Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 22

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/223995009

Stress and health: Research and clinical applications

Chapter · January 2000

CITATIONS READS

41 991

4 authors, including:

Dianna Theadora Kenny


DK Consulting
356 PUBLICATIONS   5,451 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Singing, Body Movement and Sound Intensity View project

Classical singing View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Dianna Theadora Kenny on 05 April 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Psychological foundations of stress and coping: A
developmental perspective
Dianna T. Kenny

Chapter 4 (pp. 73-104), Kenny, D. T., Carlson, J. G., McGuigan, F. J., & Sheppard, J. L. (Eds.) (2000). Stress and health: Research and
clinical applications . Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Gordon Breach/Harwood Academic Publishers. ISBN 90 5702 376 8.

This paper represents an attempt to achieve some synthesis across a range of theoretical perspectives whose goals are, among others,
to predict how individuals are likely to cope with stressors. The paper takes a developmental perspective, and searches for the keys
to coping capacity within the context of the earliest relationships with primary caregivers. Theories of attachment, human develop-
ment, personality, and psychopathology each contribute both theory and research to our understanding of risk and resilience, and these
perspectives are linked to theories of stress and coping, and life events research via key concepts that occur across each discipline. A
model is offered that proposes that the quality of attachment, which influences the development of either adequate or inadequate object
relations, (ie internal working models or mental representations of relationships) is the key resource upon which the development of
other resources depends. It is argued that “good enough” attachment is necessary but not sufficient for adequate coping, and that both
personality and psychopathology represent the outcomes of developmental, attachment, and coping experiences that can either inten-
sify or reduce the risk of adverse consequences. The model is transgenerational, in that vulnerable and resilient children transfer their
attachment and coping experiences, via their internal working models, into their parenting of the next generation of children.

Key words: Attachment, stress and coping; development; children, adolescents.

INTRODUCTION does development affect coping? What are the


critical precursors of coping in children, and how
In 1985, a massive earthquake devastated Mexico does coping affect development? In attempting
City. The physical and psychological ravages of the to address these issues, I will draw on a range of
earthquake were mitigated briefly by the report of theories in the developmental psychology and
the astonishing survival of 23 newborn babies who stress literature. Bodies of knowledge related
were rescued between four and eight days after the to development, attachment, stress and coping,
earthquake from the rubble of collapsed maternity personality, and developmental psychopathol-
hospitals. What can account for the extraordi- ogy initially pursued essentially parallel courses,
nary resilience displayed by these neonates, some and developed idiographic models, descriptors,
of whom had survived without food, warmth, or theories, and research paradigms. As each body of
contact comfort for up to eight days? A follow up knowledge matured, main effect models have given
investigation of eight of these infants 15 months way to more interdisciplinary, interactive, and
after the earthquake did not reveal any overt signs transactional models (Compas, Hinden, & Gehardt,
of pathology as a result of their experience (Lopez 1995). This paper represents a further attempt to
& Leon, 1989). integrate theories in each of these domains and to
Studies of 55 Mexican school children aged present a heuristic, unitary model that incorporates
6-12 years who had survived the earthquakes the fundamental principles of each of these domains
revealed very different cognitive representations into a coherent and testable framework.
of earthquakes depending on their age and level of
cognitive development (Vega, Ollinger, Zimmerman, The Model
& Figueroa, 1987). Among the adult survivors aged In this paper, I propose that coping be defined as the
18-64 years, a number of researchers reported very acquisition and conservation of resources throughout
different crisis-response patterns depending on age, the lifespan, and the utilisation of these resources in
past losses, and pre-existing psychosocial problems such a way that a net loss of resources is minimised.
at the time of the earthquake (de la Fuente, 1990; Attachment is conceptualised as a key resource that
Dufka, 1988). is necessary but not sufficient for adequate coping.
In this chapter, I will address some key questions Both personality and psychopathology represent the
highlighted in the above vignette pertaining to coping outcomes of developmental, attachment, and coping
in children and adolescents. For example, how does experiences that can either intensify or reduce the
coping change over the course of development? risk of adverse consequences. The interplay of
Is coping in childhood continuous with adult risk reducing and risk enhancing experiences may
coping or are there qualitative differences? How produce either resilient or vulnerable individuals.
Chapter 4, Stress and Health Psychological foundations of stress and coping 31
The development of psychopathology is conceptu- social adjustment achievable, their ability to cope,
alised as an outcome of a major imbalance between and the emergence of psychopathology. All of these
risk reducing (ie prevention of resource loss) and factors contribute to the quality of parenting that they
risk enhancing (ie acceleration of resource loss) are able to provide to their children. The quality of
factors. Such an imbalance can occur at any stage attachment is determined by the quality of parenting
of the lifespan, and may be brought about by the and by the presence and quality of compensatory
interaction of intra-personal, interpersonal, and relationships that are available to the child. Object
environmental factors. Psychopathology itself relations and available resources, both material and
may be viewed as a resource conserving strategy, personal, determine the way in which experiences
whereby the pathology, for example, depression, are appraised, and these factors underpin the coping
reduces the physical and social demands on an repertoire of the individual. From this repertoire,
individual whose resources have been seriously behavioural attempts to cope with challenges emerge,
depleted by preceding life stresses and/or everyday and the outcome of this coping behaviour is either
challenges. A schematic representation of the model resilience (positive coping under conditions of risk)
is contained in Figure 1. or vulnerability (maladaptive coping, including the

Social Context
• social climate
• social support

Parent Child

Innate
characteristics
Early life
experiences Psychosocial
adjustment /
Innate characteristics
coping /
• temperament
Current life psychopathology
• intelligence
experiences • physical potential

Quality of Development Life


parenting of resources experiences

Quality of Quality of
attachment object relations
Appraisal of stress

Coping repertoire
Presence and
quality of
compensatory
relationships and
social support Coping behaviour

Resilient Vulnerable

Fig.1 A model of the generational transmission of coping

The model is predicated on the view that the quality development of psychopathology). The child then
of attachment, which influences the development transfers these experiences into their parenting of
of either adequate or inadequate object relations the next generation of children.
(ie internal working models or mental representa- This model draws on theory and research
tions of relationships), is the key resource upon into stress and coping, lifespan development,
which the development of other resources depends. attachment, personality, psychopathology, and the
Attachment quality is multi-determined, and includes related concepts of vulnerability and resilience. In
the social context into which both parents and their the following sections, each of these theories will
children are born. Innate characteristics of parents, be described, their commonalities elucidated, and
their early life experiences, including relationships their unique contributions to the proposed model
with their caregivers, interact with current life highlighted.
experiences, such as a supportive or abusive partner
and financial status to influence the level of psycho-

Psychological foundations of stress and coping 32 Kenny D.T., 2000


Theories of stress (Abouserie, 1994; Kliewer & Sandler, 1992),
optimism (Scheier & Carver, 1992, 1993), resource-
Over the past twenty years, there has been a shift
fulness (Garmezy, 1983; Kobasa & Puchetti,
in the focus of stress theories from the identification
1983; Rutter, 1989), self-efficacy (Bandura, 1995,
of stressors that cause ill health and psychopathol-
Schwarzer, 1994), and mastery (Felston, 1991;
ogy to the identification of resources that promote
health and well-being (Antonovsky, 1979, Bandura, Hobfoll & Lerman, 1988; Younger, 1991). I will
1995, Dohrenwend & Dohrenwend, 1981, Hobfoll, argue that quality of attachment and subsequent
1988). According to Hobfoll (1989), resources are quality of object relations subsumes all of these
“the single unit necessary for understanding stress” personal characteristics.
(p. 516). His proposed model of stress, ie Conser- Although Hobfoll’s theory pertains to adult stress
vation of Resources (COR), posits that “people and coping, one can readily describe these resources
strive to retain, protect, and build resources and as they relate to children. Children, like adults,
that what is threatening to them is the potential or value object resources, such as bicycles, computers,
actual loss of these valued resources” (p. 516). He swimming pools, and the latest clothes. Conditions
further states that “loss is the key element of stress such as class captain, captain of the football team,
and that loss spirals are especially likely under dux of the school, or having a best friend serve the
chronic stress conditions because of the ongoing same stress buffering effect that adult conditions
depletion of resources” (Lane & Hobfoll, 1992, p. described above exert on adults. All of the personal
935). In other words, “stress occurs when resources characteristics described for adults have been
are lost, threatened with loss, or where resources reported in children. Children displaying such
are invested without subsequent resource gain” characteristics are frequently described as resilient
(Hobfoll, Schwarzer, & Chon, 1996, p.14). (Brown & Harris, 1986; Rutter, 1985, 1986, 1987).
A number of resource models of stress propose The fourth group of resources are called energies,
that key resources, that is, resources that control, and include money, time, and knowledge. Energies
promote, or organise the interaction of other facilitate the acquisition of other resources. Social
resources are central to understanding coping relationships may be energies if they are self-
(Hobfoll, Schwarzer, & Chon, 1996). Bandura’s enhancing, that is, if they provide needed social
(1995) concept of self-efficacy, or Kobasa’s (1979) support. They can also challenge resources and
concept of hardiness are examples of key resource result in net loss of other valued resources. If the
models. In this paper, I will argue that attachment quality or type of social support offered is inappro-
quality is the key resource on which people’s ability priate, the recipient may be worse off emotionally
to mobilise and utilise other resources is based, from having experienced this type of support than
including their capacity to develop self-efficacy and had they received no support at all. For example,
hardiness, and to obtain social support. grieving individuals benefit most from the
In contrast to key resource models, Hobfoll opportunity to ventilate their feelings of loss. If
(1988, 1989) and Holahan and Moos (1986; 1987) support people discourage this behaviour in favour
propose multiple resource models in which a of giving advice or encouraging future orientation
number of resources interact to deal with stressors. in the grieving person, the result is increased anger
Hobfoll proposes four types of resources which and anxiety, and other complicated grief reactions
people strive to develop and enhance. These are (Meyerowitz, 1996).
object resources such as houses and cars, which Hobfoll’s model represents an expansion and
meet basic physical needs and provide status to the refinement of a model proposed by Lazarus and
individual. The degree to which the second group Folkman (1984), in which six key areas are identified
of resources, that is, conditions such as marriage, from which an individual’s capacity to cope is
job security, or seniority exert a stress buffering derived. These are health and energy; positive
effect on individuals may be related to the extent to beliefs; material resources; problem solving skills;
which such conditions are valued by the individual. social skills; and social support. Both models assign
The third group of resources are personal charac- a significant role to social support.
teristics. A large number of personal character-
Social support
istics have been associated with stress resistance
and physical and psychological well-being. These The complexity of the construct of social support
include positive self-regard (Jalajas, 1994; Kreger, creates problems in both definition and measurement,
1995; Medvedova, 1995), internal locus of control (Kessler, 1992; Turner, 1992; Vaux, 1992; Veiel &
Chapter 4, Stress and Health Psychological foundations of stress and coping 33
Baumann, 1992) but is so central to theories of stress Jones & Carpenter, 1984). Significant differences
and coping that it deserves special attention. Social have been reported between individuals who can
support has been conceptualised in four ways: as (1) muster a great deal of social support compared to
social integration; (2) the subjectively experienced those who receive very little support. Accordingly,
quality of an individual’s relationships (Gentry & Winnubst, Buunk, and Marcelissen (1988) described
Kobasa, 1984); (3) perceived supportiveness and social support as a personality characteristic. These
helpfulness of others ( Sarason & Sarason, 1985); differences include attractiveness, sociability,
and (4) the enactment of supportive behaviours assertiveness, extroversion, emotional stability,
(Thoits, 1985; Winnubst, Buunk, & Marcelissen, sensitivity, and low social anxiety, among others
1988). Social integration is defined as the number, (Heller, Swindle & Dusenbury, 1986; Sarason &
strength, and type of connections of individuals Sarason, 1985). Adverse temperamental charac-
to significant others in their social environment teristics combined with poor quality attachments
(Antonucci, 1985; Rook, 1984). There is substantial may combine to reduce the social competence of
evidence demonstrating the important role of social individuals who most need social support yet who
embeddedness in health outcomes. Moreover, its are least capable of obtaining it. From the perspective
absence, that is, social isolation, has been strongly of a resource theory of stress, lack of social support
implicated in morbidity and mortality (Berkman, would result in further loss of resources, making
Leo-Summers, & Horwitz, 1992; Williams et al, the individual increasingly vulnerable to further
1992). Conversely, Hobfoll (1985) has argued stressful experiences (Lane & Hobfoll, 1992).
that it is not the number of relationships that is When it comes to psychological resources, the rich
important. Rather, it is the presence of one or two get richer and the poor, poorer.
intimate relationships that protects against psycho-
social and health risk. The importance of at least Theories of development
one intimate, confiding relationship to later mental Paul Baltes (1987) conceptualises lifespan
health in children has been stressed by a number of development as the interchange of gains (growth)
researchers, and is discussed in more detail below, and losses (declines) throughout life. He notes that
in the section on attachment. “the nature of what is considered a gain or a loss can
From the foregoing, it is clear that social support change with age: it involves objective in addition to
as a construct is multi-determined, emanating both subjective criteria; and is conditioned by theoretical
from the environment (Cohen & Syme, 1985, Thoits, predilection, standards of comparison, cultural and
1986) and from perceptions within the person historical context, as well as by criteria of functional
ie from object relations (Shumaker & Brownell, fitness or adaptivity” (Baltes, 1997, p. 367).
1984); multi-faceted, incorporating social relation- According to Baltes, an individual’s field of
ships, social attachments, social assistance, and the developmental potential is dependent on age (ie
perception of the availability and quality of support; lifespan stage and the status of the gain/loss ratio),
and multi-dimensional, in that support can be genetic endowment (ie plasticity, innate charac-
instrumental, tangible, informational, or emotional teristics), the prevailing historical, cultural, and
(House, 1981). I argue that perceived social support economic conditions (ie historical embeddedness),
is the affective quality of object representations that and the context in which the individual is developing
have arisen from early attachment experiences. It is (contextualism). These factors, if present on the
the perception of the availability of positive social gains side of the ledger, are identifiable resources
support that makes real supportive relationships described by Hobfoll in the previous section.
possible. This view has been expressed in a number Clearly, the goal of development is to maintain an
of different ways by others (eg Cohen & Wills, optimal gains/loss ratio throughout the lifespan in
1985; Sarason, Sarason, & Pierce, 1990). Whatever the same way that an individual strives to enhance
the definition, social support is a transactional and maintain resources in Hobfoll’s theory.
process that may emanate as much from within the According to Baltes’s model, lifespan development
individual as from the social environment. That is, is characterised by an age-related increase in special-
social support is both given and received. isation (selection) of resources and skills. A balance
Individuals differ widely in relational or social between gains and losses is achieved through the
competence (ie those behaviours and skills that process of selective optimisation with compensa-
facilitate the acquisition and maintenance of tion. That is, limits to functioning in any develop-
satisfying relationships), a skill which has been mental domain as a result of age or other limiting
found to be deficient in lonely individuals (Hansson, processes, such as illness or injury, is compensated
Psychological foundations of stress and coping 34 Kenny D.T., 2000
by increasing specialisation and the development of many hypothetical possibilities. Not all children
of substitute or compensatory mechanisms. For develop the capacity for formal operational thought
example, young infants can perceive very fine speech once they reach adolescence, and of those who do,
contrasts that occur in languages other than the one they tend to demonstrate another Baltian develop-
to which they are exposed (ie their native language mental characteristic, that is, specialisation. Formal
environment). As they develop and are increas- operations may only develop in areas in which the
ingly exposed to only one language environment, adolescent exhibits great interest and involvement
they lose the perceptual capacity to distinguish in, for example, physics, music, fashion design,
phonologically irrelevant contrasts due to lack of social relationships, or literary appreciation (Piaget,
exposure to particular speech sounds (Burnham, 1972). These capacities constitute the gains of this
1986). Perception of speech contrasts diverges period of development. Good cognitive development
with age, increasing for the native language and includes the capacity to problem-solve, to perform
decreasing for non-native languages between the well academically, and to develop functional coping
ages of two and six years. Hence, the developmental strategies. These skills have been identified as key
loss of speech perception for all languages is offset resources in resilient adolescents that protect them
by greater specialisation in the native language against adversity (Werner, 1995).
(Burnham, Earnshaw, & Clark, 1991). Similarly, as However, the attainment of formal operations
children become more efficient in the use of their may also constitute a vulnerability factor, and may
first language, they experience increasing difficulty hinder an adolescent’s attempts to deal with the
in learning a second language (Davies, Criper, & many demands of this stage of development. The
Howatt, 1994; Kellerman & Smith, 1986). losses from formal operations are the development
Cognitive development also lends itself to a of two unique forms of adolescent self-conscious-
similar gains/loss explanation. For example, during ness, or adolescent egocentrism, that Elkind (1967)
the preschool years, children slowly develop the termed the imaginary audience and the personal
capacity to monitor their own thought processes fable. The cognitive skill of formal operations
and performances, a skill that Flavell (1979) that allows adolescents to imagine other people’s
called metacognition. The same capacity has been thoughts, lead them to mistakenly believe that
labelled “reflective-self function” by attachment others are as preoccupied with their thoughts and
theorists and will be discussed in the next section. appearance as the adolescents themselves. This can
For now, we will focus on the gains/loss dichotomy create anxiety and self-criticism in the developing
for this attribute. The emergence of metacognition adolescent that may paralyse action and make
is associated with improved impulse control and communication with adults difficult. This in turn
delay of gratification, which is related to the coping may preclude adolescents from an important source
strategies used during the waiting period. Hence, of potential social support, the adult world. Further,
development of metacognition is closely linked to the formal operational thinking enables adolescents to
development of coping, and the ability to verbalise speculate about a range of philosophical, moral,
the coping strategies adopted to deal with frustration social, and religious dilemmas. Some formal
(Maccoby, 1983). Other gains from the emergence of operational adolescents develop grandiose plans for
metacognition include the adoption of more mature solving all the ills of the world and assign themselves
problem-solving strategies, the capacity to plan, and a central role in these solutions (Inhelder & Piaget,
the development of future orientation. Metacogni- 1958), while others may become depressed and
tion, however, makes children more aware of risks, overwhelmed by the enormity of the dilemmas
more vulnerable to negative self-evaluation, more confronting humanity and themselves, and become
vulnerable to being humiliated (since they have frozen by self-deprecation and feelings of hopeless-
become more sensitive to the reactions of others to ness into inaction. In extreme cases, in conjunction
the self), and more anxious. with other serious vulnerabilities, this process may
During adolescence, a cognitive shift occurs in lead to suicide, which is one of the leading causes of
which some concrete-operational children become death in adolescence (Kenny & Waters, 1995).
formal operational adolescents (Piaget, 1970). The Let us return for a moment to our Mexican
formal operational thinker is able to think logically neonates trapped under the rubble of an earthquake.
about a number of interconnected variables, to Since they survived, apparently with no discernible
develop and test hypotheses, to think abstractly, to ill-effects, can they be said to have ‘coped’ with this
connect past experience with future possibilities extraordinary life event? An analysis focused on the
and to become aware that concrete reality is just one gains/loss ratio theory of development may provide
Chapter 4, Stress and Health Psychological foundations of stress and coping 35
some insight into this question. On the loss or debit structures for self-regulation of affect arousal, the
side, the newborns were unfamiliar with the event need for environmental control diminishes. Very
and had no reservoir of coping strategies to deal young children have a limited bank of prepared
with it. This combination of factors would render an reactions to stressful situations. The over-arching
adult vulnerable to the stressor and place him/her at strategy is to retreat to the attachment figure. This
risk for negative outcomes. However, on the gains pattern of coping in very young children was
side of the ledger, the neonates would not have elegantly demonstrated in the strange situation
perceived the event to be threatening or harmful, experiments conducted by Mary Ainsworth (1973).
and would not have responded to the event in such As children grow, they develop situationally specific
a way that would have resulted in the depletion of coping strategies and learn to seek support from
their physiological and psychological resources. In other important figures, such as peers, siblings, and
fact, when rescuers reached the babies, they found teachers, and to rely less on the attachment figure.
some of them asleep! Such resource conserving If the initial attachments were faulty, this creates
activity no doubt contributed in a significant way potential vulnerabilities at each stage of the lifespan.
to their survival. Resource conserving behaviours In a later section, the central role of secure attachment
have been observed in most stages of the lifespan; in healthy development will be discussed. For now,
for example, in pre-school children following let us turn our attention to life events research and
reunion with mothers from whom they had been its contribution to our understanding of stress and
separated. Following their mothers’ return, children coping in childhood and adolescence.
demonstrated decreased positive affect, activity,
heart rate, and active sleep, features which are also Life events in childhood and adolescence
present in adult depression. This pattern is hypoth- Current conceptualisations of life-span
esised to be homeostatic or resource-conserving, development are underpinned by transactional
following as it does, a period of increased arousal models in which genes, persons, and environments
during the period of separation (Field & Reite, are thought to influence each other in reciprocal ways
1984; Field, 1996). (Compas, 1987; Rutter, 1997; Scarr, 1992; Scarr &
Human development unfolds simultaneously on McCartney, 1983). Plomin, DeFries, McClearn, &
multiple levels and in multiple domains of human Rutter (1997) have described three such interac-
functioning (physical, cognitive, and psychoso- tions.
cial), all mutually influencing the developmental “... passive correlations that arise because parental
trajectories that children follow to adulthood. For genes, insofar as they influence parental behaviour,
example, there is a complex relationship between also influence the child’s experiences ....evocative
the onset of puberty in girls and the emergence of correlations that derive from the effect of one person’s
dieting and eating disorders. Puberty results, among characteristics in eliciting particular responses in
other changes, in the accumulation of fat around the other people...[and] active correlations that arise
hips and thighs, which can be disturbing to some from the processes by which individuals shape and
adolescent females. Attitudes to their changing body select their environments’ (Rutter, 1997, p. 393).
shape may be mediated by prevailing community In contrast, life events research has remained
attitudes and media influences, as well as by a range rooted in a linear model in which life events are
of factors associated with individual cognitive seen as causal factors in the development of coping
and social development (Rutter, 1994). Although and psychopathology (Compas, 1987). As with
puberty may trigger abnormal eating behaviours and many linear models, the direction of causality
poor body image, other factors related to intra-and in the observed associations between life events
interpersonal difficulties are more strongly related to and psychopathology or illness is problematic.
maintenance and severity of eating disorders (Attie Do stressful life events cause physical or mental
& Brooks-Gunn, 1989; Kenny & Adams, 1995). illness or are vulnerable individuals more likely
Changes in the ability to cope are linked to major to experience life events as stressful? Life events
maturational changes throughout the lifespan, such research has also suffered from problems in classi-
as the development of language, the capacity to fication, definition, and measurement (Boekaerts,
walk, and the onset of puberty. The younger the 1996). Various classificatory systems have been
child, the greater the need for external structures developed to describe different life events, but most
to reduce the child’s vulnerability to maladaptive have proven unsatisfactory (Goodyer, 1988). Brim
coping behaviours in stressful situations (Maccoby, and Ryff (1980) argue for a central role for life events
1983). As the child matures and develops internal within lifespan developmental theory, conceptu-
Psychological foundations of stress and coping 36 Kenny D.T., 2000
alising them as states of disequilibrium that make reflective self-function, quality of attachment,
further development possible. Rutter (1994) on the social support, self-concept, and adaptability that
other hand, argues that life events are more likely link multiple variables is likely to be a more fruitful
to exacerbate or accentuate pre-existing psychologi- methodology in the study of the impact of stressful
cal characteristics, rather than to change them or to life events on children and adolescents, and of the
deflect development along another trajectory. outcome of such experiences in terms of vulnerabil-
The majority of life event studies have employed ity and resilience.
a checklist or questionnaire format, asking partici- A life event is a rather crude measure of life
pants to indicate which of the listed life events they experience or life change. Life events do not provide
have experienced in a given time frame (Goodyer, information about the desirability or frequency of
1990; Program for Prevention Research, 1992; the occurrence, the duration of impact, controllabil-
Sandler, Ramirez, & Reynolds, 1986). One of ity, or personal meaning to the individual. Timing
the problems in life events research with children (in terms of development) of onset of the event,
and adolescents is the failure to obtain subjective experience with similar previous events, and the
appraisals of particular life events to permit the intensity of the event will also have an impact on
variance in individual perception of events to be the effect of the event on individuals.
accounted for (Monroe, 1982). Westen (1995) and Life events research will only advance our
Shedler, Mayman, & Manis (1995) also caution understanding of human stress and coping if
against reliance on self-report (ie checklist or causal models are developed and tested. The field
questionnaire) data, because at best, they can only of developmental psychopathology has taken up
reveal aspects of an individual’s functioning of this challenge, shifting from a conceptualisation
which s/he is aware. The existence of both explicit of aetiology based on linear models of causality
(conscious) and implicit (unconscious) knowledge involving discrete factors, such as gender, birth
of oneself has been demonstrated in many domains order, family size, and socioeconomic status;
of human functioning, including memory (Schacter,
specific life events such as death of a significant
1992), cognitions (Holyoak & Spellman, 1993), and
other; and acute stressors, such as separation from
affect and motivation (Westen, 1990). Moreover,
a caregiver or the birth of a sibling, to a view that
reliance on self-report may result in misclassifica-
is transactional and based on interacting processes.
tion of people as psychologically healthy, when
More complex interactive models that examine
expert clinical judgment and physiological measures
the combined and potentiating effects of specific
indicate that some of these individuals are psycho-
life events or adversities have been proposed to
logically distressed (Shedler, Mayman, & Manis,
overcome the shortcomings of earlier methodolo-
1995). Defensive denial of psychological distress
gies (Chess & Thomas, 1983; Goodyer, 1990).
has been reported in both adults and adolescents
(Weinberger, 1990). Methods for assessing implicit A hierarchical model has been proposed by Rutter
or unconscious knowledge need to be developed and his colleagues (Brown & Harris, 1986; Rutter,
in stress research, particularly with children and 1985, 1986, 1987) in which factors either contribute
adolescents. a direct effect to subsequent pathology or indirect
or modifier effects of risk. If a particular factor
A large number of life events have been implicated
intensifies the risk, the variable is described as a
in the psychological and physical well-being of
vulnerability factor; if the risk is diminished, the
children and adolescents. These include hospital
factor exerts a protective effect against risk. In many
admission (Rudolph, Dennig, & Weisz, 1995), birth
cases, vulnerability and protection may indicate the
of a sibling (Rutter, 1983), parental conflict and/or
presence or absence of the same factor. These factors
divorce (Sandler, Tein, & West, 1994), and loss of
a parent (Brown, 1988). Specific illnesses, such as can also exert catalytic effects as they may change
asthma, recurrent abdominal pain, headaches, and the mechanisms and effects of other variables.
juvenile rheumatoid arthritis have also been linked to Although this line of investigation has contributed
stressful life events (Forman, 1993). Reporting long major insights into our understanding of the putative
lists of observed associations between particular factors in the development of resilience and psycho-
life events and particular physical and psychologi- pathology, Lyons-Ruth (1996) has argued that
cal outcomes does not advance knowledge about such models still do not resolve the methodologi-
the causal chain in these associations. Rather, a cal problem of focusing on variables rather than
shift in focus from enumerating discrete events to a individuals. Like Westen (1995), she questions
search for underlying mechanisms, such as parental whether the current analytic models that ...use

Chapter 4, Stress and Health Psychological foundations of stress and coping 37


variables as units of analysis rather than individuals the open and synchronous responsiveness to infant
and which emphasise causal, linear relations among communications (Lyons-Ruth, 1996).
variables, are well suited to identifying and describing
The path from attachment to resilience is complex,
organisational coherence in individual behaviour (p.
71).
and a number of theories have been proposed to
account for the relationship. Fonagy, Steele, Steele,
Compas, Hinden, and Gerhardt (1995) also call Higgit, & Target (1994), for example, argue that the
for a shift from the search for ‘static markers’ key factor is the capacity for reflective-self function
(p. 273) of risk to a “...search for processes and in at least one primary caregiver that is most
mechanisms that account for negative outcomes associated with secure attachment in the infant.
during adolescence” (p. 273). In a series of studies, Fonagy and his colleagues
Theories of attachment demonstrated that attachment quality is based on
the inter-generational transmission of internal
Empirical theories of attachment that brought the working models of relationships, that is, their
significance of the maternal-child relationship into object relations. Object relations are defined as the
sharp focus had their origins in the work of John internalised, cognitive representations of relation-
Bowlby (1951, 1969, 1973, 1980), Mary Ainsworth ships experienced with the primary attachment
(1973), and White & Watts (1973), among others. figures. These representations provide a secure base
Subsequently, many researchers have emphasised for cognitive development and the development of
the importance of the empathic capacity of a primary social cognition (Revelle, 1995). Parents at risk of
caregiver as a protective factor against chronic transmitting insecurity to their children, by virtue of
family discord and other ongoing adverse social rejection, abuse, neglect, deprivation, or other risk
experiences (Rutter, 1985), and the development factors in their own childhoods, but who do not do
of child, adolescent, and adult psychopathology so, can be differentiated from at risk parents who
(Bowlby, 1980; Cantwell & Rutter, 1994; Hinde, have insecurely attached children by the complexity
1987; Hinde & Stevenson-Hinde, 1988; Kenny & of their internal working models of relationships.
Waters, 1995; Rutter, 1985, 1986, 1987; Sroufe Parents demonstrating such complexity differ from
& Fleeson, 1988). Parallel work reported in the parents who do not in their capacity for reflective
literature of psychology and psychoanalysis, partic- self-function. Differences in reflective-self function
ularly object relations theory and self-psychology account for individual differences in parental
have drawn similar conclusions (Garmezy & Rutter, sensitivity or empathy. Each parent transmits his/her
1985; Kohut, 1977; Ornstein, 1981). working model independently of the other parent.
Recent conceptualisations of attachment have Belonging to a two parent family can therefore
described a process that may occur across the be a protective factor against the development of
lifespan (Field, 1985), is almost certainly a insecurity of attachment and/or later psychopathol-
universal occurrence (Petrovich & Gewirtz, 1985), ogy if one of the parents is depressed, alcoholic,
and that may involve multiple caregivers (Tronick, violent, or in some way emotionally unavailable to
Winn, & Morelli, 1985). Field (1996) has suggested their child. The independence of the transmission of
a need for alternative methods to the strange each parent’s internal working model can account
situation paradigm (Ainsworth, 1967) for studying for resilient maltreated children, and other resilient
attachment, to include the study of how mothers children who have developed normally under
and infants interact in everyday situations. The list conditions of risk.
of attachment behaviours need to be expanded to Garmezy & Masten (1986) argue that attachment
include those that occur with attachment figures influences resilience through the child’s capacity
other than the mother, for example, fathers, siblings, to develop cognitive and social competence.
and peers. Field proposes the following definition. A resilient child is defined as one who remains
“Attachment is viewed as a relationship that develops competent despite exposure to adverse events or
between two or more organisms as they become experiences. The terms competence and coping
attuned to each other, each providing the other have sometimes been used interchangeably, with
meaningful stimulation and arousal modulation” some researchers claiming that the advantage of
(Field, 1996, p. 545). The critical features of recent assessing competence rather than coping or coping
definitions of attachment include its capacity for style is that competence can be operationalised and
arousal reduction through the caregiver’s prompt has a measurable outcome while coping is a process
response to distress and negative affect, the reinstate- that lends itself less well to operational definition
ment of a sense of security following arousal, and and measurement (Garmezy, Masten, & Tellegen,
Psychological foundations of stress and coping 38 Kenny D.T., 2000
1984). Others (eg Zeidner & Endler, 1996) describe 1989). Longitudinal studies of insecurely attached
coping as a multi-component process, incorporat- infants found that at ages 10-14 years, these children
ing perception of the stressor, coping repertoire, and were more dependent, less socially competent, and
coping goals. Unlike competence, which implies demonstrated lower self-esteem and resilience
successful coping, coping per se may include all compared to children of the same ages who had been
attempts, whether adaptive or maladaptive, to deal securely attached infants. Goodyer (1990) summed
with taxing situations. The term competence, as it is up from this research that:
used here, refers to the adaptive coping repertoire ...early secure attachments provide a learning
available to an individual, and its appropriate experience through which individuals internalise
application in given situations. or represent relationships. This representation of
relations seems to be carried forward to influence
How does attachment influence the development
expectations and attitudes towards the self and others.
of competence? A number of sources of evidence Thus early parenting experiences exert a significant
is available to make such a link. Firstly, attachment influence on relations in later social interactions (p
quality has consistently been related to the degree to 25).
which infants use their primary caregiver as a secure
Once adequate mental representations ie object
base to explore the environment. The maintenance
relations of significant relationships have been
of a strong sense of security in the infant supports established, they constitute a protective factor
optimal interaction with the environment, which in against potentially adverse experiences such as
turn provides many opportunities for the infant to separation from a caregiver (Fonagy, et al., 1994;
develop cognitive competence through a process of Goodyer, 1990; Quinton & Rutter, 1988; Wolkind
exploration, problem-solving, and the development & Rutter, 1985). Conversely, the absence of a
of mastery over both the social and non-social confiding, empathic relationship with one’s mother
environments (Basic Behavioural Science Task (or primary caregiver), that results in one of the
Force of the National Advisory Mental Health several forms of impaired attachment (see Lyons-
Council, 1996; Field, 1996; Jacobson, Edelstein, Ruth, 1996), is one of the most powerful and most
& Hofmann, 1994; Lyons-Ruth, 1996; Maccoby, frequently cited vulnerability factors for a range
1983). of adverse outcomes, including deficits in social
Secondly, there is evidence for a link between and cognitive competence and the development of
attachment quality and later cognitive development, psychopathology (Zeanah, 1996). Such represen-
suggesting that early experiences related to tations form the basis of the capacity to perceive
cognitive challenges may have an enduring impact social support as helpful, thereby facilitating the
on subsequent cognitive development. In one individual’s willingness to seek and receive it in
study, infants displaying disorganised attachments times of need. This ability is resource conserving
in infancy were found to have lower infant mental and is critical to coping.
development scores at 18 months, compared to Object relations theory has been theoretically useful
securely attached children (Lyons-Ruth, 1996). in linking affective and cognitive development. The
Other studies (Cox, Puckering, Pound, & Mills, theoretical formulations of object relations theory
1987; Mills, Puckering, Pound, & Cox, 1985) have have now found experimental validation in the work
identified direct pathways from maternal depression, of Fonagy et al., (1994) and others. Westen (1995),
to impaired attachment and subsequent cognitive for example, has proposed a “clinical-empirical
and social deficits in children. model of personality” (p.495) that integrates theories
Thirdly, there is a strong association between of coping and attachment, and is based on three
secure attachment and the development of social central questions: (a) What psychological resources
competence. For example, securely attached - cognitive, affective, and behavioural dispositions
infants exhibited more prosocial behaviours - does the individual have at his or her disposal?
towards parents and peers in the preschool years Westen describes four major classes of psychologi-
(Bretherton, 1985), compared to children displaying cal resources, assessment of which are necessary
either avoidant or disorganised attachment patterns, to describe personality, as follows: (1) cognitive
whose behaviour was characterised by a range functioning, expectancies, and belief systems, (2)
of maladaptive responses including aggression, capacity for, and awareness of affective experience,
hyperactivity, passive withdrawal, helplessness, (3) affect regulation, including coping strategies,
and depression (Main & Solomon, 1990; Renken, and (4) behavioural skills, including physical and
Egeland, Marvinney, Mangelsdorf, & Stroufe, motor skills, and other procedural skills that translate
Chapter 4, Stress and Health Psychological foundations of stress and coping 39
thought into action. Most personality theories with longer episodes of respiratory and viral
neglect the role of objective or external resources, illnesses, and poorer wound healing (Kiecolt-Glaser,
and how their presence or absence impacts on an Glaser, Gravenstein, Malarkey, & Sheridan, 1996).
individual’s personality development or capacity Marital conflict (Kiecolt-Glaser, Fisher, Ogrocki,
to cope. Lazarus and Folkman (1984) and Hobfoll Stout, Speicher, & Glaser, 1987), examination stress
(1989) have correctly included such resources in (Kiecolt-Glaser, Garner, Speicher, Penn, Holliday,
their models of stress, in a manner similar to the & Glaser, 1984), clinical depression (Herbert &
incorporation of social, historical, and cohort effects Cohen, 1993a), and psychological distress (Herbert
in theories of lifespan development (Baltes, 1987). & Cohen, 1993b) have all been associated with
The second and third questions posed by Westen down regulation of the immune system. Stressful life
(ie What does the person wish for, fear, and value, events per se appear to be more strongly associated
and how do these motives combine and conflict?); with subsequent disease onset than either the
and [How does the person experience the self and perception of stress, the negative affect generated,
others and to what extent can the individual enter or the degree to which the person coped adequately
into intimate relationships? (p. 495)] pertain to the with the stressor (Cohen, Tyrell, & Smith, 1991,
nature of the individual’s attachment experiences 1993). However, studies of the relationship between
and how these have impacted upon the development stressful life events and immune function have
of the self-concept, self-awareness, and the capacity generally reported small effects. Life events per se
to seek, obtain, and provide social support. Westen’s can only provide a crude measure of both the quantity
theory, in accord with the attachment theories or the quality of stress experienced by individuals.
described above, demonstrates the relationships Moreover, life event studies often fail to consider
between secure and insecure attachment and later either the intra-personal or inter-personal context
abilities to express and fulfil needs, to resolve intra in which the stressful event is occurring (Brown
psychic conflict, and to develop a coherent set & Harris, 1989). Measuring stress reactivity, that
of values that guide behaviour and contribute to is, the physiological, autonomic, and endocrine
identity formation. Westen (1995) emphasises the changes that result from identified stressors as a
roles of observation, clinical judgment, and the case means for understanding individual differences
study in providing dynamic explanations as opposed in responding to stress that occur in individuals
to static descriptions of personality, methodolo- under certain conditions, may overcome some of
gies that have a strong tradition in developmental the problems associated with life event approaches.
psychology (Garmezy & Masten, 1986; Piaget, Stress reactivity has been shown to vary between
1951, 1952, 1954). individuals whose life circumstances, self-report
of stress, coping behaviour, and performance have
Stress and health been found to be similar (Cacioppo et al, 1992;
Biopsychosocial models link stress, coping, and Shedler, Mayman, & Manis, 1995).
psychological adjustment with health and illness. A similar relationship between stress and illness
There are an impressive array of such theories has been found in infants and young children
related to aspects of adult health (Ader & Cohen, (Forman, 1993). For example, significant changes
1984; Andersen, Kiecolt-Glaser, & Glaser, 1994). in salivary cortisol and behavioural response to the
Essentially, such models propose that stressors, pain of immunisation injections during the first six
whether of maximum (eg negative life events, months of life have been reported (Lewis, 1988;
chronic psychological distress, natural disasters, Lewis & Thomas, 1990; Worobey & Lewis, 1989).
wars, poverty) or minimum (eg. choices, challenges, This decrease suggests that as the infant matures, it
daily hassles, or irritations) magnitude, are consist- becomes more able to cope with stressors, such as
ently associated with autonomic and neuroendo- pain. Response to pain stimuli has been proposed as
crine responses, which in some cases result in down a possible benchmark for assessing an individual’s
regulation of the immune system, thereby rendering stress reactivity, since many pain stimuli, particu-
the organism more vulnerable to illness (Cacioppo, larly those induced by medical procedures, have
1996). some uniformity from one individual to another
Stress serves a somewhat paradoxical function for in terms of the physiological impact of the pain
humans, since it is both necessary for survival and stimulus (Haggerty, Sherrod, Garmezy, & Rutter,
strongly associated with susceptibility to disease, 1994). There appear to be individual differences
disease severity, and prognosis (Cacioppo, 1996). over and above those explained by maturation, that
For example, caregiver stress has been associated are demonstrable from about two months of age in
Psychological foundations of stress and coping 40 Kenny D.T., 2000
infants’ responses to pain stress. During follow- cial stressors and onset, duration, and severity of
up at 18 months of age, infants who quieted more both physical and mental illnesses in children and
quickly following immunisation at two months of adolescents. Individual variation in stress reactivity
age had significantly fewer atopic disorders and was hypothesised to underlie the modest associations
infections than infants who were slow to quiet. In found, and a case was made for the role of mediator
an interesting and innovative study, Lewis, Thomas, and moderator variables to enhance our understand-
and Worobey (1990) demonstrated a developmen- ing of the role of individual differences in respon-
tal trend in newborns’ ability to cope with stressful siveness to stressors. For example, in one study,
situations and subsequent illness. They found that children displaying high laboratory induced cardiac
although reactivity at two days of age was inversely stress reactivity had significantly more injuries than
related to subsequent frequencies of illnesses at 18 children displaying low cardiac stress reactivity under
months, reactivity at two months of age strongly high but not under low stress conditions. Cardiovas-
predicted this association. Healthier neonates, cular reactivity therefore functioned as a moderator
defined by Apgar and birth weight scores, were more variable, elucidating the relationship between
reactive at birth, but rapidly became less reactive reactivity and injury proneness (Boyce, Chesney,
at two months of age, suggesting that they were Kaiser, Alkon-Leonard, & Tschann, 1992). Recent
developing effective coping strategies to control evidence indicates that the cluster of characteristics
their reactivity to painful stimuli (ie immunisation). described as Type A personality in adults can be
Generally, coping with pain improves with age, observed in children, and that such a biobehavioural
although decreases in overt signs of distress are not pattern involving both greater heart rate and blood
always followed by similar decreases in self-reports pressure reactivity and increased competitiveness,
of pain or anxiety. Further, there are qualitative aggression, impatience, and anger demonstrates
differences in the way that younger and older modest stability throughout the lifespan (Haggerty
children express their distress. Younger children et al., 1994). Measures of autonomic and neuroen-
tend to cry and thrash about, while older children docrine reactivity have utility in further clarifying
will flinch and groan (Le Baron & Zeltzer, 1984). the direction and strength of associations between
Older children are more likely to employ cognitive stressors and morbidity (Barr, Boyce, & Zeltzer,
coping ie secondary control strategies when 1994).
confronted with uncontrollable medical procedures. Further support for the link between biology and
Younger children may be able to generate such behaviour is provided by studies that demonstrate
strategies but are less able to implement them in the improved immune function and prognosis of disease
stressful setting (Rudolph, Dennig, & Weisz, 1995). outcome following psychological interventions
A number of other factors affect a child’s response (Eysenck & Grossarth-Maticek, 1991; Fawzey,
to the stress of pain, including gender and prior Fawzy, Hyun, Gutherie, Fahey, & Morton, 1993;
experience with the stressor. Boys tend to display Gruber et al., 1993). There has been little parallel
less overt signs of distress when experiencing pain work in this area with children and adolescents
than girls, a difference attributed to different social- (Rutter, 1994), although there have been some
isation experiences (McGrath, 1993). Conflicting recent attempts to develop programs that promote
evidence regarding the effect of prior experience the development of social competence as a stress
with painful medical procedures exists, since both buffering strategy (The Consortium on the School-
habituation and sensitisation experiences may co- based Promotion of Social Competence, 1994; Pless
occur in the one experience (Rudolph, Dennig, & & Stein, 1994).
Weisz, 1995). There has been recent interest in the
role that temperament plays in moderating stress Coping
and coping responses of children and adolescents. Freud’s theory of the defences (Freud, 1938) is
Individuals vary on both biological and psychologi- arguably the first theory of coping, although current
cal dimensions such as physiological reactivity and formulations ignore this work in favour of a social-
behavioural adaptability, dimensions which impact cognitive perspective that is predicated on the
directly on responsiveness to stressors (Boyce, Barr, (implicit) assumption that the cognitive appraisal
& Zeltzer, 1992). that underlies coping behaviours is conscious and
In a review of available studies of the stress- that it can be reliably accessed through self-report
illness association in children and adolescents, Barr, (Auerbach & Gramling, 1998). However, some
Boyce and Zeltzer (1994) concluded that modest, forms of coping become habitual and automatic
significant relationships exist between psychoso- (ie unconscious) such that we engage in coping
Chapter 4, Stress and Health Psychological foundations of stress and coping 41
behaviour without being aware of any cognitive Saltoun, & Hayes, 1988; Suls & Fletcher, 1986);
appraisal occurring prior to the event, and without avoidant versus transformational coping (Maddi,
being able to articulate our reasons for acting in 1981); repression-sensitization (Byrne, 1961);
the way that we did (Boekearts, 1996; Kihlstrom, internal-external locus of control (Rotter, 1966);
1987; Loftus & Klinger, 1992). Westen (1995) coper-avoider (DeLong, 1970); and monitoring-
argues that the unconscious components of coping blunting (Miller, 1987). A number of comprehen-
are the expectancies and belief systems around sive reviews on coping styles are available (see
self-efficacy and behaviour-outcome expectancies Rudolph, Dennig, & Weisz, 1995; Sandler, Tein, &
that impact upon the motivation to perform and the West, 1994; Zeidner & Endler, 1996).
performance itself. These unconscious expectancies There appears to be considerable variability in
are akin to Freudian defences, which are activated in the dimensions derived from different studies to aid
certain circumstances to regulate affect (Eisenberg our understanding of the ways in which children
& Fabes, 1992; Westen, 1994). Another feature and adolescents cope with stress. Moreover, the
of dynamic formulations which are missing from dimensions derived from factor analysis, cluster
social-cognitive perspectives of stress and coping analysis, or other exploratory statistical techniques
is the possible existence of conflicting motives, often lack a clear theoretical underpinning and
goals, and behaviours and the unique solutions therefore have limited explanatory power (Sandler,
that individuals find to compromise among these Tein, & West, 1994). These conceptualisations
(Brenner, 1982). of coping are oversimplifications of a complex
Numerous attempts abound to identify, classify and phenomenon, and therefore leave much of the
categorise both the types of stressors experienced variance in coping behaviours unexplained. It
and the coping strategies used by children and seems unlikely that individuals would respond to
adolescents. For example, some of the commonest stressors in a unilateral way, that is, with either a
family stressors identified by adolescents include problem-focused or an emotion-focused coping
arguments with parents and siblings, not having response, and there is some evidence that all coping
enough money, and being treated like a child (Thomas involves both problem-focused and emotion-
& Groer, 1986). School based stressors have been focused strategies (Patterson & McCubbin, 1987). A
divided into academic (ie mastery of subject matter, number of researchers have pointed to the primacy
academic performance, evaluation, and meeting the of affect in directing behaviour; good moods induce
high expectations of others); and social stressors optimism which in turn increases the probability of
(ie relationships with teachers and peers, fear of positive coping rather than a retreat into defensive
rejection) (Matheny, Aycock, & McCarthy, 1993). responding (Maccoby, 1983). Current mood not
Elkind (1986) identified three categories of stressors only influences behaviour, but also biases later
faced by students: (1) forseeable and avoidable stress recall. People who are currently depressed are more
(eg drug use and breaking the law); (2) unforsee- likely to recall depressing events, while other more
able and unavoidable (eg illness and death); and (3) optimistic individuals are more likely to recall
forseeable but unavoidable (eg examinations). positive experiences (Bower, 1981). Westen (1995)
Using The Ways of Coping Checklist, Folkman suggests that actions, including coping attempts,
& Lazarus (1985) identified two main types of are motivated by wishes, fears, and values, each
coping - problem-focused and emotion-focused - of which involves both cognitive and affective
that could be broken down into eight subcategories, components: that is, the cognitive representation
three that are problem-focused (confrontive coping, has affect inextricably attached to it. Similarly,
planful problem-solving, and seeking social support MacDonald (1995) argues that personality itself is
to solve the problem); and five that are emotion- a series of motivational systems with an affective
focused (distancing, self-control, accepting respon- core, and that the attainment or avoidance of certain
sibility, escape-avoidance, positive re-appraisal, affective states is at the core of what motivates
and seeking emotional support). A number of other people to act.
typologies of coping styles and dispositions have From a very early age, infants engage in combina-
been proposed. These usually take the form of uni- tions of problem- and emotion-focused coping. The
dimensional scales, and include behavioural versus quintessential coping strategy, crying, elicits both
cognitive coping (Curry & Russ, 1985); primary problem-solving behaviour (eg offer of milk) and
control, secondary control, and relinquished control emotional comfort from the care-giver. Indeed, most
(Weisz, McCabe, & Dennig, 1994); approach versus of the infant’s other early attempts at mastery of the
avoidance (Ebata & Moos, 1991; Hubert, Jay, environment, for example, self-exploration, such
Psychological foundations of stress and coping 42 Kenny D.T., 2000
as sucking on lips, hands, fingers and toes, serves Sameroff, Seifer, Zax & Barocas, 1987; Werner &
a problem-solving function while simultaneously Smith, 1982) concluded that
providing emotional comfort through self-soothing. resilience is associated with two groups of
Many of the infant’s early self-directed behaviours protective factors (1) personal resources or disposi-
appear to have affect regulation as their primary goal. tional characteristics of the individual and (2) social
These prototypical coping attempts are tested within resources or characteristics of the relationships and
the relationship with primary attachment figures. If support within or outside the family (Bliesener &
the response is sufficiently attuned to the infant, such Losel, 1990, p 300-301).
that the interaction results in affect arousal reduction Protective factors within the individual include a
and a growing sense of mastery over one’s reactions coping pattern that combines an easy temperament,
and the environment, a sufficient basis is laid for autonomy together with the willingness to seek
later coping (Bandura, 1990; Rutter, 1990; Suomi, assistance and ask for help when one’s own
1991). Future research on coping may benefit from resources are not sufficient; intelligence and
the adoption of a clinical-empirical methodology academic achievement; internal locus of control,
described by Westen (1995). and a positive self-concept (Egeland, Jacobvitz,
In this chapter, I have argued that attachment is a & Stroufe, 1988; Werner, 1995). The primary
key resource upon which other resources are built, protective factor within the family is the presence
which in turn influence coping. Temperament and of at least one competent, emotionally stable and
intelligence are two such resources. Intelligence has empathic caregiver who is attuned to the needs of
consistently been associated with resilience (Cohler, the child (Anthony, 1987; Wallerstein & Blakeslee,
1987; Felsman & Vaillant, 1987; Garmezy & 1989). Other factors interact with gender to produce
Rutter, 1985; Egeland, Jacobvitz, & Stroufe, 1988; resilient boys and girls. For example, resilient
Werner, 1995), but intelligence, in the absence of boys have usually experienced a positive male role
a secure attachment, does not, by itself, enhance model, and have grown up in an environment that
coping. Recent attempts to identify other predictors has clear structures and rules. Resilient girls, on the
of coping have lead to the description of a number other hand, have experienced encouragement to take
of personality traits, such as hardiness (Kobasa, risks and to develop independence in the context of
1982), locus of control orientation (Rotter, 1966; reliable support from a female caregiver (Block &
Strentz & Auerbach, 1988), learned resourcefulness Gjerde, 1986, in Werner, 1995). Protective factors
(Rosenbaum, 1990), self-efficacy (Bandura, 1990), within the community include adult (eg teacher,
and learned helplessness/optimism (Seligman, coach) and peer support that is available in times
1991), among others. Successful personal mastery of crisis, and structures such as school, church and
experiences form the basis for the development of youth groups (Werner, 1995).
such characteristics (Aldwin, Sutton, & Lachman, Using this framework, psychopathology can be
1996). These begin in infancy, as described above, understood in terms of inadequate resources, in
in the presence of a secure attachment with a particular, impaired attachments, competencies,
primary caregiver, who is both attuned to the child’s temperament, and self-esteem, and the subsequent
needs and responds appropriately by providing the problems arising as a consequence, that form the
child with a safe environment in which to explore basis of impaired capacities to seek, obtain, and
and solve problems. The attributions that we make give social support. These impaired capacities in
about why we coped, our ability to generalise these turn impair the individual’s capacity to conserve
coping strategies appropriately, and our capacity resources and to cope adequately with stress. The
to modify ineffective coping behaviours are also group of resources proposed by Hobfoll (1988)
important factors in later adjustment (Auerbach & may interact in additive or synergistic ways to
Gramling, 1998). enhance or deplete the overall reservoir of resources
Coping models have been variously described as available to the individual. For example, economic
‘stress resistance’, ‘invulnerability’, ‘resilience’, hardship may exert a direct negative effect on the
‘protective factors’, ‘hardiness’, or ‘learned quality of the marital relationship, which in turn
resourcefulness’ (see for example, Anthony & may impact upon the couple’s capacity or energy
Cohler, 1987; Garmezy, 1987; Kobasa & Pucetti, for parenting. Poor quality parenting may result in
1983; Rutter, 1989). Longitudinal studies that have impaired attachments in their children which may
examined the interplay of risk and protective factors lead to psychological distress, psychopathology, or
in developmental psychopathology and resilience illness during childhood or adolescence. Impaired
(see, for example, Magnusson & Bergman, 1988; attachments and psychological distress negatively
Chapter 4, Stress and Health Psychological foundations of stress and coping 43
influence cognitive functioning, which in turn may REFERENCES
lead to poor school grades, lowered self-esteem,
Abouserie, R. (1994). Sources and levels of stress in
reduced capacity to deal effectively with stressors,
relation to locus of control and self esteem in university
fewer vocational opportunities, and so on. Such a students. Educational Psychology, 14 (3), 323-330.
model suggests a range of possible interventions to
Ader, R., & Cohen, N. (1984). Behavior and the immune
break this negative cycle of resource depletion, since system. In W. D. Gentry (Ed.). Handbook of behavioral
it suggests many possibilities to decrease stress by medicine (pp. 117-173). New York: Guilford Press.
enhancing resources via the most accessible route. Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1967). Infancy in Uganda: Infant
Relieving the economic hardship would constitute a care and the growth of love. Baltimore: John-Hopkins
sensible and fundamental intervention, but this may Press.
not be possible. Providing compensatory attachment Ainsworth, M.D.S. (1973). The development of infant-
figures and academic support to the children through mother attachment. In B. M. Caldwell & H. N. Ricciuti
the school may constitute an effective intervention (Eds). Review of child development research, Vol III.
to prevent educational disadvantage. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Aldwin, C. M., Sutton, K. J., & Lachman, M. (1996). The
development of coping resources in adulthood. Journal
CONCLUSION of Personality, 64, 837-872.
In this paper I have attempted to provide a Andersen, B. L., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Glaser, R.
synthesis of the literature in key but discrete areas (1994). A biobehavioural model of cancer stress and
of investigation and to propose a heuristic model disease course. American Psychologist, 49 (5), 389-
404.
that links each of these bodies of knowledge in such
a way that multiple interventions are suggested at Anthony, E. J. (1987). Children at high risk for psychosis
growing up successfully. In E.J. Anthony & B.J. Cohler
different levels of complexity. I have attempted
(Eds.) The invulnerable child (pp. 147-184). New York:
to argue that theorising about stress and coping Guilford Press.
without providing a developmental and social
Anthony, E.J. & Cohler, B.J. (Eds.) (1987). The
context in which the stress and coping occurs, invulnerable child (pp. 147-184). New York: Guilford
is to provide an incomplete picture of individual Press.
differences that remains rooted in description rather Antonovsky, A. (1979). Health, stress, and coping. San
than explanation. The proper starting point for an Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
investigation of how and why people experience Antonucci, T. C. (1985). Social support: Theoretical
stress in the way that they do and cope with it in advances, recent findings and pressing issues. In I.
idiosyncratic ways must begin with an examination G. Sarason & B. R. Sarason (Eds.). Social support:
of the social and relational context of the first Theory, research, and application (pp. 391-414). The
attempts to interact with the world. This includes an Hague, The Netherlands: Nijhoff.
elucidation of parental internal working models of Attie, I., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1989). Development of
relationships, their reflective self-function, and how eating problems in adolescent girls: A longitudinal
these interact with child characteristics, including study. Developmental Psychology, 25, 70-79.
temperament, intelligence, physical characteristics, Auerbach, S. M., & Gramling, S. E. (1998). Stress
and early life experiences to determine the quality management: Psychological foundations. Sydney:
of attachment, the development of object relations Prentice-Hall.
and other resources, and how these combine to Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theoretical propositions of life-span
determine coping. developmental psychology: On the dynamics between
growth and decline. Developmental Psychology, 23
Future research will need to elucidate the relation- (5), 611-626.
ships between parental internal working models,
Baltes, P. B. (1997). On the incomplete architecture
attachment, and coping; the relationships between of human ontogeny: Selection, optimization, and
attachment quality, immune function, and health; compensation as foundation of developmental theory.
the relationships between quality of attachment American Psychologist, 52 (4), 366-380.
and learned helplessness/resourcefulness; and Bandura, A. (1990). Conclusion: Reflections on nonability
importantly, since prevention is the best cure, determinants of competence. In R. J. Sternberg & J.
how we can ensure the development of adequate Kolligan (Eds.). Competence considered (pp. 315-
attachment and object relations in the majority of 362). New Haven: Yale University Press.
children, so that resilience is the major outcome of Bandura, A. (Ed.). (1995). Self-efficacy in changing
exposure to risk. societies. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Psychological foundations of stress and coping 44 Kenny D.T., 2000


Barr, R. G., Boyce, W. T., & Zeltzer, L. K. (1994). The (1-2, Serial No. 209), 66-104.
stress-illness association in children: A perspective Brim, O. G., Jr., & Ryff, C. D. (1980). On the properties
from the biobehavioral interface. In R. J. Haggerty, of life events. In P. Baltes & O. G. Brim, Jr. (Eds.) Life-
L. R. Sherrod, N. Garmezy, and M. Rutter (Eds.). span development and behavior ( Vol 3, pp. 368-388).
Stress, risk and resilience in children and adolescents: New York: Academic Press.
Processes, mechanisms, and interventions (pp. 182-
Brown, G. W. (1988). Early loss of parent and depression
224). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
in adult life. In S. Fisher, & J. Reason (Eds.). Handbook
Basic Behavioral Sciences Task Force of the National of life stress, cognition and health. (pp.441-465). John
Advisory Mental Health Council (1996). Basic Wiley & Sons: Chichester, England.
Behavioral Science Research for Mental Health:
Brown, G. W. & Harris, T. (1986). Stressor, vulnerability
Vulnerability and resilience. American Psychologist,
and depression - a question of replication. Psychological
51 (1), 22-28.
Medicine, 16, 739-744.
Berkman, L. F., Leo-Summers, L., & Horwitz, R. I.
Brown, G. W. & Harris, T. O. (1989). Life events and
(1992). Emotional support and survival following
illness. New York: Guilford Press.
myocardial infarction: A prospective population-based
study of the elderly. Annals of International Medicine, Burnham, D. K. (1986). Developmental loss of speech
117, 1003-1009. perception: Exposure to and experience with a first
language. Special issue: Language loss. Applied
Bliesener, T. & Losel, F. (1990). Resilience in adolescence:
Psycholinguistics, 7 (3), 207-239.
A study on the generalizability of protective factors.
In K. Hurrelmann & F. Losel (Eds.) Health hazards in Burnham, D. K., Earnshaw, L. J., & Clark, J. E. (1991).
adolescence (pp 299-320). Berlin: Aldine de Gruyter. Development of categorical identification of native
and non-native bilabial stops: Infants, children and
Block, J. (1995). A contrarian view of the five-factor
adolescents. Journal of Child Language, 18 (2), 231-
approach to personality description. Psychological
260.
Bulletin, 117, 187-215.
Byrne, D. (1961). The repression-sensitisation scale:
Block, J., & Gjerde, P. F. (1986). Early antecedents of
Rationale, reliability, and validity. Journal of
ego resilience in late adolescence, Paper presented
Personality, 29, 334-349.
at the annual meeting of the American Psychological
Association, Washington, DC, August, 1986. Cacioppo, J. T. (1996). Somatic Responses to
Psychological Stress: The reactivity hypothesis. Invited
Boekaerts, M. (1996). Coping with stress in childhood
address presented at the XXVI International Congress
and adolescents. In M. Zeidner, & N. S. Endler (Eds.).
of Psychology, Montreal, Canada, August, 1996.
Handbook of coping: Theory, research, applications
(pp. 452-484). Brisbane: John Wiley and Sons. Cacioppo, J. T., Uchino, B. N., Crites, S. L., Jr. Snydersmith,
M. A., Smith, G., Berntson, G. G., & Lang, P. J. (1992).
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American
Relationship between facial expressiveness and
Psychologist, 36, 129-148.
sympathetic activation in emotion: A critical review,
Bowlby, J. (1951). Maternal care and mental health. with emphasis on modelling underlying mechanisms
Geneva: World Health Organization. and individual differences. Journal of Personality and
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss. Vol 1: Attachment. Social Psychology, 62, 110-128.
London: Hogarth Press. Cantwell, D., & Rutter, M. (1994). Classification:
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss. Vol 2: Separation. Conceptual issues and substantive findings. In M.
New York: Basic Books. Rutter, E. Taylor, & L. Hersov (Eds.). Child and
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss. Vol 3: Loss, adolescent psychiatry: Modern approaches (pp. 3-21).
sadness, and depression. New York: Basic Books. London: Blackwell Scientific Publications.
Boyce, W. T., Barr, R. G., & Zeltzer, L. K. (1992). Chess, S. & Thomas, A. (1983). Origins and evolution
Temperament and the psychology of childhood stress. of behavior disorders: From infancy to adult life. New
Pediatrics, 90 (suppl.), 483-486. York: Brunner Mazel.
Boyce, W. T., Chesney, M., Kaiser, P., Alkon-Leonard, Cohen, S., & Syme, S. L. (1985). Social support and
A., & Tschann, J. (1992). Childcare stressors, health. London, New York: Academic Press.
cardiovascular reactivity, and injury incidence in Cohen, S., Tyrrell, D. A. J., & Smith, A. P. (1991).
preschool children (abstract). Pediatric Research, 31, Psychological stress and susceptibility to the common
9A. cold. New England Journal of Medicine, 325, 606-
Brenner, C. (1982). The mind in conflict. New York: 612.
International Universities Press. Cohen, S., Tyrrell, D. A. J., & Smith, A. P. (1993).
Bretherton, I. (1985). Attachment theory: Retrospect and Life events, perceived stress, negative affect, and
prospect. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.). Growing susceptibility to the common cold. Journal of
points of attachment theory and research. Monographs Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 131-140.
of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50 Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support,

Chapter 4, Stress and Health Psychological foundations of stress and coping 45


and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 31.
98, 310-357. Fawzy, F. I., Fawzy, N. W., Hyun, C. S., Gutherie, D.,
Cohler, B. J. (1987). Adversity, resilience, and the study Fahey, J. L., & Morton, D. (1993). Malignant melanoma:
of lives. In E. J. Anthony & B. J. Cohler (Eds.). The Effects of an early structured psychiatric intervention,
invulnerable child (pp. 363-424). New York: Guilford. coping, and affective state on recurrence and survival
Compas, B. E. (1987). Coping with stress during six years later. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50,
childhood and adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 681-689.
101, 393-403. Felsman, J. K., & Vaillant, G. E. (1987). Resilient
Compas, B. E., Hinden, B. R., and Gerhardt, C. A. (1995). children as adults: A 40 year study. In E. J. Anthony, &
Adolescent development: Pathways and processes of B. J. Cohler (Eds.). The invulnerable child
child. New York:
risk and resilience. Annual Review of Psychology, 46, Guilford Press.
265-293. Felston, G. (1991). Influences of situation-specific
Cox, A. D., Puckering, C., Pound, A., & Mills, M. (1987). mastery beliefs and satisfaction with social support on
The impact of maternal depression in young children. appraisal of stress. Psychological Reports, 69 (2), 483-
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 28, 917- 495.
928. Field, T. (1985). Attachment as psychobiological
Curry, S. L., & Russ, S. W. (1985). Identifying coping attunement: Being on the same wavelength. In M.
strategies in children. Journal of Clinical Child Reite, and T. Field (Eds.). Psychobiology of attachment
Psychology, 14, 61-69. and separation (pp. 455-480). New York: Academic.
Davies, A., Criper, C., & Howatt , A. P. R. (Eds.). (1984). Field, T. (1996). Attachment and separation in young
Interlanguage. Edinburgh, Scotland: Edinburgh children. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 541-561.
University Press. Field, T., & Reite, M. (1984). Children’s responses to
De la Fuente, R. (1990). Las consecuencias del desastre separation from mother during the birth of another
en la salud mental.l. ((Disaster consequences in regard child. Child Development, 55, 1308-1316.
to mental health.) Symposium: The September 1985 Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive
earthquakes in Mexico City: Medical aspects (1986, monitoring. American Psychologist, 34, 906-911.
Mexico City, Mexico). Salud Mental, 9 (3), 3-8. Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1985). If it changes it must
DeLong, R. D. (1970). Individual differences in patterns be a process: Study of emotion and coping during three
of anxiety arousal, stress-relevant information and stages of a college examination. Journal of Personality
recovery from surgery. Doctoral dissertation. University and Social Psychology, 48, 150-170.
of California, Los Angeles. Fonagy, P., Steele, M., Steele, H., Higgitt, A., & Target,
Dohrenwend, B. P., & Dohrenwend, B. S. M. (1994). The Emanuel Miller memorial lecture 1992:
(1981). Socioenvironmental factors, stress, and The theory and practice of resilience. Journal of Child
psychopathology. American Journal of Community Psychology and Psychiatry, 35 (2), 231-257.
Psychology, 9 (2), 128-164. Forman, S. (1993). Coping skills interventions for
Dufka, C. L. (1988). The Mexico City earthquake disaster. children and adolescents. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Social Casework, 69 (3), 162-170. Freud, S. (1915-1916) (1971 edition). The complete
Ebata, A. T., & Moos, R. H. (1991). Coping and introductory lectures on psychoanalysis. London:
adjustment in distressed and healthy adolescents. George Allen and Unwin.
Journal of Applied and Developmental Psychology, Freud, S. (1938). In A. A. Brill (Ed. & translation). The
12, 33-54. basic writings of Sigmund Freud
Freud. New York: Modern
Egeland, B., Jacobvitz, D., & Sroufe, L. A. (1988). Library.
Breaking the cycle of child abuse. Child Development, Garmezy, N. (1983). Stressors of childhood. In N.
59, 1080-1088. Garmezy, and M. Rutter, (Eds.). Stress, coping, and
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (Eds.). (1992). Emotion development in children. pp. 43-84. Baltimore, MD:
and regulation in early development: New directions Johns Hopkins University Press.
for child development (vol. 55). San Francisco: Jossey- Garmezy, N. (1987). Stress, competence, and
Bass. development: Continuities in the study of schizophrenic
Elkind, D. (1967). Egocentrism in adolescence. Child adults, children vulnerable to psychopathology, and the
Development, 38, 1025-1034. search for stress-resistant children. American Journal
Elkind, D. (1986). Stress and the middle grader. School of Orthopsychiatry, 57, 159-174.
Counseling, 196-206. Garmezy, N., & Masten, A. S. (1986). Stress, competence,
Eysenck, H. J., & Grossarth-Maticek, R. (1991). Creative and resilience: Common frontiers for therapist and
novation behaviour therapy as a prophylactic treatment psychopathologist. Behavior Therapy, 17, 500-521.
for cancer and coronary heart disease: II. Effects of Garmezy, N., Masten, A. S., & Tellegen, A. (1984). The
treatment. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 29 (1), 17- study of stress and competence in children: A building

Psychological foundations of stress and coping 46 Kenny D.T., 2000


block for developmental psychopathology. Child Hobfoll, S. E., & Lerman, M. (1988). Personal
Development, 55, 97-111. relationships, personal attributes, and stress resistance:
Garmezy, N. & Rutter, M. (1985). Acute reactions Mothers’ reactions to their child’s illness. American
to stress. In M. Rutter & L. Hersov (Eds.). Child Journal of Community Psychology, 16 (4), 565-589.
psychiatry: Modern approaches. Oxford: Blackwell. Hobfoll, S. E., Schwarzer, R. & Chon, K. K. (1996).
Gentry, W. D., & Kobasa, S. C. (1984). Social and Disentangling the stress labyrinth: Interpreting the
psychological resources mediating stress-illness meaning of the term stress as it is studied
studied. Paper
relationships in humans. In W. D. Gentry (Ed.). presented for the First Meeting of the International
Handbook of behavioral medicine (pp. 87-113). New Society of Health Psychology, Montreal Quebec,
York: Guilford Press. Canada, August 1996.
Goldberg, L. R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic Holahan, C. J., & Moos, R. H. (1986). Personality, coping
personality traits. American Psychologist, 48, 26-34. and family resources in stress resistance. A longitudinal
analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Goodyer, I. M. (1988). Stress in childhood and
51, 389-395.
adolescence. In S. Fisher & J. Reason (Eds.). Handbook
of life stress, cognition and health (pp. 23-40). England: Holahan, C. J., & Moos, R. H. (1987). Risk, resistance
John Wiley & Sons. and psychological distress: A longitudinal analysis with
adults and children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
Goodyer, I. M. (1990). Life experiences, development, 96, 3-13.
and childhood psychopathology. England: John Wiley
& Sons Ltd. Holyoak, K., & Spellman, B. (1993). Thinking. Annual
Review of Psychology, 44, 265-315.
Gruber, B. L., Hersh, S. P., Hall, N. R., Waletzky, L. R.,
House, J. S. (1981). Work stress and social support.
Kunz, J. F., Carpenter, J. K., Kverno, K. S., & Weiss, S.
Reading, MA.: Addison-Wesley.
M. (1993). Immunological responses of breast cancer
patients to behavioral interventions. Biofeedback and Hubert, N. C., Jay, S. M., Saltoun, M., & Hayes, M.
Self-Regulation, 18, 1-22. (1988). Approach-avoidance and distress in children
undergoing preparation for painful medical procedures.
Haggerty, R. J., Sherrod, L. R., Garmezy, N., & Rutter, M.
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 17, 194-202.
(1994) (Eds.). Stress, risk and resilience in children and
adolescents: Processes, mechanisms, and interventions. Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. thinking from childhood to adolescence. New York:
Basic Books.
Hansson, R. O., Jones, W. H., & Carpenter, B. N. (1984).
Relational competence and social support. Review of Jacobson, T., Edelstein, W., & Hofmann, V. (1994).
Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 265-284. A longitudinal study of the relation between
representations of attachment in childhood and
Heller, K., Swindle, R. W., & Dusenbury, L. (1986).
cognitive functioning in childhood and adolescence.
Components of social support processes: Comments
Developmental Psychology, 30, 112-124.
and integration. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 54 (4), 466-470. Jalajas, D. S. (1994). The role of self-esteem in the stress
process: Empirical results from job hunting. Journal of
Herbert, T. B., & Cohen, S. (1993a). Depression and
Applied Social Psychology, 24 (22), 1984-2001.
immunity: A meta-analytic review. Psychological
Bulletin, 113, 472-486. John, O. (1990). The big five-factor taxonomy:
Dimensions of personality in the natural language and
Herbert, T. B., & Cohen, S. (1993b). Stress and immunity in questionnaires. In L. Pervin (Ed.). Handbook of
in humans: A meta-analytic review. Psychosomatic Personality: Theory and research (pp. 66-100). New
Medicine, 55, 364-379. York: Guilford.
Hinde, R. A. (1987). Individuals, relationships, and Kellerman, E., & Smith, M. S. (1986). Crosslinguistic
culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. influence in second language acquisition. Oxford,
Hinde, R.A. & Stevenson-Hinde, J. (Eds) (1988). England: Pergamon Press.
Relationships within families. Oxford: Clarendon Kessler, R. C. (1992). Perceived support and adjustment
Press. to stress: Methodological considerations. In H. O.
Hobfoll, S. E. (1985). Personal and social resources and F. Veiel & U. Baumann (Eds.). The meaning and
the ecology of stress resistance. In P. Shaver (Ed.). measurement of social support (pp. 259-271). New
Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 6, York: Hemisphere.
pp. 265-290). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Kenny, D. T., & Adams, R. (1995). Body shape, gender,
Hobfoll, S. E. (1988). The ecology of stress. Washington, age and adolescent eating attitudes. In D. T. Kenny, &
D. C.: Hemisphere. R. F. S. Job (Eds.). Australia’s adolescents: A health
Hobfoll, S. E. (1989). Conservation of resources: A psychology perspective (pp. 45-52). Armidale, NSW:
new attempt at conceptualising stress. The American University of New England Press.
Psychologist, 44, 513-524. Kenny, D. T., & Waters, B. (1995). Current issues in

Chapter 4, Stress and Health Psychological foundations of stress and coping 47


adolescent mental health. In D. T. Kenny, & R. F. S. Job Developmental organization, stress and illness.
(Eds.). Australia’s adolescents: A health psychology Psychological Science, 1 (5), 316-318.
perspective (pp. 68-88). Armidale, NSW: University of Loftus, E. F., & Klinger, M. R. (1992). Is the unconscious
New England Press. smart or dumb? American Psychologist, 47, 761-765.
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Fisher, L., Ogrocki, P., Stout, Lopez, M. I., & Leon, N. A. (1989). Babies of the
J. C., Speicher, C. E., & Glaser, R. (1987). Marital Earthquake: Follow-up study of their first 15 months.
quality, marital disruption, and immune function. Hillside Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 11 (2), 147-
Psychosomatic Medicine, 49, 13-34. 168.
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Garner, W., Speicher, C. E., Penn, Lyons-Ruth, K. (1996). Attachment relationships among
G. M., Holliday, J., & Glaser, R. (1984). Psychosocial children with aggressive behavior problems: The role
modifiers of immunocompetence in medical students. of disorganized early attachment patterns. Journal of
Psychosomatic Medicine, 46, 7-14. Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64 (1), 64-73.
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Glaser, R., Gravenstein, S., Maccoby, E. E. (1983). Social-emotional development
Malarkey, W. B., & Sheridan, J. (1996). Chronic stress and response to stressors. In N. Garmezy and M. Rutter
alters the immune response to influenza virus vaccine (Eds.). Stress, coping and development in children.
in older adults. Proceedings of the National Academy New York: McGraw-Hill.
of Sciences, 93, 3043-3047.
MacDonald, K. (1995). Evolution, the Five Factor Model,
Kihlstrom, J. F. (1987). The cognitive unconscious. and levels of personality. Journal of Personality, 63
Science, 237, 1445-1452. (3), 525-567.
Kliewer, W., & Sandler, I. N. (1992). Locus of control Maddi, S. (1981). Individual development: Its significance
and self-esteem as moderators of stressor-symptom for stress responsivity and stress adaptation. Personality
relations in children and adolescents. Journal of development. In C. Moore (Ed.). Adolescence and stress
Abnormal Child Psychology, 20 (4), 393-413. (DHHS Publication No. ADM 81-10098). Washington,
Kobasa, S. C. (1979). Stressful life events, personality, DC.: US Government Printing Office, 15-33.
and health: An inquiry into hardiness. Journal of Magnusson, D., & Bergman, L.R. (1988). Individual and
Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1-11. variable-based approaches to longitudinal research
Kobasa, S. C. (1982). The hardy personality: Toward a no early risk factors. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Studies of
social psychology of stress and health. In G. Sanders & psychosocial risk: The power of longitudinal data (pp.
J. Suls (Eds.). Social psychology of health and illness 45-61). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
(pp. 3-32). Hillsdale, N. J.: Erlbaum. Main, M., and Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for
Kobasa, S. C., & Puccetti, M. C. (1983). Personality identifying infants as disorganized / disoriented during
and social resources in stress resistance. Journal of the Ainsworth Strange Situation. In M. Greenberg, D.
Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 839-850. Cicchetti, & E. M. Cummings (Eds.). Attachment in
Kohut, H (1977). The restoration of the self
self. New York: the preschool years: Theory, research and intervention
International Universities Press Inc. (pp. 121-160). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Kreger, D. W. (1995). Self-esteem, stress, and depression Matheny, K. B., Aycock, D. W., & McCarthy, C. J. (1993).
among graduate students. Psychological Reports, 76 Stress in school-aged children and youth. Educational
(1), 345-346. Psychology Review, 5 (2), 109-133.
Lane, C., & Hobfoll, S. E. (1992). How loss affects McCrae, R., & Costa, P. T. Jr. (1990). Personality in
anger and alienates potential supporters. Journal of adulthood. New York: Guilford.
adulthood
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60 (6), 935-942. McGrath, P. (1993). Psychological aspects of pain
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, perception. In N. Schecter, C. Berde, & M. Yaster
and coping. New York: Springer Publishing Co. (Eds.). Pain in infants, children, and adolescents (pp.
LeBaron, S., & Zeltzer, L. (1984). Assessment of acute 39-63). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
pain and anxiety in children and adolescents by self- Medvedova, L. (1995). Personal resources of adaptive
reports, observer reports, and a behavior checklist. coping with stress in adolescents. Studia Psychologica,
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52, 37 (3), 177-179.
729-738. Meyerowitz, B. (1996). Cited in Azur, B., Scientists
Lewis, M. (1988). Psychology of stress: Infants examine cancer patients? fears. APA Monitor, 27 (9),
behavioral and salivary cortisol responses to stress. 32.
Paper presented at the Sixth Biennial International Miller, S. M. (1987). Monitoring and blunting: Validation
Conference on Infant Studies, Washington, DC. of a questionnaire to assess styles of information
Lewis, M., & Thomas, D. (1990). Cortisol release in seeking under threat. Journal of Personality and Social
response to inoculation. Child Development, 61, 50- Psychology, 52 , 345-353.
59. Mills, M., Puckering, C., Pound, A., & Cox, A. (1985).
Lewis, M., Thomas, D., & Worobey, J. (1990). What is it about depressed mothers that influences

Psychological foundations of stress and coping 48 Kenny D.T., 2000


their children’s functioning. In J. Stevenson (Ed.). Rosenbaum, M. (1990). Role of learned resourcefulness
Recent advances in developmental psychopathology. in self-control of health behavior. In M. Rosenbaum
Permagon Press: Oxford. (Ed.). Learned resourcefulness: On coping skills, self-
Monroe, S. M. (1982). Life events assessment: Current control, and adaptive behavior (pp. 3-30). New York:
practices, emerging trends. Clinical Psychology Springer.
Review, 2, 435-453. Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal
Ornstein, A. (1981) Self-pathology in childhood: versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological
Developmental and clinical considerations. Psychiatric Monographs, 80 (1, Whole No. 609).
Clinics of North America, 4(3), 435-453. Rudolph, K. D., Dennig, M. D., Weisz, J. R. (1995).
Patterson, J. M., & McCubbin, H. I. (1987). Adolescent Determinants and consequences of children’s coping
coping style and behaviours: Conceptualisation and in the medical setting: Conceptualization, review, and
measurement. Journal of Adolescence, 10, 163-186. critique. Psychological Bulletin, 118 (3), 328-357.
Rutter, M. (1983). Stress, Coping, and Development:
Pervin, L. (1994). A critical analysis of current trait
Some issues and some questions. In N. Garmezy, &
theory. Psychological Inquiry, 5, 103-113.
M. Rutter (Eds.). Stress, coping and development in
Petrovich, S. B., and Gewitz, J. L. (1985). The attachment children (pp. 1-41). New York: McGraw-Hill.
learning process and its relation to cultural and biological
Rutter, M. (1985). Family and school influences on
evolution: Proximate and ultimate consideration. In M.
cognitive development. In R.A.Hinde, A.N. Perret-
Reite, and T. Field (Eds.). Psychobiology of attachment
Clermont, and J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds), Social
and separation (pp. 51-92). New York: Academic.
relationships and cognitive development. Oxford:
Piaget, J. (1951). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood
childhood. Clarendon Press.
New York: Norton.
Rutter, M. (1986). Psychosocial resilience and protective
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. mechanisms. In D. Cicchetti, K. Neuchterlein, and
New York: International Universities Press. S. Weintraub (Eds.), Risk and protective factors in
Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child
child. the development of psychopathology. Cambridge:
New York: Basic Books. Cambridge University Press.
Piaget, J. (1970). Piaget’s theory, In P. H. Mussen (Ed.). Rutter, M. (1987). Temperament, personality and
Carmichael’s Manual of Child Psychology. New York: personality disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry,
Wiley. 150, 443-458.
Piaget, J. (1972). Intellectual evolution from adolescence Rutter, M. (1989). Psychosocial resilience and protective
to adulthood. Human Development, 15, 1-12. mechanisms. In J. Rolf, A. Master, D. Chichetti, K.
Pless, I. B., & Stein, R. E. K. (1994). Intervention Nuecherlein, & S. Weintraub (Eds.). Risk and protective
research: Lessons from research on children with factors in the development of psychopathology. New
York: Cambridge University Press.
chronic disorders. In R. J. Haggerty (Ed.). Stress, risk
and resilience in children and adolescents: Processes, Rutter, M. (1990). Psychosocial resilience and
mechanisms, and interventions (Chapter 9). Cambridge: protective mechanisms. In J. Rolf, A. S. Masten, D.
Cambridge University Press. Chicchetti, K. H. Neuchterlein, & S. Weintraub (Eds.).
Risk and protective factors in the development of
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., McClearn, G., & Rutter, M.
psychopathology (pp. 181-214). New York: Cambridge
(1997). Behavioral genetics. (3rd ed.). New York: W.
University Press.
H. Freeman.
Rutter, M. L. (1994). Stress research: Accomplishments
Program for Prevention Research (1992). Divorce
and tasks ahead. In R. J. Haggerty (Ed.). Stress, risk
adjustment project documentation. (Available from
and resilience in children and adolescents: Processes,
the Program for Prevention Research, Arizona State
mechanisms, and interventions (pp. 354-385).
University, Tempe, AZ).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Quinton, D., & Rutter, M. (1988). Parenting breakdown: Rutter, M. L. (1997). Nature-nurture integration: The
The making and breaking of inter-generational links. example of antisocial behavior. American Psychologist,
Aldershot: Averbury. 52 (4), 390-398.
Renken, B., Egeland, B., Marvinney, D., Mangelsdorf, S., Sameroff, A.J., Seifer, R., Zax, M. & Barocas, R. (1987).
& Stroufe, L. A. (1989). Early childhood antecedents of Early indicators of developmental risk: Rochester
aggression and passive-withdrawal in early elementary Longitudinal Study. Schizophrenia bulletin, 13, 383-
school. Journal of Personality, 57, 257-281. 394.
Revelle, W. (1995). Personality processes. Annual Review Sandler, I. N., Ramirez, R., & Reynolds, K. (1986).
of Psychology, 46, 295-328. Life stress for children of divorce, bereaved and
Rook, K. S. (1984). The negative side of social interaction: asthmatic children. Poster presented at the American
Impact on psychological well-being. Journal of Psychological Association Convention, Washington,
Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 1097-1108. DC.

Chapter 4, Stress and Health Psychological foundations of stress and coping 49


Sandler, I. N., Tein, J. Y., West, S. G. (1994). Coping, Whelan (Eds.), The childhood environment and adult
stress, and the psychological symptoms of children of disease (pp. 171-188). Chichester: Wiley.
divorce: A cross-sectional and longitudinal study. Child The Consortium on the School-Based Promotion of Social
Development, 65 (6), 1744-1763. Competence. (1994). The school-based promotion
Sarason, I. G., & Sarason, B. R. (1985). Social support: of social competence: Theory, research, practice and
Insights from assessment and experimentation. In I. policy. In R. J. Haggerty, L. R. Sherrod, N. Garmezy,
G. Sarason & B. R. Sarason (Eds.). Social support: and M. Rutter (Eds.). Stress, risk and resilience in
Theory, research, and application (pp. 391-414). The children and adolescents: Processes, mechanisms,
Hague, The Netherlands: Nijhoff. and interventions (Chapter 8). Cambridge: Cambridge
Sarason, B. R., Sarason, I. G., & Pierce, G. R. (1990). University Press.
Traditional views of social support and their impact on Thoits, P. A. (1985). Social support and psychological
assessment. In B. R. Sarason, & G. R. Pierce (Eds.). wellbeing: Theoretical possibilities. In I. G. Sarason &
Social support: An interactional view (pp. 9-25). New B. R. Sarason (Eds.). Social support: Theory, research,
York: Wiley. and applications. The Hague, The Netherlands:
Schacter, D. L. (1992). Understanding implicit memory: Nijhoff.
A cognitive neuroscience approach. American Thoits, P. A. (1986). Social support as coping assistance.
Psychologist, 47, 559-569. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54 (4),
Scarr, S. (1992). Developmental theories for the 1990s: 416-423.
Developmental and individual differences. Child Thomas, S. P., & Groer, M. W. (1986). Relationship of
Development, 63, 1-19. demographic, life-style, and stress variables to blood
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make pressure in adolescents. Nursing Research, 35 (3),
their own environments: A theory of genotype _ 169-172.
environment effects. Child Development, 54, 424-435. Tronick, E. Z., Winn, S., & Morelli, G. A. (1985). Multiple
Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1992). Effects of optimism caretaking in the context of human evolution: Why
on psychological and physical well-being: Theoretical don’t the Efe know the western prescription for child
overview and empirical update. Cognitive Therapy and care? In M. Reite, and T. Field (Eds.). Psychobiology
Research, 16, 201-228. of attachment and separation (pp. 292-322). New
Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1993). On the power York: Academic.
of positive thinking: The benefits of being optimistic. Turner, R. J. (1992). Measuring social support: Issues of
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 26- concept and method. In H. O. F. Veiel & U. Baumann
30. (Eds.). The meaning and measurement of social support
Schwarzer, R. (1994). Optimism, vulnerability, and (pp. 217-233). New York: Hemisphere.
self-beliefs as health-related cognitions: A systematic Vaux, L. (1992). Assessment of social support. In H.
overview. Psychology and Health, 9, 161-180. O. F. Veiel & U. Baumann (Eds.). The meaning and
Seligman, M. E. P. (1991). Learned optimism. New York: measurement of social support (pp. 193-216). New
Alfred A. Knopf. York: Hemisphere.
Shedler, J., Mayman, M., and Manis, M. (1995). The Vega, L., Ollinger, E., Zimmerman, R., & Figueroa, J.
illusion of mental health. American Psychologist, 48 et al (1987). Children’s representation of earthquakes.
(11), 1117-1131. Salud Mental, 10 (1), 66-71.
Shumaker, S. A., & Brownell, A. (1984). Toward a theory Veiel, H. O. F., & Baumann, U. (1992). The many
of social support: Closing conceptual gaps. Journal of meanings of social support. In H. O. F. Veiel & U.
Social Issues, 40, 11-36. Baumann (Eds.). The meaning and measurement of
Sroufe, L. A. & Fleeson, J. (1988). The coherence of social support (pp. 1-12). New York: Hemisphere.
family relationships. In R. A Hinde & J. Stevenson- Wallerstein, J. S., & Blakeslee, S. (1989). Second chances:
Hinde (Eds), Relationships Within Families. Oxford: Men, women, and children a decade after divorce. New
Oxford University Press. York: Ticknor & Fields.
Strentz, T., & Auerbach, S. M. (1988). Adjustment to Weinberger, D. (1990). The construct validity of the
the stress of simulated captivity: Effects of emotion- repressive coping style. In J. L. Singer (Ed.). Repression
focused vs. problem-focused preparation on hostages and dissociation: Implications for personality theory,
differing in locus of control. Journal of Personality psychopathology, and health (pp. 337-385). Chicago:
and Social Psychology, 55, 652-660. University of Chicago Press.
Suls, J., & Fletcher, B. (1986). The relative efficacy of Weisz, J. R., McCabe, M., & Dennig, M. D. (1994).
avoidant and nonavoidant coping strategies: A meta- Primary and secondary control among children
analysis. Health Psychology, 4, 249-288. undergoing medical procedures: Adjustment as a
Suomi, S. J. (1991). Early stress and adult emotional function of coping style. Journal of Consulting and
reactivity in rhesus monkeys. In G. R. Bock & J. Clinical Psychology, 62, 324-332.

Psychological foundations of stress and coping 50 Kenny D.T., 2000


Werner, E. E. (1995). Resilience in development. Current Journal of the American Medical Association, 267,
Directions in Psychological Science, 4 (3), 81-85. 520-524.
Werner, E. E., & Smith, R. S. (1982). Vulnerable but Winnubst, J. A. M., Buunk, B. P., & Marcelissen, F. H.
invincible: A study of resilient children. New York: G. (1988). Social support and stress: Perspectives and
McGraw-Hill. processes. In S. Fisher & J. Reason (Ed.). Handbook
Westen, D. (1990). Psychoanalytic approaches to of life stress, cognition and health (pp. 511-528).
personality. In L. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Chichester, England: John Wiley and Sons.
Theory and research (pp. 21-65). New York: Guilford. Wolkind, S., & Rutter, M. (1985). Separation, loss and
Westen, D. (1994). Toward an integrative model of family relationships. In M. Rutter & L. Hersov (Eds.).
affect regulation: Applications to social-psychological Child psychiatry- Modern approaches. Blackwell:
research. Journal of Personality, 62, 641-667. Oxford.
Westen, D. (1995). A Clinical-Empirical Model of Worobey, J., & Lewis, M. (1989). Individual differences
Personality: Life after the Mischelian Ice Age and the in the reactivity of young infants. Developmental
MEO-Lithic Era. Journal of Personality, 63 (3), 495- Psychology, 25, 663-667.
524.
Younger, J. B. (1991). A theory of mastery. Advances in
White, B. L. & Watts, J. C. (1973). Experience and
Nursing Science, 14 (1), 76-89.
environment: Major influences on the development
of the young child
child. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice- Zeanah, C. H. (1996). Beyond insecurity: A reconceptu-
Hall. alisation of attachment disorders of infancy. Journal of
Williams, R. B., Barefoot, J. C., Califf, R. M., et al. Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64 (1), 42-52.
(1992). Prognostic importance of social and economic Zeidner, M., & Endler, N. S. (Eds.). (1996). Handbook
resources among medically treated patients with of coping: Theory, research, applications. Brisbane :
angiographically documented coronary artery disease. John Wiley and Sons.

Chapter 4, Stress and Health Psychological foundations of stress and coping 51

View publication stats

Вам также может понравиться