8 years and over re- Form and Colour uses Lego R
bricks as representations
port favourable results for a wide array of different of elements of grammar. Word classes and functional
grammatical targets. However, while there is a grow- morphemes are represented by bricks of specific colours,
ing body of evidence in research on interventions for shapes and sizes. By manipulating the bricks and laying
grammar in school-age children with SLI, much work them out sequentially on a base plate, children learn
still has to be done. For example, it is still unclear which (literally) to build and expand sentences.
grammatical targets are most appropriate at what ages, Thyme’s intervention programme was adapted for
whether individual intervention is equally effective as Dutch by van Geel (1973), but interest in the approach
group therapy, whether digital media (apps) can con- has gradually waned, and it has not been developed fur-
tribute to the efficacy of intervention, and whether ther. For our study, we adopted the programme and
‘MetaTaal’ and SLI 277
expanded it with new codes not present in the original used in developmental studies on RCs and involves four
programme. These codes enabled the representation of different RC types. Sentences in (1–4) give an example
complex sentences, notably relative clauses (RCs) and of each type.
relative pronouns. Because of the changes to the origi-
nal intervention method, the new version was renamed (1) The boy (S) [that (S) drank the juice] fell ill. (SS)
‘MetaTaal’, a compound of the Dutch words for ‘met- (2) Mary kissed the boy (O) [that (S) brought the flow-
alinguistic’ and ‘language’. Before we discuss the present ers]. (OS)
study in detail, we address the topic of RCs in Dutch (3) The boy (S) [that (O) John kicked] ran away. (SO)
which forms the content of the intervention to be tested. (4) John kicked the boy (O) [that (O) Mary knew].
(OO)
Relative clauses (RCs) and (Dutch) SLI
Sentences (1) and (2) are classified as subject RCs,
Results have shown that children with SLI fail on RC
because the relative pronoun takes the subject role.
comprehension and production at ages beyond the nor-
Sentences in examples (3) and (4) are defined as ob-
mal age of RC acquisition in different languages, such
ject RCs, because the relative pronoun takes the ob-
as English (Adams 1990, Schuele and Nicholls 2000),
ject role. RCs can furthermore be classified on the
Italian (Cipriani et al. 1998), Danish (Jensen de López
basis of their position within the sentence, and can ei-
et al. 2014), Hebrew (Friedmann and Novogrodsky
ther be centre-embedded (examples 1 and 3) or right-
2004), Greek (Stavrakaki 2001), Swedish (Håkansson
branching (examples 2 and 4). In the centre-embedded
and Hansson 2000), and Dutch (Zwitserlood 2014).
versions, there is a distance (non-local dependency) be-
Several explanations have been proposed to account
tween the subject and the finite verb of the main clause.
for this phenomenon. It is beyond the limits of this
This distance is determined by the length of the RC.
study to discuss these in any detail. Generally speak-
From a processing view, sentences containing (longer)
ing, nativist approaches (e.g. van der Lely et al. 1998,
centre-embedded clauses are harder to process because of
Friedmann and Novogrodsky 2004) argue that children
these non-local dependencies (Just and Carpenter 1992,
with SLI have a specific deficit in syntax that prevents
Gibson 1998).
them from building the grammatical structure underly-
Dutch and English RCs are different in a number
ing RCs. By contrast, processing hypotheses pose that
of grammatical aspects. A first difference concerns the
the difficulty children with SLI have with RCs is due to
relative pronoun. In English, the relative pronoun in
deficits of the processing mechanisms underlying lan-
object RCs (in italics in examples 3 and 4) is optional,
guage perception or production. Cases have been made
whereas in Dutch it is obligatory. Second, Dutch RCs
for severe working memory limitations (e.g. Deevy and
require gender agreement between the head noun and
Leonard 2004, Hestvik et al. 2010), reduced attentional
the relative pronoun. The relative pronoun can either
capacity (Finneran et al. 2009), general processing speed
take common gender form ‘die’, or neuter gender form
(Leonard et al. 2007), and executive function (Im-Bolter
‘dat’. A third difference between Dutch and English RCs
et al. 2006). Recently, usage-based approaches to lan-
relates to their verb placement requirements. Dutch is a
guage acquisition have linked the RC acquisition tra-
so-called SOV + verb-second language. The finite verb
jectory in typically developing children to quantitative
always takes second position in main clauses, but appears
properties of the input (e.g. Brandt et al. 2009). How-
in clause-final position in subordinate (relative) clauses.
ever, these approaches have so far not been brought to
A fourth contrast is that, because of these verb place-
bear on the delay of RC acquisition in children with
ment requirements, Dutch RCs with animate subjects
SLI.
and objects are not disambiguated by word order, as is
In order to appreciate the difficulties children with
the case in English. Such clauses can remain ambiguous
SLI experience with RCs, some elaboration on Dutch
between a subject RC reading and an object RC reading
RCs is appropriate at this point. An RC can be defined
(example 5). Disambiguation can be effected by mor-
as a subordinate clause that modifies a (head) noun or a
phosyntactic means (e.g. subject–verb agreement, as in
noun phrase in an adjacent main clause. The classifica-
example 6), or—defeasibly—by pragmatic plausibility
tion of RCs is based on two structural features. The first
(example 7):
of these is the syntactic role of the head, i.e. the main
clause element that is modified by the RC, which mostly
is either subject (S) or object (O). The second feature (5) Het konijntje SING (S), dat (S or O) de jager SING (S
is the syntactic role of the gap, i.e. the element that is or O) ziet SING , zit in het gras.
gapped or relativized inside the RC, which is either the
subject or object of the RC. The taxonomy put forward The rabbit (S), that (S) sees the hunter, sits in the grass.
by Goodluck and Tavakolian (1982) has been widely (SS), or:
278 Rob Zwitserlood et al.
The rabbit (S), (that) (O) the hunter sees, sits in the Table 1. Means, standard deviations and range of age, and
grass. (SO) quotient scores of the Wechsler Non Verbal-NL, Peabody
Picture Vocabulary Test-NL, and CELF-4-NL subtests
(6) Het konijntje SING (S), dat (O) de jagers PLU (S) zien Mean SD Range
PLU , zit in het gras. Age (months) 134.75 13.72 112–154
Wechsler Non Verbal-NL 99.64 10.31 85–116
The rabbit (that) the hunters see, sits in the grass. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-NL 88.09 9.70 71–102
CELF Number Repetition Total 82.92 20.60 55–120
Formulated sentences 83.73 7.99 75–95
(7) Het konijntje dat de jager op de korrel heeft, zit in Recalling sentences 71.64 12.19 55–96
het gras. Word definitions 77.75 11.78 55–90
Word classes 84.45 13.48 63–110
The rabbit (S) (that) the hunter draws a bead on, sits in Understanding spoken paragraphs 91.82 8.15 80–105
the grass.
In summary, (Dutch) RCs can be regarded as com-
plex grammatical structures with characteristics such scores increased post-therapy, this increase should be at-
as (1) variety in types of RCs; (2) gender agreement tributed to the ‘MetaTaal’ intervention. Furthermore,
between relative pronoun and the relativized noun; maintained improvement at a retention measurement
(3) different verb placement requirements in main and administered 12 weeks later without providing any fur-
subordinate clauses; and (4) non-local dependencies as- ther intervention would indicate that the children had
sociated to centre-embedded versions. effectively improved on RC use.
In view of their complexity, targeting RCs in an in-
tervention study is challenging. If we should find that
school-aged children with SLI can learn to use and un- Method
derstand RCs through the ‘MetaTaal’ programme, there
Participants
will be all the more reason to adopt this approach for
children with persistent language difficulties older than A total of 13 monolingual Dutch-speaking children with
8 or 9 years. SLI were included. The descriptives of the participants
can be found in table 1. All children were admitted to
a special school for children with language impairments
The present study
after extensive clinical and psychometric examination by
In the present study, a concurrent within-subjects an independent team of specialists (i.e. a psychologist, a
multiple-baseline design was used. In this quasi- speech–language therapist, an educational specialist and
experimental design, the children with SLI were tested an audiologist). The admission to Dutch special edu-
three times on criterion-referenced tasks during three cation is only valid for 3 years, and children have to
subsequent monthly measurements (T1–T3) prior to be reassessed within this period. The criteria for admis-
the treatment. These tasks were especially constructed sion to special education entail that children have to be
for this study to test RC production and comprehen- diagnosed as having SLI.
sion. After the 3 months of baseline testing, treatment This implies that: (1) The children have to score
was started for all children. Children were assessed di- –1.5 SD on standardized tests for at least two out of
rectly after treatment at T4, and 12 weeks after the last four language domains (speech, auditory processing,
therapy session at T5. lexical–semantic knowledge, morphosyntactic knowl-
An advantage of this approach is that it moderates edge). Children answering to this criterion may also
several threats to validity, history effects in particular have additional pragmatic problems, which are weighed
(Carr 2005, Harris and Jenson 1985). However, this additionally by the admission committee. (2) Grades at
quasi-experimental design does not meet the require- maths and reading skills also have to be poor (lowest
ments of an RCT. It only involved a comparison of no 10%). (3) The language problems must be persistent,
treatment (i.e. T1–T3) with treatment (i.e. T4 and T5) which in practice means that 6 months of language ther-
conditions and the intervention procedures and goal apy had not been enough not produce sufficient progress
were limited. Accordingly, it can be regarded as an early on language tests. Furthermore, the children with SLI
efficacy study (Fey and Finestack 2009). in this study had non-verbal cognitive abilities within
In this multiple-baseline design, participants can be normal limits and no diagnosed co-morbidity such as
regarded to act as their own controls. Therefore, we ADHD or autism spectrum disorders. All children were
reasoned that if the school-aged children with SLI did tested by the school therapists on a yearly basis; the lan-
not improve in their production and comprehension of guage scores in table 1 refer to the last assessment prior
RCs during the pre-therapy baseline period, and if their to the onset of the intervention.
‘MetaTaal’ and SLI 279
The participating children had a mean age of 11;2 speech therapist and the first author discussed the infor-
years (SD = 1;1 years, age range = 9;3–12;8 years). mation in the logbook in order to make sure that pro-
Around this age the children supposedly have acquired tocol was followed. After the intervention programme
reading skills, and their metalinguistic awareness is de- was completed, the children did not receive any further
veloped to such an extent that they are able to reflect speech/language therapy. The retention assessment was
on grammatical features of language. It must be noted conducted by a research assistant 12 weeks after the last
that one child dropped out after three therapy sessions therapy session. Importantly, RCs were not part of the
because of poor cognitive skills. Her non-verbal IQ ap- school curriculum for the duration of the intervention
peared to be 76 on retesting, even though it had been study.
within the normal range at the initial selection. Dur-
ing therapy, she was not able to make the abstraction
Materials
from words to Lego R
bricks. Consequently, 12 children
(eight boys, four girls) completed all measurements and Description and scoring of the RC tasks
all sessions of the intervention programme.
Standardized language tests could not be used to mea-
sure the progress of RC use, because these tests do not
Procedure contain enough items assessing RCs and are not sen-
sitive enough. Therefore, specific, dedicated tasks were
This study was reviewed and approved by the
constructed to assess the RC types that were targeted
Dutch Central Committee on Research Involving Hu-
in this intervention study. The six tasks are described in
man Subjects (Centrale Commissie Mensgebonden
detail below. Each example of the tasks starts with the in-
Onderzoek—CCMO). Informed written consent was
struction of the investigator (IN), followed by the target
obtained from the parents of all participating children.
response (TR) expected from the children, in italics.
To minimize the effects of various extra-experimental
variables (e.g. different speech therapists involved, ther-
apy settings, school curriculum differences and period Task 1: Right-branching OS RC completion (10
in the school year) we chose to confine our study to just items).In this elicited production task, adapted from
one school, one therapist and one period. This decision Novogrodsky and Friedmann (2006), the children were
limited the number of children that could participate, instructed to complete sentences after a verbal prompt
and consequently ruled out the inclusion of a control was presented, as illustrated in example (7) below. Test
group. We explored the possibility of a waiting-list con- items were adjusted to participant gender (example 7).
trol group, but this was not in accordance with the rules Responses were scored as correct when a complete sen-
of the ethics committee. tence containing an RC was produced, and the RC con-
Comprehensive manuals and protocols were con- tained a relative pronoun and an inflected verb in clause
structed for the assessments and therapy programme final position. All responses that did not meet these cri-
and they were followed strictly. A concurrent within- teria were classified as incorrect:
subjects multiple-baseline design was used, with three
baseline measurements (T1–T3) prior to the treatment (7) IN: Eén jongen (meisje) koopt een lolly en één
jongen koopt een ijsje.
and two measurements after treatment, i.e. T4 directly Welk(e) jongen (meisje) zou jij willen zijn?
after treatment and a retention measurement 12 weeks Begin je antwoord met: Ik kies de jongen
after the last therapy session (T5). (het meisje) . . . .
The assessment of six children at T1 was carried out TR: die (dat) de lolly koopt.
by the speech therapist who also delivered the interven- IN: One boy (girl) buys a lollipop and one boy (girl)
tion. Two research assistants tested the remaining seven buys an ice cream.
children at T1 and all children in the four subsequent Which boy (girl) would you like to be?
measurements. These assistants were experienced clini- Start your answer with: I choose the boy (the girl) . . . .
cal linguists who were not involved in either the school or TR: who the lollipop buys. (‘I choose the boy who buys
the intervention programme. The assistants scored the the lollipop’).
tasks and scores were checked and analysed afterwards
by the experimenter. All treatments were delivered by Task 2: Production of centre-embedded SS RCs (10
one and the same speech therapist and during the en- items).Because centre-embedded RCs are difficult to
tire testing and treatment period, this speech therapist elicit, this task contained test items with three writ-
remained blind to the test results. ten short sentences (example 8). From these three short
During intervention, the speech therapist, who was sentences the children had to construct a sentence with
also a clinical linguist and researcher, kept a logbook, an embedded SS RC. The sentences were first read aloud
which contained information on all treatment sessions together by the child and the research assistant, while
for all participating children. After each session, the the research assistant pointed at the words in synchrony
280 Rob Zwitserlood et al.
with reading them. The children were instructed to use (11) De clown, die ballonnen opblaast, ziet Marloes. (7
all bold printed words (verbs) and to not use the con- words, SS type)
junction ‘en’ (and). Responses were scored as correct The clown, who balloons blows up, sees Marloes. (The
when a complete complex sentence containing a centre- clown who blows up balloons, sees Marloes.)
embedded RC was produced. The RC had to contain
a relative pronoun and an inflected verb in clause final Task 6: RC comprehension (28 items).In this picture
position. All responses that did not meet these criteria selection task, sentences containing SS and OS RCs were
were classified as incorrect: presented to the participants. The children had to select
the correct picture from a set of four pictures. The images
(8) IN: Er is een chauffeur./ Hij draagt een hoed./ Hij were full colour photographs of scenes with Playmobil R

zit in de auto. figurines and props. Figure 1 shows one of the test items.
Begin je zin met: De chauffeur . . . . The accompanying test item of an embedded SS type
TR: die een hoed draagt, zit in de auto. RC is shown in example (12). Responses were scored as
IN: There is a driver./ He wears a hat./ He sits in the correct when the child selected the correct picture:
car.
Start your sentence with: The driver . . . (12) De jongen die naar de kinderen wijst, draagt een
TR: who a hat wears, sits in the car. (‘The driver who pet.
wears a hat, sits in the car’). The boy that to the children points, wears a cap.
(‘The boy who points to the children wears a cap’).
Task 3: Recreating OS and SS RCs (16 items).This task
For all tasks, two versions (A and B) were con-
was based on the subtest ‘Sentence Assembly’ from the
structed, which were alternated, in order to suppress
Dutch version of the CELF-4 (Semel et al. 2008). The
a possible learning effect from multiple presentations of
different parts of the sentences were visually presented
the tasks. The items in the A and B versions of each test
in frames in a scrambled and ungrammatical order (ex-
were carefully matched with regard to lexical content,
ample 9). The test items were read aloud by the child
word order and, where pictures were used, the order in
and IN. The children were invited to reconstruct the
which these were presented. For instance, the italicized
sentences and were instructed to start with the under-
nouns in a test sentence changed places (one boy plays
lined word(s), which also had a capital letter. Responses
the violin, the other boy plays the flute), or high fre-
were scored as correct when the children produced a
quency nouns and verbs differed between versions (boy
grammatical sentence containing an RC pattern:
becomes girl, or writes a letter becomes types a letter).
(9) IN: Begin je zin met: Stefanie . . . . In the comprehension task, either the order of the four
pictures differed between the A and B versions, or the
TR: die dorst heeft, drinkt water. word order in the test sentence was changed (The boy
IN: Start your sentence with: Stefanie . . . who points to the children wears a cap versus The children
TR: who thirst has, drinks water (‘Stefanie who is thirsty, who point to the boy wear a cap) (figure 1, example 12).
drinks water’). The tasks were always administered in the same
order; the three production tasks were presented first,
Tasks 4 and 5: Sentence repetition (short: 12 items; followed by the two sentence repetition tasks and the
long: 15 items).Two sets of sentences containing both comprehension task. All tasks contained two or three
OS- and SS-type RCs had to be repeated by the children. practice items depending on the difficulty of the task.
Working memory limitations are often seen in children
with SLI. In order to investigate the role of sentence Intervention programme
length in a repetition task, two different sets of sentences
with OS- and SS-type RCs were constructed. The first The children received a total of 10 individual ther-
set contained 12 seven-word sentences and the second apy sessions lasting 30 min each, twice a week during
set contained fifteen 12-word sentences. Examples (10) 5 weeks. All therapy sessions were protocolled in a ther-
and (11) show the two RC types presented in the seven- apy manual which was followed strictly by the speech
word condition. Responses were scored as correct when therapist who delivered the intervention. An example
an RC pattern was produced. The RC had to contain of a MetaTaal therapy session is given in appendix A.
a relative pronoun and an inflected verb in clause final First, the children were introduced to the concept that
position: words would be represented by Lego R
bricks in differ-
ent shapes, sizes and colours. The children also practised
(10) Hij ziet een vrouw die cake eet. (7 words, OS type) with exercises aimed at the identification of different
He sees a woman who cake eats. (He sees a woman who clause types and conjunctions in written and spoken
eats cake.) language. When the concepts and procedures of the
‘MetaTaal’ and SLI 281

Figure 1. Example of one test item from the relative clause comprehension task.

intervention programme were understood, the children and the main clause. An example of a centre-embedded
started building simple declarative sentences consisting (SS) type RC can be seen in figure 5. The ‘comma
of a subject, verb and object. Subsequently, more ele- bridge’ was created by stacking a small corner brick on
ments such as prepositional phrases, plural nouns and top of an arched brick. The children easily understand
subject–verb agreement were introduced. this concept, because in running speech it is natural
The next step was to build coordinated sentences to pause at that point in the sentence, while in writ-
by connecting two main clauses with an arched brick (a ten text a comma is often placed between two inflected
bridge). An example of a coordinated sentence built with verbs.
Lego R
bricks can be seen in figure 2. Next, the children The speech therapist delivering the intervention
were instructed that this sentence could be truncated by checked during sessions and retrospective at the start
deleting the subject of the second main clause. This op- of a new session, whether the child was ready for the
eration resulted in a sentence containing coordination next step in the programme, or that further rehearsal was
with reduction, as is illustrated in figure 3. Obviously, necessary. During the intervention sessions, the children
the terms ‘coordination’ and ‘subordination’ were not did not have to memorize the functions of the different
used with the children. Instead, these sentences were colours, bricks and bridges. They always had a crib sheet
called ‘bruggetjeszinnen’ (bridge sentences). When the with pictures of all the bricks and examples of their func-
children performed well on building coordinated sen- tions at hand (see appendix B). Instruction was mostly
tences, right-branching (OS) subject RCs were intro- oral; minor reading and writing activities were included
duced. As can be seen in figure 4, these RCs were built only when necessary, e.g. in exercises where the children
on the base plate on a lower level than the main clause. had to write the relative pronoun die or dat between
The bridge was used to connect the main clause with the printed sentences. While building sentences, the chil-
RC. The last step in the intervention programme was re- dren were free to add, move and remove bricks. Several
served for building centre-embedded (SS) subject RCs. sentences could be constructed on the base plate be-
Two bridges were used to express subordination and low one another, so that they could be easily compared.
centre-embedding. The first bridge connected the main Another option was to check for differences between
clause to the lower RC. A second bridge was needed to sentences built by the child and by the therapist. Con-
connect the RC to the second part of the main clause versations, short stories and pictures were used to elicit
again. This extra bridge was called a ‘comma bridge’, and RCs during therapy. Each therapy session also had a
was placed between the two inflected verbs of the RC game activity to consolidate the target structures. The
282 Rob Zwitserlood et al.

Figure 2. Coordinated sentences built with Lego


R
bricks.

Figure 3. Coordination with reduction sentence built with Lego


R
bricks.

Figure 4. Sentence built with Lego


R
bricks containing a right-branching OS type relative clause.

Figure 5. Sentence built with Lego


R
bricks containing a centre-embedded SS type relative clause.

children did not practice RCs in the classroom and no presented in table 2. Because the participant group
homework was given. was small and the data violated the assumptions for
general linear model (GLM) analyses on repeated
measures, non-parametric tests were used. Differences
Results between the five measurement points were analysed
The scores and significant differences between mea- with Friedman’s non-parametric analysis of variance
surements on the repeated baseline, post-therapy and (ANOVA), using the exact statistic. When Fried-
retention measurements of the six RC tasks are man’s tests reached significance, subsequent post-hoc
‘MetaTaal’ and SLI 283
Table 2. Means (standard deviations) of the raw scores and maximum score (MaxSc) on the relative clause tasks at the five
measurement points (T1–T5)

Pre-therapy
Post-therapy Retention
T1 T2 T3 T4 T5
RC tasks MaxSc Mean (SD)
Relative clause production
OS Completion 10 6.25a (2.05) 7.83a (2.17) 8.08a (1.51) 9.42a (0.90) 8.75a (1.14)
SS Composition 10 1.75a (3.25) 3.00a (3.86) 3.00a (4.00) 7.08b (4.23) 7.00b (4.20)
Recreation 16 6.50a (4.23) 8.00a (4.05) 9.25a (4.05) 11.42b (3.50) 12.33b (3.63)
Relative clause repetition
Repetition 7 word 12 11.42a (1.17) 10.92a (1.31) 11.25a (1.22) 11.00a (1.13) 11.50a (0.80)
Repetition 12 word 15 9.25a (3.72) 9.58a (4.85) 9.75a (3.96) 10.75ab (3.30) 11.83b (3.46)
Relative clause comprehension
Comprehension 28 19.25a (2.30) 18.25a (2.90) 21.17a (2.37) 20.17a (1.75) 21.67a (2.19)
Note: Mean scores in each row that share subscripts (a, b) do not differ significantly. All significant differences are p < 0.01.

Wilcoxon signed-rank tests were used to test p = 0.094, r = –0.039). In sum, on the OS Com-
differences between time points (exact statistic, two- pletion task no significant gains were found between
tailed). A Bonferroni correction was applied for all post- the measurements. Further inspection on the individual
hoc tests, as a result of which all effects are reported at a trajectories (see figure C1 in appendix C) revealed that
p = 0.008 level of significance (p = 0.05/6 compar- between T1 and T3 some children first showed some
isons). The effect size r was computed by dividing the improvement (at T2) and then decline (at T3), whereas
Z-score (of the scores of the particular tasks) by the others showed first decline (at T2) and then improve-
square root of the total observations (Field 2009). An ment (at T3). However, after T3 none of the children
effect size r from 0.10 to 0.30 represents a small effect, showed a decline: they either had the same or higher
from 0.30 to 0.50 a medium effect and beyond 0.50 a scores on T4 and T5 (as compared with T3).
large effect. With respect to the SS Composition task, Fried-
man’s ANOVA returned a significant difference between
measurements, χ 2 (4) = 31.40, p < 0.001. Post-hoc
RC production Wilcoxon tests indicated that no significant difference
On the three RC production tasks a clear pattern was was found between T1 (mdn = 0.00) and T2 (mdn =
seen with no pre-therapy progress between T1 and T2, 0.50) (Z = –1.68, p = 0.125, r = –0.34), between T2
T2 and T3, and T1 and T3. On two tasks, progress was (mdn = 0.50) and T3 (mdn = 0.00) (Z = 0.00, p =
found post-therapy between T3 and T4 and between T3 1.00, r = –0.00), and between T1 (mdn = 0.00) and
and T5 (table 2), which was also maintained at retention T3 (mdn = 0.00) (Z = –2.41, p = 0.014, r = –0.49).
measurement. Post-therapy, the difference was significant between T3
Regarding the OS Completion task, analysis with (mdn = 0.00) and T4 (mdn = 9.50) with Z = –2.83,
Friedman’s ANOVA showed that the scores of the chil- p = 0.002 and r = –0.58. The difference in scores be-
dren between baseline measurements, pre- and post- tween T4 (mdn = 9.50) and T5 (mdn = 9.50) was not
therapy tests and the final retention measurement did significant, with Z = –0.264, p = 0.984, r = –0.05. The
change significantly (χ 2 (4) = 20.72 and p < 0.001). difference between the last baseline test (T3) and the re-
However, post-hoc Wilcoxon tests indicated that no tention measurement (T5) was significant (Z = –2.67, p
significant difference was found between T1 (median = 0.004, r = –0.55). Thus, on the SS Composition task,
(mdn) = 6.50) and T2 (mdn = 8.50) (Z = –1.86, p = the children did not improve between baseline measure-
0.070, r = –0.38), between T2 (mdn = 8.50) and T3 ments, but post-therapy significant gains were found
(mdn = 8.00) (Z = –0.23, p = 0.855, r = –0.05), and that were maintained at retention measurement. Fur-
between T1 (mdn = 6.50) and T3 (mdn = 8.00) (Z = ther inspection on the individual trajectories (see figure
–2.41, p = 0.014, r = –0.49). Post-therapy the differ- C2 in appendix C) revealed a rather diffuse pattern of
ence was not significant between T3 (mdn = 8.00) and scores between T1 and T3. However, between T3 and
T4 (mdn = 10.00) (Z = –2.41, p = 0.016, r = –0.49) T4, all but one of the children made progress on this
and between T4 (mdn = 10.00) and T5 (mdn = 9.00) task and for most children, this progress was maintained
(Z = –1.93, p = 0.094, r = –0.39). The difference at T5.
between the last baseline test (T3) and the retention On the RC Recreation task, Friedman’s ANOVA
measurement (T5) was also not significant (Z = –1.93, yielded significant differences over time, χ 2 (4) = 34.40,
284 Rob Zwitserlood et al.
p < 0.001. Post-hoc Wilcoxon tests indicated that no On the second Repetition task (12-word sen-
significant difference was found between T1 (mdn = tences), Friedman’s ANOVA yielded significant dif-
7.00) and T2 (mdn = 7.50) (Z = –1.58, p = 0.134, ferences, χ 2 (4) = 17.51, p = 0.001. Post-hoc
r = –0.32), between T2 (mdn = 7.50) and T3 Wilcoxon tests indicated that no significant differ-
(mdn = 10.00) (Z = –1.36, p = 0.203, r = –0.27), ence was found between T1 (mdn = 11.00) and T2
and between T1 (mdn = 7.00) and T3 (mdn = 10.00) (mdn = 12.00) (Z = –0.54, p = 0.652, r = –0.11),
(Z = –2.32, p = 0.018, r = –0.47). Post-therapy, the between T2 (mdn = 12.00) and T3 (mdn = 11.00)
difference was significant between T3 (mdn = 10.00) (Z = –0.30, p = 0.848, r = –0.06), and between T1
and T4 (mdn = 11.50) with Z = –2.67, p = 0.006, r = (mdn = 11.00) and T3 (mdn = 11.00) (Z = –1.00, p =
–0.54. The difference in scores directly post-therapy 0.359, r = –0.20). Post-therapy, the difference was not
(T4, mdn = 11.50) and the retention measure- significant between T3 (mdn = 11.00) and T4 (mdn =
ment (T5, mdn = 12.00) was not significant 10.50) (Z = –2.16, p = 0.047, r = –0.44), and between
(Z = –1.72, p = 0.113, r = –0.35). The dif- T4 (mdn = 10.50) and T5 (mdn = 13.00) (Z = –2.14,
ference between the last baseline test (T3) and p = 0.043, r = –0.44). However, the difference between
the retention measurement (T5) was significant the last baseline test (T3) and the retention measurement
(Z = –2.96 p = 0.001, r = –0.60). Thus, on the Recre- (T5) was significant (Z = –2.58, p = 0.008, r = –0.53).
ation task, the children did not improve between baseline Inspection of the scores per child (see figure C5 in ap-
measurements, but gains were significant post-therapy pendix C) revealed that most of the individual growth
and were maintained 12 weeks later. A closer look at the trajectories were relatively flat, but that a trend of limited
individual trajectories (see figure C3 in appendix C) re- progress over all measurements could be seen.
vealed that the children scored relatively stable between
T1 and T3. Furthermore, progress between T3 and T4
RC comprehension
was small per child, but nonetheless significant.
The RC comprehension tasks showed that no differ-
ences were found between pairs of measurements. Fried-
RC repetition
man’s ANOVA showed a significant difference over time,
Two sentence repetition tasks were used. In the first sen- χ 2 (4) = 15.16, p = 0.002. Post-hoc Wilcoxon tests re-
tence repetition task, sentences containing seven words turned no significant difference between T1 (mdn =
were used. On this task, no differences were found be- 19.50) and T2 (mdn = 19.00) (Z = –1.28, p = 0.236,
tween pairs of measurements. Results of the second task r = –0.26), between T2 (mdn = 19.00) and T3 (mdn =
containing sentences with 12 words, showed a more 21.00) (Z = –2.32, p = 0.018, r = –0.47), and between
complicated pattern. T1 (mdn = 19.50) and T3 (mdn = 21.00) (Z = –1.56,
On the first Repetition task (seven-word sentences), p = 0.124, r = –0.32). Post therapy, no significant dif-
analysis with Friedman’s ANOVA showed that the chil- ferences were found between T3 (mdn = 21.00) and
dren obtained significantly different scores over time, T4 (mdn = 20.50) (Z = –1.01, p = 0.355, r = –0.21),
χ 2 (4) = 13.08, p = 0.006. Post-hoc Wilcoxon tests between T4 (mdn = 20.50) and T5 (mdn = 22.00)
yielded no significant difference between T1 (mdn = (Z = –1.86, p = 0.067, r = –0.38), and between the last
12.00) and T2 (mdn = 11.50) (Z = –1.51, p = 0.250, baseline test (T3) and the retention measurement (T5)
and r = –0.31), between T2 (mdn = 11.50) and T3 (Z = –0.71, p = 0.518, r = –0.14). Finally, a closer
(mdn = 12.00) (Z = –2.00, p = 0.125, r = –0.41), and look at the individual scores on each measurement (see
between T1 (mdn = 12.00) and T3 (mdn = 12.00) (Z = Figure C6 in Appendix C) showed that between T1 and
–0.82, p = 0.750, r = –0.17). Post-therapy, the T2 children did not progress, but that between T2 and
difference was not significant between T3 (mdn = T3 progress was made by most of the children, followed
12.00) and T4 (mdn = 11.00) (Z = –1.73, p = 0.250, by a decrease in scores between T3 and T4. However,
r = –0.35). The difference between post-therapy scores these trends were not significant.
(T4, mdn = 11.00) and retention measurement (T5,
mdn = 12.00) was also not significant, with Z = –2.45,
Discussion
p = 0.031, r = –0.50. The difference between the last
baseline test (T3) and the retention measurement (T5) The purpose of this study was to examine the efficacy
was not significant (Z = –1.73, p = 0.250, r = –0.35). of the metalinguistic ‘MetaTaal’ approach for school-
At further inspection on the individual scores (see figure age children with SLI. This intervention study fol-
C4 in appendix C) five out of 12 children appeared to lowed a repeated-baseline design and targeted complex
have extreme ceiling effects with maximum scores at all sentences comprising OS- and SS-type subject-RCs.
measurements and the rest of the children also showed RCs were selected as target since school-age children
ceiling effects. with SLI are known to have persistent difficulties with
‘MetaTaal’ and SLI 285
these types of complex structures. In order to be effec- would have an effect on comprehension. Two possible
tive, we expected that children’s performance on tests explanations for not finding such an improvement come
assessing their ability to produce and comprehend sen- to mind. First, the comprehension task may have been
tences comprising RCs would improve significantly too complex for the children. The children had to re-
immediately after the intervention, and that this im- tain complex sentences with RCs in working memory
provement would be maintained 12 weeks after the in- and compare these sentences to four different pictures.
tervention had been completed. Moreover, the sentences in the comprehension task were
In order to evaluate the effectiveness of the interven- also longer than those used in the production tasks.
tion, we first examined whether spontaneous growth in Second, it may have been the case that 5 h of therapy
RC production and comprehension occurred prior to focused at RC production was not enough to cause sig-
the onset of therapy. Therefore, three baseline measure- nificant progress on the comprehension of RCs. Our
ments were executed pre-therapy once a month. The result contrasts with Camarata et al. (2009), who found
results showed that the children did not improve on any that children with SLI made significant gains in language
of the RC tasks during baseline measurements. How- comprehension when exposed to language intervention
ever, significant gains were found on two of the three that was focused on production. However, in Camarata
production tests (i.e. the SS Composition and the Recre- et al. (2009), intervention amounted to 24 h in 12 weeks,
ation tasks) directly post-therapy (T4). These gains were which is almost five times the amount of intervention
also maintained at the retention measurement 12 weeks provided in our study.
later (T5) when scores on T3 and T5 were compared. Another methodological issue concerns the use of
On the 12-word Sentence repetition task, a significant two separate versions of the five RC tasks. In the mate-
improvement was found between scores on T3 and T5. rials section, we explained that A and B versions were
However, on the other three tasks (i.e. OS Comple- administered in order to minimize possible learning ef-
tion, seven-word Sentence repetition, and Comprehen- fects and that both versions were only slightly different.
sion tasks) no significant differences were found. These We did not validate the tasks and did not compare both
results partly confirm the efficacy of the ‘MetaTaal’ pro- versions beforehand. However, the two versions can be
gramme. Significant improvement was observed for RC compared post hoc, because the A version was admin-
production, but not for comprehension. In the follow- istered at T1, T3, and T5 and the B version at T2 and
ing paragraphs, these differences in results and some T4. No significant differences were found for all five
methodological issues will be discussed. tasks between time points T1 (A) and T2 (B), as well
The first issue that needs to be addressed is whether as between T4 (B) and T5 (A). Therefore we assume
learning effects due to the repeated administration of that the two measures can be considered parallel task
the tasks could have contributed to the observed gains. versions. The comparison of the two versions between
We suppose that multiple exposure to sentence repeti- T3 (A) and T4 (B) cannot be made, because these time
tion tasks and a picture selection task probably would points were the pre- and post-therapy measurements.
not lead to learning effects. Both tasks are relatively sim- Furthermore, it is important to have a closer look at
ple and not much learning can be expected to occur the performance of the children on the two sentence rep-
from repeating sentences and picture pointing. How- etition tasks. The answers of the children were scored
ever, the production tasks were more complex and repe- as correct when they fulfilled the minimum require-
tition of these tasks may have produced a learning effect. ments (i.e. realization of a relative pronoun and correct
Nonetheless, in none of these tasks significant changes verb placement in the RC). However, the children made
were found during baseline, although upward trends many omission and substitution errors, even though
were observed (see appendix C). This trend was most they mostly succeeded in correctly conveying the mean-
apparent for the OS Completion task. For the SS Com- ing of the sentences. For instance, a phrase such as ‘the
position and the Recreation task more variation in score clown who has a balloon’ was changed into ‘the clown
patterns was seen (see appendix C). Some children did with a balloon’. Apparently, the children were able to
improve during baseline measurements, but in other process the meaning of the sentences correctly, but failed
children scores fell off, which makes the presence of an to reconstruct the grammatical structure. This finding is
overall learning effect less obvious. also in line with the very poor results of the children on
Next, the lack of improvement on the Comprehen- the CELF-4-NL subtest Recalling Sentences (see table
sion task needs to be considered. On this task, scores 1). Moreover, on the 12-word repetition task, the chil-
remained well below ceiling in all measurements. Al- dren improved between T3 and T5. Although no effect
though RC production was the main component of the was seen directly post-therapy, the children scored sig-
intervention, it is conceivable that a metalinguistic ap- nificantly higher on T5 (retention) than on pre-therapy
proach aimed at explicit learning of linguistic knowledge measures after a period of 12 weeks with no further
286 Rob Zwitserlood et al.
training. This result suggests that the metalinguistic To conclude, the results of this intervention pro-
training was effective, because a learning effect can vide a preliminary indication that a relatively short met-
probably be ruled out for assessments with a sentence alinguistic training period of 5 weeks, with a total of
repetition task. This finding also illustrates the impor- 5 h of treatment, can result in a significant improve-
tance of administering a retention measurement. The ment of RC production in school-age children with SLI.
fact that no short-term effects were observed directly The medium to large effect sizes of the post-hoc com-
post-therapy does not exclude the possibility of finding parisons between pre- and post-therapy and retention
effects after a longer time span. For now, it remains un- measurements hold a promise as regards the clinical rel-
clear why such an effect surfaces 12 weeks post-therapy. evance of this study. It is still unclear to what extent the
Sentence repetition tasks have proven their diagnostic effects of the intervention can be generalized to natu-
value in identifying language disorders (Hesketh et al. ralistic situations. We have no direct evidence that the
2012, Seeff-Gabriel et al. 2010). Perhaps sentence repe- children started using more RCs in their spontaneous
tition tasks may prove to be valuable instruments in the speech.
evaluation of interventions. In our study, scoring was Of course, several limitations apply to the present
very tolerant, as the answer was scored as correct when study. To begin with, the sample size in our study was
children would realize a relative pronoun and a clause- rather small. A follow-up study with larger groups and a
final inflected verb in the RC. A more strict scoring more rigorous design could be informative. Further-
system, taking other (grammatical or semantic) errors more, we tried to take working memory limitations
into account, such as the SASIT-E32 (Marinis et al. into consideration by constructing two tasks that con-
2011) might have been more revealing. tained relatively short sentences (the OS Completion
The results of the present study have some impli- and seven-word Sentence repetition tasks). However,
cations for theoretical frameworks of SLI. The finding these tasks caused some children to perform at ceiling
that explicit teaching of grammatical rules to school-age already at baseline measurements. Therefore, it might
children with SLI yields positive results appears to lend have been more revealing to use tasks containing items
support to the Procedural Deficit Hypotheses (PDH) with an increasing sentence length. In follow-up studies
(Ullman and Pierpont 2005). The PDH claims that chil- it would be advisable to assess RC production and com-
dren with SLI are impaired at grammatical rule learning prehension in the children with SLI beforehand. Only
because of a deficit in their procedural memory system. the children performing poorly at such an assessment
Instead, they rely on their declarative memory system should consequently be included in the intervention
to compensate for their procedural deficit. Apparently, study. Furthermore, the present study showed that sen-
children with SLI are able to learn grammatical rules tences with centre-embedded RCs are difficult to elicit.
when they are taught explicitly (in declarative memory) The addition of a grammatical judgment task, tapping
and learning is enhanced by visual (or multimodal) sup- into the children’s grammatical knowledge of RCs could
port. Furthermore, when we consider the present results provide a more comprehensive picture of the effective-
in the light of grammatical competence versus perfor- ness of the intervention. Moreover, the collection and
mance, it is apparent that their grammatical competence analysis of pre- and post-therapy language samples or
is not deficient, as children with SLI are able to learn such classroom observations might be informative to eval-
complex grammatical syntactic constructions, although uate transfer to therapy-external settings. Finally, the
not as easy and early as typically developing children. assessment of metalinguistic skills pre- and post-therapy
An important question is what exactly constitutes could be included in a future study in order to evaluate
the active ingredient of the ‘MetaTaal’ programme. It such skills as a possible mediator of language gains in an
is possible that the metalinguistic approach with mul- explicit metalinguistic intervention.
timodal support produced the observed progress, but For now, the ‘MetaTaal’ approach holds a promise
it could also have been just the extensive exposure or for clinical practice. Apart from the positive effects it
practice with RCs (Sarilar et al. 2013). Furthermore, a appears to have on children’s verbal performance, the
point that is not often discussed in studies on metalin- approach has a number of practical advantages. For
guistic intervention is that, although children with SLI one, the children thoroughly enjoyed working with
appear to have poorer metalinguistic skills than typically the material. Some children wanted to use the bricks
developing children (Kamhi and Koenig 1985, Menyuk in the classroom and asked why they had not started
1993), these skills can in fact be employed successfully using them much earlier on. Another advantage is that,
to remediate grammatical problems in a metalinguistic although it is certainly possible to use written language
approach. It is possible that the metalinguistic interven- together with the Lego R
bricks, this is not necessary.
tion also enhanced metalinguistic skills in children with Consequently, the children with SLI who are also poor
SLI and has been a mediating factor in the children’s readers can work with the material as well in the spoken
language gains. modality.
‘MetaTaal’ and SLI 287
Acknowledgements impairment: the long term follow-up of an Italian child. In-
ternational Journal of Language and Communication Disorders,
The authors would like to thank the staff and speech therapists of 33, 245–80.
Royal Auris Group ‘De Taalkring’, especially Lieke Kuipers. They CLARK, J. M. and PAIVIO, A., 1991, Dual coding theory and educa-
also express their gratitude to the children and parents who par- tion. Educational Psychology Review, 3, 149–210.
ticipated in this study; and to Liset Bergevoet, Hanneke Creemers, CLEGG, J., HOLLIS, C., MAWHOOD, L. and RUTTER, M., 2005,
Jonna Genuit and Nadia Lemouesset for help with the ‘MetaTaal’ Developmental language disorders a follow-up in later
tests and therapy programme. Declaration of interest: The authors adult life. Cognitive, language and psychosocial outcomes.
report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 128–149.
the content and writing of the paper. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00342.x
CONTI-RAMSDEN, G. and BOTTING, N., 2004, Social difficulties and
victimization in children with SLI at 11 years of age. Journal
References of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 47, 145–161.
CONTI-RAMSDEN, G. and DURKIN, K., 2008, Language and inde-
ADAMS, C., 1990, Syntactic comprehension in children with ex- pendence in adolescents with and without a history of spe-
pressive language impairment. British Journal of Disorders of cific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Speech–Language
Communication, 25, 149–171. and Hearing Research, 51, 70–83. doi:10.1044/1092-
ARAM, D. M., EKELMAN, B. L. and NATION, J. E., 1984, Pre- 4388(2008/005)
schoolers with language disorders: 10 years later. Journal of CONTI-RAMSDEN, G., DURKIN, K., SIMKIN, Z. and KNOX, E., 2009,
Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 232–244. Specific language impairment and school outcomes. I: Identi-
BEITCHMAN, J. H., WILSON, B., BROWNLIE, E. B., WALTERS, H. and fying and explaining variability at the end of compulsory edu-
LANCEE, W., 1996, Long-term consistency in speech/language cation. International Journal of Language and Communication
profiles: 1. Developmental and academic outcomes. Journal Disorders, 44, 15–35. doi:10.1080/13682820801921601
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, DEEVY, P. and LEONARD, L. B., 2004, The comprehension of Wh-
35, 804–814. questions in children with specific language impairment.
BIRSH, J. R., 2005, Multisensory Teaching of Basic Language Skills, Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 17, 802–
2nd edn (Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes). 815.
BISHOP, D. V. M., 1992, The underlying nature of specific language DOCKRELL, J. E., LINDSAY, G., LETCHFORD, B. and MACKIE, C.,
impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 2006, Educational provision for children with specific speech
3–66. and language difficulties: perspectives of speech and language
BISHOP, D. V. M., 1994, Grammatical errors in specific language im- therapist managers. International Journal of Language and
pairment: Competence or performance limitations? Applied Communication Disorders, 41, 423–440.
Psycholinguistics, 15, 507–550. EBBELS, S. H., 2007, Teaching grammar to school-aged children
BISHOP, D. V. M., 2003, The Test for Reception of Grammar—TROG- with specific language impairment using Shape Coding. Child
2 (London: Psychological Corporation). Language Teaching and Therapy, 23, 67–93.
BISHOP, D. V. M., 2008, State-of-Science Review: SR-D1 Specific EBBELS, S., 2014, Effectiveness of intervention for grammar in
Language Impairment (Government Office for Science), pp. school-aged children with primary language impairments: a
1–10 (available at: http://www.foresight.gov.uk). review of the evidence. Child Language Teaching and Therapy,
BISHOP, D. V. M., 2009, Specific language impairment. In C. L. 30, 7–40. doi:10.1177/0265659013512321
Cooper, J. Field, U. Goswami, R. Jenkins and B. J. Sahakian EBBELS, S. H. and VAN DER LELY, H. K. J., 2001, Meta-syntactic
(eds), Mental Capital and Wellbeing (Malden, MA: Wiley- therapy using visual coding for children with severe persistent
Blackwell), pp. 767–775. SLI. International Journal of Language and Communication
BISHOP, D. V. M., BRIGHT, P., JAMES, C., BISHOP, S. J. and VAN DER Disorders, 36(Suppl.), 345–350.
LELY, H. K. J. 2000, Grammatical SLI: a distinct subtype of EBBELS, S. H., MARIC, N., MURPHY, A. and TURNER, G., 2014,
developmental language impairment? Applied Psycholinguis- Improving comprehension in adolescents with severe recep-
tics, 21, 159–181. tive language impairments: a randomized control trial of in-
BOLDERSON, S., DOSANJH, C., MILLIGAN, C., PRING, T. and CHIAT, tervention for coordinating conjunctions. International Jour-
S., 2011, Colourful semantics: a clinical investigation. Child nal of Language and Communication Disorders, 49, 30–48.
Language Teaching and Therapy, 27, 344–353. doi:10.1111/1460-6984.12047
BRANDT, S., KIDD, E., LIEVEN, E. and TOMASELLO, M., 2009, The EBBELS, S. H., VAN DER LELY, H. K. J. and DOCKRELL, J. E., 2007,
discourse bases of relativization: an investigation of young Intervention for verb argument structure in children with
German and English-speaking children’s comprehension of persistent SLI: a randomized control trial. Journal of Speech,
relative clauses. Cognitive Linguistics, 20, 539–570. Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 1330–1349.
BRYAN, A., 1997, Colourful semantics. In S. Chiat, J. Law and J. ELLIS WEISMER, S. and EVANS, J., 2002, The role of processing limi-
Marshall (eds), Language Disorders in Children and Adults: tations in early identification of specific language impairment.
Psycholinguistic Approaches to Therapy (London: Whurr), pp. Topics in Language Disorders, 22, 15–29.
143–161. FEY, M. E. and FINESTACK, L. H., 2009, Research and development
CAMARATA, S., NELSON, K. E., GILLUM, H. and CAMARATA, M., in children’s language intervention: a 5-phase model. In R.
2009, Incidental receptive language growth associated with Schwartz (ed.), Handbook of Child Language Disorders (New
expressive grammar intervention in SLI. First Language, 29, York, NY: Psychology Press), pp. 513–532.
51–63. FEY, M. E. and PROCTOR-WILLIAMS, K., 2000, Recasting, elicited
CARR, J. E., 2005, Recommendations for reporting multiple-baseline imitation and modelling in grammar intervention for children
designs across participants. Behavioral Interventions, 20, 219– with specific language impairment. In D. V. M. Bishop and
224. L. B. Leonard (eds), Speech and Language Impairments in
CIPRIANI, P., BOTTARI, P., CHILOSI, A. M. and PFANNER, L., 1998, Children: Causes, Characteristics, Intervention and Outcome
A longitudinal perspective on the study of specific language (Hove: Psychology Press), pp. 177–194.
288 Rob Zwitserlood et al.
FIELD, A., 2009, Discovering Statistics Using SPSS, 3rd edn (London: KAMHI, A. G. and KOENIG, L. A., 1985, Metalinguistic awareness in
Sage). normal and language-disordered children. Language, Speech,
FINNERAN, D. A., FRANCIS, A. L. and LEONARD, L. B., 2009, Sus- and Hearing Services in Schools, 16, 199–210.
tained attention in children with specific language impair- KULKARNI, A., PRING, T. and EBBELS, S., 2013, Evaluating the ef-
ment (SLI). Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, fectiveness of therapy based around Shape Coding to develop
52, 915–929. the use of regular past tense morphemes in two children with
FORTGENS, C. A., 2009, Auris-Taalbeleid [Language Policy of Auris] language impairments. Child Language Teaching and Therapy,
(Gouda: Koninklijke Auris Groep). 30, 245–254. doi:10.1177/0265659013514982
FRIEDMANN, N. and NOVOGRODSKY, R., 2004, The acquisition of LAW, J., BOYLE, J., HARRIS, F., HARKNESS, A. and NYE, C., 2000,
relative clause comprehension in Hebrew: a study of SLI and Prevalence and natural history of primary speech and language
normal development. Journal of Child Language, 31, 661– delay: findings from a systematic review of the literature. In-
681. doi:10.1017/S0305000904006269 ternational Journal of Language and Communication Disorders,
GERRITS, E., 2011, Spreken we dezelfde taal? [Do We Speak the Same 35, 165–188.
Language?] (Utrecht: Kenniscentrum Innovatie van Zorgver- LAW, J., GARRETT, Z. and NYE, C., 2003, Speech and
lening). language therapy interventions for children with pri-
GIBSON, E., 1998, Linguistic complexity: locality of syntactic de- mary speech and language delay or disorder. Cochrane
pendencies. Cognition, 68, 1–76. Database of Systematic Reviews (Online), CD004110.
GILLON, G. T., 2000, The efficacy of phonological awareness in- doi:10.1002/14651858.CD004110
tervention for children with spoken language impairment. LAW, J., GARRETT, Z. and NYE, C., 2004, The efficacy of treat-
Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 31, 126– ment for children with developmental speech and language
141. delay/disorder: a meta-analysis. Journal of Speech, Language
GOODLUCK, H. and TAVAKOLIAN, S., 1982, Competence and pro- and Hearing Research, 47, 924–943.
cessing in children’s grammar of relative clauses. Cognition, LEONARD, L. B., 1998, Children with Specific Language Impairment
11, 1–27. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
GUENDOUZI, J., 2003, ‘SLI’, a generic category of language im- LEONARD, L. B., WEISMER, S. E., MILLER, C. A., FRANCIS, D. J.,
pairment that emerges from specific differences: a case study TOMBLIN, J. B. and KAIL, R. V., 2007, Speed of process-
of two individual linguistic profiles. Clinical Linguistics and ing, working memory, and language impairment in children.
Phonetics, 17, 135–52. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 408–
HÅKANSSON, G. and HANSSON, K., 2000, Comprehension and pro- 428.
duction of relative clauses: a comparison between Swedish LEVY, H. and FRIEDMANN, N., 2009, Treatment of syntactic move-
impaired and unimpaired children. Journal of Child Language, ment in syntactic SLI: a case study. First Language, 29, 15–49.
27, 313–333. LINDSAY, G., DOCKRELL, J. E., MACKIE, C. and LETCHFORD, B.,
HARRIS, F. N. and JENSON, W. R., 1985, Comparisons of multiple- 2005, Local educational authorities’ approaches to provision
baseline across persons designs and AB designs with repli- for children with specific speech and language difficulties in
cation: issues and confusions. Behavioral Assessment, 7, 121– England and Wales. European Journal of Special Needs Educa-
127. tion, 20, 329–345.
HESKETH, A., RICHES, N. and VANCE, M., 2012, Sentence repeti- LINDSAY, G., SOLOFF, N., LAW, J., BAND, S., PEACEY, N., GAS-
tion in children with communication impairment. Thematic COIGNE, M. and RADFORD, J., 2002, Speech and language
panel presented at the 14th Meeting of the International Clin- therapy services to education in England and Wales. Interna-
ical Linguistics and Phonetics Association, Cork, Ireland, June tional Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 37,
2012. 273–288.
HESTVIK, A., SCHWARTZ, R. G. and TORNYOVA, L., 2010, Relative LUM, J., CONTI-RAMSDEN, G., PAGE, D. and ULLMAN, M., 2012,
clause gap-filling in children with specific language impair- Working, declarative and procedural memory in specific lan-
ment. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 39, 443–56. guage impairment. Cortex, 48, 1091–1250.
HIRSCHMAN, M., 2000, Language repair via metalinguistic means. MARINIS, T., CHIAT, S., ARMON-LOTEM, S., PIPER, J. and ROY,
International Journal of Language and Communication Disor- P., 2011, School-Age Sentence Imitation Test—English 32
ders, 35, 251–268. (SASIT-E32). Unpublished test.
IM-BOLTER, N., JOHNSON, J. and PASCUAL-LEONE, J., 2006, Process- MCARTHUR, G. M., HOGBEN, J. H., EDWARDS, V. T., HEATH, S.
ing limitations in children with specific language impairment: M. and MENGLER, E. D., 2000, On the ‘specifics’ of specific
the role of executive function. Child Development, 77, 1822– reading disability and specific language impairment. Journal
1841. of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 869–874.
JENSEN DE LÓPEZ, K., SUNDAHL OLSEN, L. and CHONDRO- MENYUK, P., 1993, Metalinguistic difficulties in children with
GIANNI, V., 2014, Annoying Danish relatives: comprehen- specific language impairment: implications for diagnosis
sion and production of relative clauses by Danish children and intervention. In H. Grimm and H. Skowronek (eds),
with and without SLI. Journal of Child Language, 41, 51–83. Language Acquisition Problems and Reading Disorders: As-
doi:10.1017/S0305000912000517 pects of diagnosis and intervention (Berlin: De Gruyter), pp.
JOHNSON, C. J., BEITCHMAN, J. H., YOUNG, A., ESCOBAR, M., 3–23.
ATKINSON, L., WILSON, B., BROWNLIE, E. B., DOUGLAS, MENYUK, P. and CHESNICK, M., 1997, Metalinguistic skills, oral lan-
L., TABACK, N., LAM, I., WANG, M., 1999, Fourteen-year guage knowledge, and reading. Topics in Language Disorders,
follow-up of children with and without speech language im- 17, 75–87.
pairments: speech language stability and outcomes. Journal of MONTESSORI, M., 1912, The Montessori Method (New York, NY:
Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 42, 744–760. Frederick A. Stokes).
JUST, M. A. and CARPENTER, P. A., 1992, A capacity theory MONTESSORI, M., 1949, The Absorbent Mind (Madras: Theosophical
of comprehension: individual differences in working mem- Publ. House).
ory. Psychological Review, 99, 122–149. doi:10.1037/0033- MONTGOMERY, J. W., 2000, Verbal working memory and sen-
295X.99.1.122 tence comprehension in children with specific language
‘MetaTaal’ and SLI 289
impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Re- STOTHARD, S. E., SNOWLING, M., BISHOP, D. V. M., CHIPCHASE, B.
search, 43, 293–308. B. and KAPLAN, C. A., 1998, Language-impaired preschool-
MONTGOMERY, J. W., 2003, Working memory and comprehension ers: a follow-up into adolescence, Journal of Speech, Language,
in children with specific language impairment: what we know and Hearing Research, 41, 407–18.
so far. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 221–231. TANNOCK, R., PURVIS, K. L. and SCHACHAR, R. J., 1993, Narrative
NORBURY, C. F. and BISHOP, D. V. M., 2003, Narrative skills of chil- abilities in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disor-
dren with communication impairments. International Journal ders and normal peers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,
of Language and Communication Disorders, 38, 287–313. 21, 103–116.
NOVOGRODSKY, R. and FRIEDMANN, N., 2006, The production of TOMBLIN, J. B., RECORDS, N. L., BUCKWALTER, P., ZHANG, X.,
relative clauses in syntactic SLI: a window to the nature of the SMITH, E. and O’BRIEN, M., 1997, Prevalence of specific lan-
impairment. International Journal of Speech–Language Pathol- guage impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech,
ogy, 8, 364–375. Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 1245–1260.
QUAIL, M., WILLIAMS, C. and LEITÃO, S., 2009, Verbal working ULLMAN, M. T. and PIERPONT, E. I., 2005, Specific language
memory in specific language impairment: the effect of provid- impairment is not specific to language: the procedural
ing visual support. International Journal of Speech–Language deficit hypothesis. Cortex, 41, 399–433. doi:10.1016/S0010-
Pathology, 11, 220–233. 9452(08)70276-4
ROBERTSON, S. B. and ELLIS WEISMER, S., 1999, Effects of treat- VAN DER LELY, H. K. J., 1997, Language and cognitive development
ment on linguistic and social skills in toddlers with delayed in a grammatical SLI boy: modularity and innateness. Journal
language development. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hear- of Neurolinguistics, 10, 75–107.
ing Research, 42, 1234–1248. VAN DER LELY, H., ROSEN, S. and MCCLELLAND, A., 1998, Evidence
ROSEN, S., ADLARD, A. and VAN DER LELY, H. K. J., 2009, Back- for a grammar-specific deficit in children. Current Biology, 8,
ward and simultaneous masking in children with grammatical 1253–1258.
specific language impairment: no simple link between audi- VAN DER LELY, H. K. J. and STOLLWERCK, L., 1997, Binding theory
tory and language abilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and and grammatical specific language impairment in children.
Hearing Research, 52, 396–411. Cognition, 62, 245–290.
SANKEY, M., BIRCH, D. and GARDINER, M., 2010, Engaging stu- VAN GEEL, C. A. A., 1973, Terreinverkenning van de therapie bij
dents through multimodal learning environments: the jour- vertraagde spraak- en taalontwikkeling. Toepassing van ‘Gram-
ney continues. In C. H. Steel, M. J. Keppell, P. Gerbic matica in vorm en kleur’ [Exploration of Therapy for Speech
and S. Housego (eds), Curriculum, Technology and Trans- and Language Delay. Application of ‘Grammar in Form and
formation for an Unknown Future. Proceedings of Ascilite Colour’] (Hoensbroek: Studiecentrum Hoensbroeck, Oplei-
Sydney 2010, pp. 852–863 (available at: http://ascilite. ding Logopedie).
org.au/conferences/sydney10/procs/Sankey-full.pdf). VAN KLEECK, A., 1982, The emergence of linguistic awareness: a
SARILAR, A., MATTHEWS, D. and KUNTAY } , A. C., 2013, Hearing rela- cognitive framework. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28, 237–265.
tive clauses boosts relative clause usage (and referential clarity) VAN WEERDENBURG, M., VERHOEVEN, L., BOSMAN, A.
in young Turkish language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, and VAN BALKOM, H. 2011, Predicting word decod-
available on CJO2013. doi:10.1017/S0142716413000192. ing and word spelling development in children with
SCHUELE, C. and NICHOLLS, L., 2000, Relative clauses: evidence specific language impairment. Journal of Communica-
of continued linguistic vulnerability in children with specific tion Disorders, 44, 392–411. doi:S0021-9924(10)00108-
language impairment. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 14, 5[pii]10.1016/j.jcomdis.2010.12.002
563–585. VAN WEERDENBURG, M., VERHOEVEN, L. and VAN BALKOM, H.,
SEEFF-GABRIEL, B., CHIAT, S. and DODD, B., 2010, Sentence im- 2006, Towards a typology of specific language impairment.
itation as a tool in identifying expressive morphosyntactic Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disci-
difficulties in children with severe speech difficulties. Interna- plines, 47, 176–189.
tional Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 45, WAKE, M., TOBIN, S., GIROLAMETTO, L., UKOUMUNNE, O. C.,
691–702. GOLD, L., LEVICKIS, P., SHEEHAN, J. GOLDFELD, S., and
SEMEL, E., WIIG, E. H. and SECORD, W. A., 2008, CELF-4. Clini- REILLY, S., 2011, Outcomes of population based language
cal Evaluation of Language Fundamentals. Nederlandse versie, promotion for slow to talk toddlers at ages 2 and 3 years:
Dutch version (Amsterdam: Pearson). Let’s Learn Language cluster randomised controlled trial.
SHAMS, L. and SEITZ, A. R., 2008, Benefits of multisensory learning. British Medical Journal, 343(4741), 1–10. doi:10.1136/bmj.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 411–417. d4741
SIMKIN, Z. and CONTI-RAMSDEN, G., 2006, Evidence of reading dif- WILLEMSEN, M., 2011, Logopedisten lezen niet. De rol van de lo-
ficulty in subgroups of children with specific language impair- gopedist in het aanvankelijk technisch lezen bij leerlingen
ment. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 22, 315–331. met ernstige spraak-/taalstoornissen (ESM) [Speech thera-
SPOONER, L., 2002, Addressing expressive language disorder in chil- pists don’t read. The role of the speech therapist in the read-
dren who also have severe receptive language disorder: a psy- ing process of children with SLI], Logopedie en Foniatrie, 10,
cholinguistic approach. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 292–298.
18, 289–313. ZWITSERLOOD, R., 2014, Language growth in Dutch school-age
STAVRAKAKI, S., 2001, Comprehension of reversible relative clauses children with specific language impairment. Utrecht Uni-
in specifically language impaired and normally developing versity, Utrecht LOT Dissertation Series 356 (available at:
Greek children. Brain and Language, 77, 419–431. http://dspace.library.uu.nl/handle/1874/293862).
290 Rob Zwitserlood et al.
Appendix A Build the sentence together and introduce the comma
bridge: ‘When two red bricks (verbs) are next to each
Example of a MetaTaal therapy session (Session 6,
other, a comma bridge has to be placed between them.
duration 30 min) Between two verbs, you often make a pause when you
Starting level: the child can produce and build right- speak, and you can write a comma’.
branched sentences (OS-type RCs) in a structured (ther-
apy) setting.
Targets for session 6:
r The child understands that centre-embedded sen- Then proceed to build the rest of the sentence: ‘looks at
tences (SS-type RCs) differ from previously build the carrot’.
and produced OS-type RCs.
r The child can identify both SS- and OS-type RCs (5) Next, build an SVO, an OS type and an SS type
and discriminate between the two RC types. sentence on the base plate. Examine the differences
between sentences together. Point out the differ-
Activities: ences between the main clause and subordinate
clauses.
(1) Recapitulation of the previous session: this is what
we did in the last session, and this is the sentence Next, proceed with the second SVO sentence: Marlies
type that we built with the Lego
R
bricks (optionally: carries the flowers.
build the last sentence on the base plate).
(2) Introduce the target for today’s session (today we’ll
start building sentences that have two bridges, and Then make a SS-type sentence: Marlies, who stands before
they are called double bridge sentences). the house, carries the flowers.
(3) Put the crib sheet on the table that has both types
of bridges (bridge and comma bridge).
(4) Show the picture of two goats looking at a carrot
(worksheet 1). Start with a SVO sentence:

The goat looks at the carrot. Emphasize again that the RC (double bridge sentence)
Build the sentence and ask: ‘Do we know now which adds extra information!
goat looks at the carrot? No, we don’t, both of them
look at the carrot’. ‘Now we are going to add more (6) Take worksheet 6.2 (three sentence types) and to-
information on one of the goats: The goat that has spots, gether with the child, arrange the different sentences
looks at the carrot’. according to the 3 sentence types on the base plate
(short SVO, OS- and SS-type).
‘MetaTaal’ and SLI 291
Appendix B
Table B1. Crib sheet for the Lego
R
bricks used with
MetaTaal
Green brick, 1 × 2 Determiner (the, a) Is it a small word?
(Dutch: de, het, een) Is it ‘de’, ‘het’ or ‘een’?

Red brick, 2 × 3 Verb Is it something you can do?


For instance: walk, read, talk
(Dutch: lopen, lezen, praten)

Yellow brick, 2 × 2 Preposition Can it be followed by ‘the table’?


For instance: up, under, next to
(Dutch: op, onder, naast)

Blue brick, 2 × 4 Noun Can you place ‘the’ or ‘a’ before it?
For instance: tree, woman, dog, bag, coat Is it a human, an animal or a thing?
(Dutch: boom, vrouw, hond, tas, jas)

Brown brick, 2 × 2 Adjective Does it say something about the noun it precedes?
For instance: green, beautiful, happy, small Is het een kenmerk van de mens, het dier of het ding?
(Dutch: groen, mooi, blij, klein)

Grey bridge with a white top Complementizer Can it bridge two sentences together?
For instance: who, that Is it who or that (‘die’, ‘dat’)?
(Dutch: die, dat)
292 Rob Zwitserlood et al.
Appendix C

Figure C1. Individual scores on the OS Completion task at each time point. T1–T3 = baseline measurements prior to treatment; T4 =
directly after treatment; and T5 = retention measurement.
‘MetaTaal’ and SLI 293

Figure C2. Individual scores on the SS Composition task at each time point. T1–T3 = baseline measurements prior to treatment; T4 =
directly after treatment; and T5 = retention measurement.
294 Rob Zwitserlood et al.

Figure C3. Individual scores on the Repetition task (7 words) at each time point. T1–T3 = baseline measurements prior to treatment; T4 =
directly after treatment; and T5 = retention measurement.
‘MetaTaal’ and SLI 295

Figure C4. Individual scores on the Recreation task at each time point. T1-T3 = baseline measurements prior to treatment; T4 = directly
after treatment; and T5 = retention measurement.
296 Rob Zwitserlood et al.

Figure C5. Individual scores on the Repetition task (12 words) at each time point. T1–T3 = baseline measurements prior to treatment;
T4 = directly after treatment; and T5 = retention measurement.
‘MetaTaal’ and SLI 297

Figure C6. Individual scores on the Comprehension task at each time point. T1–T3 = baseline measurements prior to treatment; T4 =
directly after treatment; and T5 = retention measurement.
Copyright of International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders is the property
of Wiley-Blackwell and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to
a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may
print, download, or email articles for individual use.

Вам также может понравиться