Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 10

INTERPERSONAL RELATIONS AND GROUP PROCESSES

Wealth and Happiness Across the World: Material Prosperity Predicts Life
Evaluation, Whereas Psychosocial Prosperity Predicts Positive Feeling

Ed Diener Weiting Ng
University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign and Singapore Institute of Management
The Gallup Organization, Omaha, Nebraska

James Harter and Raksha Arora


The Gallup Organization, Omaha, Nebraska

The Gallup World Poll, the first representative sample of planet Earth, was used to explore the reasons why
happiness is associated with higher income, including the meeting of basic needs, fulfillment of psychological
needs, increasing satisfaction with one’s standard of living, and public goods. Across the globe, the association
of log income with subjective well-being was linear but convex with raw income, indicating the declining
marginal effects of income on subjective well-being. Income was a moderately strong predictor of life
evaluation but a much weaker predictor of positive and negative feelings. Possessing luxury conveniences and
satisfaction with standard of living were also strong predictors of life evaluation. Although the meeting of
basic and psychological needs mediated the effects of income on life evaluation to some degree, the strongest
mediation was provided by standard of living and ownership of conveniences. In contrast, feelings were most
associated with the fulfillment of psychological needs: learning, autonomy, using one’s skills, respect, and the
ability to count on others in an emergency. Thus, two separate types of prosperity— economic and social
psychological— best predict different types of well-being.

Keywords: income, subjective well-being, money, wealth, happiness

Does money increase happiness? Behavioral scientists have Second, we analyzed possible mediators of the relation between
studied this popular question, and several conclusions have income and well-being, which also have been largely ignored in
emerged. For example, people in wealthy nations on average are large-scale surveys. For example, we examined whether the associa-
considerably happier than those in very poor nations, and there is tion of income with well-being is mediated by the fact that wealthy
a smaller but positive association within nations between income people are more likely to have their physical and/or psychological
and happiness (see Diener & Biswas-Diener, 2002, for a review). needs met or whether wealthy people are more satisfied because they
There is much conjecture but little scientific understanding of why have more of their material desires fulfilled.
income and well-being are associated. Another purpose of the present study is to analyze how universal
In this study, we provide initial answers to several fundamental the relation between money and well-being is by determining the
questions about the relations of money and happiness. First, we consistency of the relation across various groups. If the relations
examined whether income is equally related to different types of vary, it would suggest that context and culture can influence the
well-being. Although there are clear demarcations between life relation between income and well-being. For example, is income
evaluations, positive emotions, and negative emotions, past work on more important to well-being in poor nations where basic need
income and well-being has focused mostly on global evaluations of fulfillment is a larger issue?
life, whereas positive and negative feelings have been largely ignored. Fourth, we explored at the societal level whether citizens of
wealthy nations are happier than those of poor countries because
more people have high incomes or because of public resources
Ed Diener, Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana– such as better health facilities and education. The Gallup World
Champaign, and The Gallup Organization, Omaha, Nebraska; Weiting Ng, Poll (GWP) is the first representative survey of the globe. Thus, we
Psychology Programmes, Singapore Institute of Management, Singapore; explored the four questions about income and well-being in a
James Harter and Raksha Arora, The Gallup Organization. diverse and representative sample of the world.
James Harter is an employee and stockholder of The Gallup Organiza-
tion, whose data are used in this article. Types of Well-Being
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Ed Die-
ner, Department of Psychology, University of Illinois, 603 East Daniel In describing types of subjective well-being, Kahneman (1999)
Street, Champaign, IL 61820. E-mail: ediener@uiuc.edu drew a distinction between the online experience of utility and
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2010, Vol. 99, No. 1, 52– 61
© 2010 American Psychological Association 0022-3514/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0018066

52
HAPPINESS AND MONEY 53

global evaluations of life. Whereas broad evaluations such as life two social questions to assess the fulfillment of social psycholog-
satisfaction judgments involve a reflective analysis of a person’s ical needs. Thus, we were able to assess the mediation of both
life, online reports of experience comprise a person’s positive and physical and psychological needs on the income and well-being
negative momentary feelings over time. Supporting this distinc- relation.
tion, Lucas, Diener, and Suh (1996) found that life satisfaction, Another reason that income might be related to well-being is
pleasant emotions, and unpleasant emotions formed distinct fac- that people learn to desire material goods, and the fulfillment of
tors. Therefore, in the present study, we analyze how various desires leads to feelings of well-being, regardless of whether needs
potential mediators of the income–well-being relation are associ- are met. Emmons (1986) found that achieving goals leads to higher
ated with each form of subjective well-being. In this study, we subjective well-being. If people want money because of social
examined three types of well-being that were included in the learning, the influence of advertising, and so forth, then obtaining
GWP: Cantril’s (1965) Self-Anchoring Striving Scale, which as- it could be linked to feelings of well-being.
sesses global life evaluations; positive feelings (enjoyment and The possibility of goal fulfillment as a mediator is suggested by
smiling or laughing); and negative feelings (anger, sadness, worry, the findings of Biswas-Diener, Vittersø, and Diener (2005), who
and depression). On the basis of Scitovsky (1978), we predicted found that some groups without much income have reasonably
that life satisfaction might be more strongly related to income, high subjective well-being. The researchers found that the Maasai
positive feelings to social psychological rewards, and negative report high life satisfaction even though they live in dung houses
emotions inversely to the fulfillment of basic needs. without running water, electricity, or modern conveniences. Sim-
ilarly, the Amish are reasonably happy even though they live
Mediators of the Income and Well-Being Relation without electricity and automobiles. In addition, Biswas-Diener
and Diener (2001) showed that even some extremely poor people
Why is income associated with subjective well-being? Although in the slums of Calcutta report subjective well-being above neutral.
many studies have been conducted that establish the correlation These findings might be explained by the fact that the Amish and
between income and subjective well-being, little research has traditional Maasai have relatively low material aspirations, al-
analyzed the mediating mechanisms for this relation. In the current though their basic material needs are largely met. Conversely,
article, we examine the fulfillment of physical and psychological Graham and Pettinato (2002) found that people with rapidly rising
needs as possible mediators, as well as the fulfillment of material incomes sometimes feel frustrated because their aspirations have
aspirations. Is it the fulfillment of basic needs such as the need for outpaced their incomes. These findings suggest that it may not be
food that underlies the income and well-being association, or is it only the fulfillment of actual needs that connects income to well-
learned desires for material goods that cause the relationship? being, but it could be that people’s material desires are important
Many people who write about money and well-being implicitly as well. Thus, we analyze whether fulfilling goals versus needs is
assume that the relation exists because income allows people to most associated with well-being, with which forms of well-being,
fulfill their physical needs for food and shelter and, perhaps to and whether they play a mediational role between income and
some degree, their psychological needs as well, for example, for well-being.
respect. Previous researchers suggested that the stronger associa- In this study, we had several proxy measures to assess fulfill-
tion between money and well-being at lower income levels indi- ment of material desires. There was a measure of whether people
cates the importance of basic needs to the income and well-being felt satisfied with their standard of living. If respondents’ satisfac-
relation (Howell & Howell, 2008). This interpretation is consistent tion with their standard of living predicts well-being beyond the
with Maslow’s (1943) theory that basic biological needs are pre- effects of need fulfillment, it will suggest that desires are an
potent in that they emerge first in people’s motivations. Thus, we important mediator. Second, we had measures of whether people
expected that the fulfillment of basic needs might increase well- own luxury conveniences that are widely desired but not necessary
being, although lack of fulfillment might be most associated with for survival, such as televisions, computers, and the Internet. If we
negative emotions. The GWP included measures of not having find that possessing these items predicts well-being, it will suggest
enough money for food and for shelter and therefore allowed us to that desires can influence well-being beyond the fulfillment of
examine the path from income through basic needs to the various basic needs.
forms of subjective well-being. In addition to the effects of household income, we examined the
In recent decades, a number of theories have fleshed out psy- impact of people’s relative income positions within their societies.
chological needs beyond basic physical ones. For example, Ryan A potential reason for the income and happiness association is that
and Deci (2000) argued that there are three universal psychological money might bestow status and respect on people. Earning more
needs—for autonomy, competence, and social relationships—and money than others could have value above and beyond the goods
Ryff (1989) suggested six psychological needs—for self- that money can buy because a person’s relative position regarding
acceptance, positive relations with others, autonomy, environmen- money might be a cause of status. Furthermore, people might
tal mastery, purpose in life, and personal growth. The GWP evaluate their material lives largely by judging how they compare
included a number of measures reflecting psychological needs. In with those around them. In the GWP, we analyzed whether relative
the social realm, the GWP asked respondents whether they feel income—income compared with the income of others in one’s
respected and whether they have family and friends they can count own society—predicts well-being beyond the effects of absolute
on in the case of an emergency. In the psychological domain, the income.
GWP included questions about feeling free to choose one’s daily Another purpose of the present study was to analyze how
activities, learning new things, and having opportunities to do consistent the relation between money and well-being is by deter-
“what one does best.” We combined these three questions with the mining the consistency of the association across various groups.
54 DIENER, NG, HARTER, AND ARORA

For example, we examined the relation between income and well- positive feelings were averaged into a positive feelings score, and
being in poor versus wealthy nations, predicting that the associa- “yes” responses to the four negative feelings were averaged into a
tion would be stronger in poor nations. negative feelings score. Thus, scores could vary from 0 to 1.0 for
feelings. One advantage of using measures of emotions during the
Why Is Mean Well-Being Higher in Wealthy Nations? previous day is that it reduces the extent of memory bias. Thus, the
reports are more likely to be anchored in actual experience than are
Whereas correlations of mean income and mean well-being are reports of long-time periods or reports of emotions in general
often .70 –.80 across countries, associations within nations are (Robinson & Clore, 2002). However, anchoring the reports on a
often closer to .20 (Diener & Biswas-Diener, 2002). One plausible single day will introduce random error into the measures in that
explanation is that there is much more error and random variation short-term fluctuating influences are likely to be related to a single
in the individual figures, whereas extraneous factors are averaged day’s feelings. Thus, we expected that the correlations for the
out when societal figures are analyzed. For example, individual affect measures would be lower than those for the evaluation
differences might largely be averaged out with societal correla- measure.
tions, making the error term smaller. An alternate explanation is
that some nations have societal or public goods that, when it comes
Economic Predictors
to well-being, are more important than individual income. We are
able to determine the effects of national wealth after first control- Various measures were selected to assess economic conditions.
ling for the fulfillment of needs, the ownership of modern conve- For individuals, annual household income was used as an indicator
niences, and individual income. Thus, we can determine whether of personal income. Because of the diminishing marginal utility of
there is a societal effect after controlling for individual consump- money, the logarithm (log 10) of reported household income was
tion and income. used. We created another income measure by adjusting household
income for household size. However, household size data were not
Method available for many nations and individuals, thus the adjusted
measure was dropped so as not to lose a large portion of the
sample. Preliminary analyses with both measures showed similar
Sample
patterns of findings. Because the effects of income may not only be
The Gallup Organization conducted representative surveys of due to the absolute amount of money that one has but might also
132 countries from 2005 to 2006. These nations include about 96% be due to one’s relative standing within the nation, we also
of the world’s population and varied greatly in terms of economic calculated each respondent’s relative income by standardizing
development, political structure and stability, and culture. A few incomes within nation.
nations such as North Korea were not accessible to the poll, and A measure assessing the modern luxury household conve-
most small nations such as Andorra and Vanuatu were not in- niences that respondents possessed was derived to reflect the
cluded. There was representative sampling of the entire adult fulfillment of material desires that go beyond basic needs. Respon-
population within each nation. In wealthy nations, this was dents were asked whether their home has a television, a computer,
achieved through telephone surveys based on random-digit dial- and access to the Internet (1 ⫽ yes, 0 ⫽ no). The number of “yes”
ing; in poorer nations, where telephones are less ubiquitous, this responses to the three items was averaged to give an overall score.
was accomplished by door-to-door interviews, with residences Respondents were also asked whether there were times in the past
selected randomly within randomly selected geographical sam- year when they did not have enough money for food or for shelter.
pling units. The “yes” responses to the two items were averaged, yielding an
The final sample size for the present study consisted of a maximum indicator for unfulfilled basic needs. Satisfaction with standard of
of 136,839 respondents (aged 15 years or older); the mean sample size living also was reported on a yes–no scale.
of each nation was 1,061 respondents. The numbers of individuals and National wealth was assessed using the 2005–2007 per capita
nations for specific analyses were often lower because of missing gross domestic product (GDP; in purchasing power parity) data of
data, although the numbers were always very large. Respondents were each nation in constant 2,000 international dollars, computed by
weighted within nations to further refine the samples to represent the the World Bank (2010). The logarithm of the GDP data was then
correct demographic proportions within societies. taken in log 10 units, using a common method of economists,
which has the effect of compressing scores as one moves progres-
Subjective Well-Being sively up the income ladder.

Three types of well-being were selected from the survey to Social Psychological Predictors
represent it. The global life evaluation measure, Cantril’s (1965)
Self-Anchoring Striving Scale, asked respondents to evaluate their Five additional items from the survey were included as social
current life on a ladder scale, which ranged from 0 (worst possible psychological measures. As an indicator of respect, respondents
life) to 10 (best possible life). Questions about positive feelings and reported whether they were treated with respect on the previous
negative feelings assessed the experience of positive and negative day (1 ⫽ yes, 0 ⫽ no). Another question inquired as to whether the
emotions “yesterday.” Respondents were asked whether they ex- respondent had family or friends he or she could count on in an
perienced certain feelings (positive ⫽ enjoyment and smiling or emergency. The questions used to assess the fulfillment of psy-
laughing; negative ⫽ worry, sadness, depression, and anger) a lot chological needs were all asked in reference to “yesterday”:
in the previous day (1 ⫽ yes, 0 ⫽ no). “Yes” responses to the two learned something new, did what you do best, and chose how your
HAPPINESS AND MONEY 55

time was spent. We also examined the number of hours that respondents did not answer this question. In terms of residential
participants worked the previous day, on a scale that could range location, 25.5% were in a rural area, 30.4% lived in a village,
from zero to 24, to analyze whether long work hours reduces the 31.4% lived in a city, and 8.6% lived in a suburb; data were
positive association between income and well-being. missing regarding the residential locations of the rest of the re-
The questionnaire was translated into the major languages of spondents. Thus, 58.3% of the sample for whom we had residential
each country. The translation process started with an English, a data lived rurally or in a village. In terms of education, data were
French, or a Spanish version, depending on the region. A translator available for only 19.6% of the respondents; of these, 25.9% had
who was proficient in the original and target languages translated a primary education or less, 50.0% had some secondary and/or
the survey into the target language. A second translator reviewed some tertiary education, and 19.6% had four or more years of
the language version against the original version and recom- tertiary education.
mended refinements. The correlations between well-being and its predictors are
Although many of the measures from the survey are dichoto- shown in Table 2. Because of the very large sample sizes, all of the
mous items, this type of measure is quicker for use in very large correlations are significantly different from zero, and most differ
surveys. Furthermore, as the survey is administered in many na- significantly from the other correlations in the table. We found that
tions across the world, dichotomous items are less likely to be income was positively associated with Self-Anchoring Striving
influenced by number-use response sets across nations. The large Scale scores and positive feelings and negatively associated with
number of respondents ensures that individual reliability is not a negative emotions. Ownership of luxury conveniences and satis-
major concern and that the results can be interpreted, although the faction with standard of living showed patterns of relations with
dichotomous items are likely to reduce the size of correlations well-being similar to that of income with well-being, whereas
compared with correlations of data gathered using scales with unmet basic needs showed an inverse relation with well-being. As
more response options. Unreliability and translation discrepancies shown, income was much more strongly associated with the Self-
would serve to reduce the effects we found rather than enhance Anchoring Striving Scale score than with the other measures ( p ⬍
them, especially in light of the very large number of nations and .001). For the Self-Anchoring Striving Scale, the strongest corre-
respondents included. Therefore, our estimates might be conser- lates were income and national income, satisfaction with standard
vative. of living, and ownership of luxury conveniences, whereas for
feelings, the fulfillment of psychological needs was the strongest
Results predictor. Satisfaction with standard of living was a significantly
stronger predictor of life evaluation than of feelings, whereas
The basic descriptive statistics for the well-being variables and psychological need fulfillment significantly more strongly pre-
predictors are shown in Table 1. As can be seen, the average dicted positive feelings than evaluations of life. It is interesting that
person in the world was a relatively happy respondent who judged societal income predicted life evaluations as well as individual
his or her current life to be slightly above neutral in the balance income. The number of hours worked did not have a sizeable
between positive and negative feelings, experienced frequent pos- association with individual well-being and is not included in the
itive feelings, and infrequently felt negative feelings. Although the remaining analyses.
majority of people had their psychological needs met to some Another approach to understanding the relation between income
extent, basic needs were unmet in about one quarter of respon- and well-being is to examine the regression slopes between the
dents. Women made up 51.1% of the sample and men 48.9%, with two, and we present these in Figure 1. Each of the three well-being
a small number not identified. The marital status of the samples measures is plotted against log income categories, and for both
was 49.3% married, 30.6% single, 4.9% divorced or separated, feelings and life evaluations, the range shown is approximately
6.1% widowed, and 2.1% in domestic partnership; the remaining half of the full range of the respective scales. As can be seen, the
slope is much steeper for income and the Self-Anchoring Striving
Scale than for income and the two measures of feelings. Thus, not
Table 1 only is there a higher correlation for the Self-Anchoring Striving
Descriptive Statistics for the Sample Scale and income, but variations in income are associated with
change across a wider portion of the possible range of scores.
Statistic M SD

Log household income 3.82 0.61 Strength of the Predictors


Cantril’s Self-Anchoring Striving Scale 5.36 2.24
Positive feelings 71 39 To explore the associations more systematically, we conducted
Negative feelings 23 30 hierarchical linear regression analyses separately on each of the
Basic needs unmet 26 37 three types of well-being, and the results are shown in Tables 3
Psychological needs met 69 28 and 4. In Table 3, we present the amount of variance that was
Luxury conveniences 40 34
Satisfaction with standard of living 59 49 accounted for by each predictor as it was added to the linear model,
Free time 4.2 4.5 as well as the total amount of variance predicted at each step. The
variance figures give a clear picture of the power of the predictors.
Note. Log income is household income from the Gallup World Poll, The amount of variance predicted is especially helpful in this case,
whereas national per capita income comes from the World Bank (2010).
Free time is in hours, and Cantril’s Self-Anchoring Striving Scale is a 0 –10
where virtually all statistics are significantly different because of
scale. The other figures represent first averaging the items composing the the large sample sizes. Luxury conveniences and satisfaction with
scale, then averaging all respondents, and finally multiplying by 100. standard of living were added at the same step because they were
56 DIENER, NG, HARTER, AND ARORA

Table 2 all of the well-being variables but was never the most powerful
Zero-Order Correlations Between Types of Well-Being explanatory variable.
and Predictors Table 4 presents the outcome of regression analyses when all
predictors were entered together. For the Self-Anchoring Striving
Predictor Striving Scale PF NF Scale score, national income and satisfaction with standard of
Log household income .44 .17 ⫺.09 living were the strongest predictors when all were entered together,
Relative income (within-nations) .21 .11 ⫺.11 whereas for feelings, the psychological needs fulfillment index
National log GDP per capita .44 .10 ⫺.03 was by far the strongest predictor. Relative income was significant
Basic needs unmet ⫺.32 ⫺.16 .19 for the Self-Anchoring Striving Scale and negative feelings but not
Psychological needs met .25 .45 ⫺.28
for positive feelings. Although life evaluation was significantly
Luxury conveniences .39 .11 ⫺.05
Satisfaction with standard of living .40 .24 ⫺.20 associated with relative income within one’s society, other predic-
Work hours .03 ⫺.02 .03 tors were stronger. The strong association of national income with
Positive feelings .23 — well-being even with many individual resources entered suggests
Negative feelings ⫺.19 ⫺.38 — the importance of societal circumstances for people’s well-being.
Note. PF ⫽ positive feelings; NF ⫽ negative feelings; GDP ⫽ gross The substantial correlation between the log income of individ-
domestic product. For all correlations, p ⬍ .001. uals and their Self-Anchoring Striving Scale scores across a large
number of nations implies that a cross-societal standard is being
used by respondents, especially in light of the fact that the effect
persisted for two of the three well-being variables even when
used together to reflect the fulfillment of material desires. Overall, within-nation income was controlled. It is striking how large the
negative feelings can be predicted least and life evaluation best, effect of societal income was, even when individual income was in
with positive feelings intermediate between them. For the Self- the equation. This suggests that people’s satisfaction with life is
Anchoring Striving Scale, the step where luxury conveniences and strongly influenced by the general circumstances of their societies.
satisfaction with standard of living were added accounted for by
far the most variance, although both physical and psychological
Consistency of Associations
needs, as well as income and national income, added additional
variance. In contrast, for both positive and negative feelings, the In Table 5, we present the correlations of income and well-being
step with psychological needs added most to the prediction. In- for various categories of respondents. The correlations for income
deed, for positive feelings, the social psychological needs ac- and life evaluations were relatively consistent across groups, vary-
counted for the vast majority of the explainable variance. Basic ing from a low of .31 to a high of .47. Similarly, there was a
needs fulfillment accounted for a nontrivial amount of variance for moderately strong association in virtually all groups between pos-

8.0 95

7.5 90

85
7.0
PF and No-NF (% of people)

80
6.5

75
Current Ladder

6.0
70
5.5
65
5.0
Ladder
60
PF
4.5
No NF 55

4.0 PF
50
No NF
3.5 Ladder 45

3.0 40
2.60 2.76 2.92 3.08 3.24 3.40 3.56 3.72 3.88 4.04 4.20 4.36 4.52 4.68 4.84 5.00

Log Income

Figure 1. Plots of log income versus mean subjective well-being, as well as the best-fit linear trend lines, for
each type of well-being. PF ⫽ positive feelings; NF ⫽ negative feelings.
HAPPINESS AND MONEY 57

Table 3
Variance Predicted in Hierarchical Regression Analyses by Potential Mediators

Cantril’s Self-Anchoring
Striving Scale Positive feelings Negative feelings

Predictors R 2
⌬R 2
p R 2
⌬R2
p R 2
⌬R2 p

Step 1: Basic needs unmet .088 .088 .001 .029 .029 .001 .035 .035 .001
Step 2: Add psychological needs .125 .037 .001 .213 .184 .001 .091 .056 .001
Step 3: Add conveniences and satisfaction
with standard of living .265 .140 .001 .223 .010 .001 .099 .007 .001
Step 4: Add log income .288 .023 .001 .224 .001 .001 .099 .000 .031
Step 5: Add comparative income .288 .000 .001 .224 .000 .002 .102 .003 .001
Step 6: Add national income (log GDP/capita) .305 .016 .001 .225 .001 .001 .104 .002 .001

Note. R2 values are adjusted for the number of predictors. GDP ⫽ gross domestic product.

itive feelings and social psychological resources, varying from .39 Prosperity of Nations
to .47. Income accounted for 19% of the variance in life evaluation
but only 3% of the variance in positive feelings, for which social Does the prosperity of nations differ depending on what type of
psychological wealth accounted for 20% of the variance. quality-of-life outcome is considered? In Table 6, we present the
The enormous sample sizes result in many small differences rank order for selected nations of the world. Whereas Northern
between correlations being significant. However, several of the European and Anglo societies are currently most successful in the
differences are systematic and account for nontrivial amounts of economic area, Latin societies appear to be relatively high in social
variance. For instance, correlations for the predictors of well-being psychological well-being. Although certain nations such as Den-
were higher for less educated and older respondents compared mark achieve consistently high scores across measures, no nation
with the corresponding correlations for more educated and is happiest in the sense of being first on all measures of prosperity
younger respondents. In contrast, there were virtually no differ- and well-being. Sierra Leone’s scores were consistently low, but
ences in the correlations for men and women. But the pattern of other nations showed divergent rankings across the measures. For
correlations is consistent, with income substantially predicting life instance, Russia and South Korea are substantially lower in social
evaluations in every group and social psychological needs sub- psychological prosperity and in positive feelings than they are in
stantially predicting positive feelings in every group. income. In contrast, Costa Rica ranks much higher in social

Table 4
Hierarchical Regression Analyses at Step 6 With All Predictors

Predictors B SE ␤ t p

Cantril’s Self-Anchoring Striving Scale


Basic needs unmet ⫺0.47 0.024 ⫺.077 ⫺19.53 .001
Psychological needs met 0.99 0.034 .110 29.53 .001
Luxury conveniences 0.63 0.034 .095 18.50 .001
Satisfaction with standard of living 0.94 0.018 .208 52.42 .001
Log income 0.25 0.025 .066 9.76 .001
Relative income 0.20 0.012 .089 17.01 .001
National income (log GDP/capita) 1.01 0.028 .221 35.73 .001
Positive feelings
Basic needs unmet ⫺0.059 0.004 ⫺.057 ⫺13.77 .001
Psychological needs met 0.625 0.006 .409 104.18 .001
Luxury conveniences 0.018 0.006 .016 3.04 .002
Satisfaction with standard of living 0.074 0.003 .096 23.02 .001
Log income 0.047 0.005 .074 10.40 .001
Relative income ⫺0.004 0.002 ⫺.010 ⫺1.74 ns
National income (log GDP/capita) ⫺0.039 0.005 ⫺.051 ⫺7.79 .001
Negative feelings
Basic needs unmet 0.109 0.004 .138 30.75 .001
Psychological needs met ⫺0.266 0.005 ⫺.225 ⫺53.27 .001
Luxury conveniences ⫺0.002 0.005 ⫺.002 ⫺0.34 ns
Satisfaction with standard of living ⫺0.056 0.003 ⫺.094 ⫺20.87 .001
Log income 0.001 0.004 .003 0.38 ns
Relative income ⫺0.009 0.002 ⫺.030 ⫺5.14 .001
National income (log GDP/capita) 0.047 0.004 .079 11.27 .001

Note. GDP ⫽ gross domestic product.


58 DIENER, NG, HARTER, AND ARORA

Table 5
Potential Moderators in Predicting Well-Being (Unstandardized Regression Slopes)

Log income predicting


Positive feelings and social Negative feelings and unmet
Moderator Striving Scale PF NF psychological needs basic needs

Entire sample .44 .17 ⫺.09 .45 .19


Residential area
Urban .39 .16 ⫺.12 .44 .21
Rural .44 .17 ⫺.08 .46 .18
Sex
Male .44 .16 ⫺.09 .45 .19
Female .44 .18 ⫺.08 .45 .19
Education
Primary only .45 .26 ⫺.20 .48 .26
4⫹ years tertiary .40 .12 ⫺.11 .39 .20
Age (in years)
Early adult (15–25) .39 .14 ⫺.04 .42 .16
Young (26–40) .45 .18 ⫺.07 .45 .18
Middle age (41–55) .47 .17 ⫺.11 .46 .19
Young-old (56–70) .45 .22 ⫺.17 .47 .23
Old-old (71–99⫹) .45 .28 ⫺.22 .46 .27
National income (GDP/capita)
⬍10,000 .31 .17 ⫺.08 .45 .18
ⱖ10,000 .40 .16 ⫺.13 .39 .18

Note. Income is reported in international dollars. PF ⫽ positive feelings; NF ⫽ negative feelings; GDP ⫽ gross domestic product.

psychological prosperity and positive feelings than it does on well-being of a nation with a single score, whether it be income or
income. In general, the former Soviet Union bloc nations have life satisfaction.
greater economic than social psychological prosperity, whereas
Central American and certain African nations have greater social Discussion
psychological than economic prosperity. Discrepancies between
positive and negative feelings can also be seen in the table, for We addressed several questions in this study and uncovered
example, in the rankings of Japan, Nepal, and India. The United relatively unequivocal answers. First, we found that income does
States was at the top in income but ranked below the middle of the indeed have a stronger association with some types of well-being
list in negative feelings. Thus, it is oversimplified to capture the than with others. Namely, the relation of income and well-being

Table 6
Rankings of Selected Nations on Types of Prosperity (Out of 89)

Social psychological Self-Anchoring Low negative


Nation GDP/capita prosperity Striving Scale Positive feelings feelings

United States 1 19 16 26 49
Denmark 5 13 1 7 1
The Netherlands 7 36 4 3 26
Japan 14 50 24 44 6
Italy 18 33 20 67 63
Israel 20 56 11 61 64
New Zealand 22 12 9 1 21
South Korea 24 83 38 58 77
South Africa 35 42 53 29 44
Russia 36 72 56 79 42
Mexico 39 22 23 17 28
Costa Rica 41 6 18 4 38
Indonesia 59 63 57 24 43
India 61 85 46 63 22
Ghana 68 51 70 68 20
Nepal 76 88 69 50 10
Sierra Leone 87 80 87 87 86
Tanzania 89 58 86 52 32

Note. GDP ⫽ gross domestic product.


HAPPINESS AND MONEY 59

was strong for life evaluations but weaker for feelings. These social relationships and learning new things predict positive feel-
findings are consistent with the conclusions of Diener, Kahneman, ings. In Berridge’s (2004) terminology, money might have more to
Tov, and Arora (2009), who found that long-term income changes do with people having what they want than liking what they have,
are more related to changes in life evaluations than to changes in and social psychological prosperity produces the feelings they like.
positive and negative feelings. We found the strongest mediation The correlations we found for income and life evaluations were
between income and life evaluations by satisfaction with standard larger than in many past studies. One reason for these larger
of living. There were weaker income pathways for all of our associations is that our sample encompassed a much wider range
mediating variables for feelings, in part because feelings were not of income and included more poor and rural nations than did
highly associated with income in the first place. Instead, feelings previous studies. The correlations we found are likely to be larger
were most strongly associated with social psychological prosper- than those found within populations with a more restricted range of
ity. In addition, we found that national income predicted life income. A second possible reason is that we examined the income
evaluations even after controlling for individual income, indicating associations separately for the various types of well-being. Our
that there are societal circumstances related to income that might results showed that the Self-Anchoring Striving Scale evaluation
substantially influence people’s life satisfaction. of life yielded the strongest associations with income, whereas
We started with the question of why money is associated with affective assessments of well-being yielded smaller correlations. In
well-being. In retrospect, it is difficult to imagine it otherwise for past studies, measures might have been used that were about
life evaluations in the modern world of market economies. Money feelings or that mixed feelings and judgments (e.g., reports of
is an object that many or most people desire and pursue during the happiness).
majority of their waking hours. Thus, it would be surprising if Might the Self-Anchoring Striving Scale score produce spuri-
success at this pursuit had no influence whatsoever when people ously high correlations with income compared with other mea-
are asked to evaluate their lives. Thus, the more interesting ques- sures? There are several arguments against this possibility. First,
tions might be why income is so weakly associated with feelings Helliwell, Barrington-Leigh, Harris, and Huang (2009) found that
of well-being and ill-being and why social psychological success is income correlated similarly with a traditional life satisfaction
so predictive of positive feelings. measure. Second, even if the Self-Anchoring Striving Scale’s
One possibility is that there is reverse causality in terms of correlation with income were higher than its correlation with other
positive feelings leading to social psychological success. People measures of life evaluation, it would not mean that this is spurious.
and societies with frequent positive feelings might develop stron- Instead, it would point to the fact that different measures might
ger social relationships, in the manner described by Fredrickson prime somewhat different information to be evaluated, just as
and Joiner (2002). Similarly, people who are enjoying themselves recent events might prime certain information.
might feel that they are autonomous, learning new things, and It is possible that response biases influenced our results, for
doing their best. Indeed, Fredrickson’s broaden-and-build model example, respondents might differ by culture or region in their
suggests that it is when people feel positive that they are most tendency to avoid scale extremes. Although this might have had
likely to develop resources for the future, for example, by learning some influence, we have reason to believe that the majority of the
new things and by strengthening social ties. We cannot, however, patterns we observed are substantive. For example, the patterns
discount the other causal direction going from social psychological repeat across cultures and groups, as well as across many lan-
resources to feelings. For example, Pavot, Diener, and Fujita guages. Several of our predictors are subjective, such as having
(1990) found that people experience more positive feelings on friends to count on and learning new things, meaning that several
average when they are with others than when alone. Csikszentmi- interpretations are possible for associations using these variables.
halyi’s (1990) theory of flow suggests that people are pleasantly Nevertheless, Diener et al. (2009) found similar patterns at the
engaged when they are using their skills in activities that are national level using objective measures taken from sources such as
optimally challenging and require their skills. Ryff (1989) and the World Bank. Just as we found that the GDP per capita predictor
Ryan and Deci (2000) suggested that there are basic and universal we used here, which comes from outside our data set and is not
human psychological needs, and these needs are highly associated subject to response artifacts from the GWP, predicts our well-
with the social psychological variables that predicted well-being in being variables quite well, other studies we have conducted on
our study. Learning new things, using one’s skills, feeling re- societal well-being show that variables such as the rated corruption
spected, and having others to count on are reflective of meeting of nations also predicts well-being. A concern is that because
needs, and thus it is not unexpected that fulfilling these needs questions about feelings were focused on “yesterday,” believing
would enhance positive feelings. that one learned things, is respected, and so forth could come from
Tibor Scitovsky (1978) in The Joyless Economy offered several short-term moods rather than long-term circumstances. Further-
ideas that help to explain our pattern of findings. He differentiated more, the psychosocial variables and positive and negative feelings
comfort and pleasure. It may be that comforts increase life eval- were both measured on yes–no formats, and this might have
uations whereas pleasures increase reports of positive feelings. increased the association between them. Thus, it will be important
Comfort comes from having one’s needs and desires continuously for future researchers to assess feelings over longer periods of time
fulfilled, whereas pleasures come from fulfilling unmet needs and so that short-term and long-term processes can be distinguished; it
from stimulating and challenging activities. One source of pleasure will also be helpful for researchers to use varying response for-
according to Scitovsky is social stimulation, which he suggested mats.
lies largely outside the realm of economics. Novelty and learning Can other research artifacts explain our findings? It is unlikely
can be sources of pleasure too. Thus, Scitovsky’s reasoning is in that item-order priming could have led to our results because the
accord with our findings that wealth predicts life satisfaction, and income questions came at the end of the survey, far removed from
60 DIENER, NG, HARTER, AND ARORA

the subjective well-being questions, which were toward the begin- people do not think of their income in these terms. The important
ning of the interview. It is also unlikely that the results are due to point is to interpret the log findings correctly.
language translation differences, because the survey was adminis- Log income is preferred by economists because it is truer to the
tered in a very large number of diverse languages. In addition, the statistical requirements of linear statistics. An additional reason
wording used in the survey was carefully developed in each that log data are desirable is that they reflect the psychological fact
location in close collaboration with local researchers. that the perception of stimuli is most frequently based on the log
In recent years, a number of authors have questioned whether of the intensity of those stimuli. Increases in light, sound, weight,
the effects of income or subjective well-being are absolute or and so forth produce increases in perception that are generally in
relative (e.g., Clark, Frijters, & Shields, 2008; Frank, 2005; Hsee, line with log increases in the stimuli, and this is likely to be true
Yang, Li, & Shen, 2009; Johnson & Krueger, 2006). We found of income as well. After all, a few dollars will not mean the same
effects of income relative to others in people’s own nations, but thing to a millionaire and to a pauper, any more than a candle will
they were smaller than the associations for absolute income. This produce the same perception of light on a bright beach and in a
might be due to the fact that people compare themselves with local dark cave.
others or those in their profession but do not often make nation- The difficulty for psychologists in using log income comes at
wide comparisons. More probably, it may be that the standard for the level of communication with the public, because people often
material well-being is now largely worldwide rather than defined do not consider their income changes in these terms. For example,
within nations. This is consistent with the conclusions of Diener et people would not think of an increase from $10,000 to $100,000
al. (2009), who found steeper income and well-being slopes be- when they think of their incomes rising, although this is only one
tween nations than within them. Thus, our findings do not dem- unit increase on the log 10 scale. The important points to keep in
onstrate that people do not use social comparison to evaluate their mind are to correctly interpret the conclusions and to use the right
incomes; they only suggest that the social comparisons are not form of description when scales of either type are used.
heavily focused at the nation level. Our findings clearly indicate The present findings on the relation of income to different types
that people evaluate their incomes relative to a global standard. If of well-being might partly resolve the Easterlin paradox debate, in
there was no global standard, we could not have found that which some maintain that there has been no change in well-being
as nations have become wealthier over time (e.g., Easterlin, 1995,
people’s life evaluations correlated so highly with their incomes
2005; Myers, 2000), whereas others have claimed the opposite
across nations with very diverse incomes. Thus, social comparison
(e.g., Stevenson & Wolfers, 2008; Veenhoven & Hagerty, 2006).
might be a significant force, but the within-nation level of analysis
If income has a low association with certain forms of well-being,
might not strongly capture these effects.
such as feelings, it would be likely that increases in income would
The analyses revealed that the income and well-being correla-
have little effect on them. However, improvements in other types
tions were fairly consistent across groups. An unexpected finding
of factors such as the social environment might influence these
was that the relation between income and life evaluations was
forms of well-being. This is an important avenue for future re-
slightly stronger in wealthy nations than in poor ones, because this
search.
is counter to the idea that money is most important when it helps
meet basic needs. However, this finding is in accord with the idea
that the income and life evaluation association rests on the fulfill- Take-Home Message
ment of material aspirations. This was supported by the finding Contrary to both those who say money is not associated with
that satisfaction with standard of living and the overall evaluation happiness and those who say that it is extremely important, we
of life were more highly correlated in wealthy than in economi- found that money is much more related to some forms of well-
cally underdeveloped nations. being than it is to others. Income is most strongly associated with
Because of the declining marginal utility of money, differences the life evaluation form of well-being, which is a reflective judg-
in raw absolute income produce larger effects for the poor than for ment on people’s lives compared with what they want them to be.
the rich. Many past studies in psychology used raw income as a Although statistically significant, the association of income with
measure. However, an increase from $10,000 to $20,000 will have positive and negative feelings was modest. Furthermore, we found
more of an impact than an increase from $80,000 to $90,000. In that societal income has a substantial influence on life evaluations
using log income as our measure, we examined increases in terms beyond the effects of personal income, indicating that it is very
of the percentage of increase at different levels of income. Thus, an desirable for life satisfaction to live in an economically developed
increase from $10,000 to $20,000 dollars is equivalent in log terms nation. However, we also discovered that social psychological
to an increase from $80,000 to $160,000 —in both cases a dou- prosperity is very important to positive feelings. Some nations that
bling of one’s income. Viewed from this perspective, income has do well in economic terms do only modestly well in social psy-
an effect on life evaluations even in wealthy nations, although the chological prosperity, and some nations that rank in the middle in
raw amount of income required to produce noticeable change is economic development are stars when it comes to social psycho-
much larger. logical prosperity.
Which is the correct way to analyze income data, given that If replicated, our findings have profound implications for both
economists prefer log income and psychologists prefer raw in- psychological theories of well-being and for societal policies. At
come? In our data, the log income correlations were higher than the theoretical level, our results indicate that different types of
those measuring raw income. Thus, one can argue for smaller or well-being can be influenced by very different predictors. It is
larger income effects depending on which scaling of income is important to note that social psychological well-being is shown to
used. Some argue that log income effects are misleading because be an important correlate of feelings across the globe. At the policy
HAPPINESS AND MONEY 61

level, our findings indicate that more than money is needed for and subjective well-being in new market economies. Journal of Devel-
quality of life, and the social psychological forms of prosperity opment Studies, 38, 100 –140.
correlate only moderately with economic development. This Helliwell, J. F., Barrington-Leigh, C., Harris, A., & Huang, H. (2009).
means that societies must pay careful attention to social and International evidence on the social context of well-being. In E. Diener,
D. Kahneman, & J. F. Helliwell (Eds.), International differences in
psychological variables, not simply to enlarging their economies.
well-being. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Our findings indicate that it is important for societies not only to Howell, H., & Howell, C. (2008). The relation of economic status to
measure economic variables but to measure social psychological subjective well-being in developing countries: A meta-analysis. Psycho-
well-being variables as well. logical Bulletin, 134, 536 –560.
Hsee, C. K., Yang, Y., Li, N., & Shen, L. (2009). Wealth, warmth and
well-being: Whether happiness is relative or absolute depends on
References whether it is about money, acquisition or consumption. Journal of
Marketing Research, 46, 396 – 409.
Berridge, K. C. (2004). Motivation concepts in behavioral neuroscience. Johnson, W., & Krueger, R. F. (2006). How money buys happiness:
Physiology & Behavior, 81, 179 –209. Genetic and environmental processes linking finances and life satisfac-
Biswas-Diener, R., & Diener, E. (2001). Making the best of a bad situation: tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 680 – 691.
Satisfaction in the slums of Calcutta. Social Indicators Research, 55, Kahneman, D. (1999). Objective happiness. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, &
329 –352. N. Schwartz (Eds.), Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychology
Biswas-Diener, R., Vittersø, J., & Diener, E. (2005). Most people are pretty (pp. 3–25). New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.
happy, but there is cultural variation: The Inughuit, the Amish, and the Lucas, R., Diener, E., & Suh, E. (1996). Discriminant validity of well-
Maasai. Journal of Happiness Studies: An Interdisciplinary Periodical being measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71,
on Subjective Well-Being, 6, 205–226. 616 – 628.
Cantril, H. (1965). The patterns of human concern. New Brunswick, NJ: Maslow, A. H. (1943). A dynamic theory of human motivation. Psycho-
Rutgers University Press. logical Review, 50, 370 –396.
Clark, A. E., Frijters, P., & Shields, M. (2008). Relative income, happiness Myers, D. (2000). American paradox: Spiritual hunger in an age of plenty.
and utility: An explanation for the Easterlin paradox and other puzzles. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Journal of Economic Literature, 46, 95–144. Pavot, W., Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1990). Extraversion and happiness.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 1299 –1306.
New York, NY: Harper & Row. Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Belief and feeling: Evidence for
Diener, E., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2002). Will money increase subjective an accessibility model of emotional self-report. Psychological Bulletin,
well-being? A literature review and guide to needed research. Social 128, 934 –960.
Indicators Research, 57, 119 –169. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the
Diener, E., Kahneman, D., Tov, W., & Arora, R. (2009). Income’s asso- facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being.
ciation with judgments of life versus feelings. In E. Diener, D. Kahne- American Psychologist, 55, 68 –78.
man, & J. F. Helliwell (Eds.), International differences in well-being Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the
(pp. 3–15). Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press. meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
Easterlin, R. A. (1995). Will raising the incomes of all increase the Psychology, 57, 1069 –1081.
happiness of all? Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 27, Scitovsky, T. (1978). The joyless economy. Oxford, United Kingdom:
35– 48. Oxford University Press.
Easterlin, R. A. (2005). Feeding the illusion of growth and happiness: A Stevenson, B., & Wolfers, J. (2008). Economic growth and subjective
reply to Hagerty and Veenhoven. Social Indicators Research, 74, 429 – well-being: Reassessing the Easterlin paradox. Third Annual Conference
443. on Empirical Legal Studies Papers. Abstract retrieved from the Social
Emmons, R. A. (1986). Personal strivings: An approach to personality and Science Research Network website: http://ssrn.com/abstract⫽1121237
subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, Veenhoven, R., & Hagerty, M. (2006). Rising happiness in nations 1946 –
1058 –1068. 2004. Social Indicators Research, 79, 421– 436.
Frank, R. (2005). Does absolute income matter? Economics and Happi- World Bank. (2010). World development indicators online database.
ness, 27, 65–91. Available from the World Bank website: http://www.worldbank.org/
Fredrickson, B. L., & Joiner, T. (2002). Positive emotions trigger upward
spirals toward emotional well-being. Psychological Science, 13, 172– Received September 10, 2008
175. Revision received July 9, 2009
Graham, C., & Pettinato, S. (2002). Frustrated achievers: Winners, losers Accepted August 13, 2009 䡲

Вам также может понравиться