Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 11

Clinical Neuropsychiatry (2009) 6, 4, 155-165

ADOLESCENT BRAIN DEVELOPMENT AND EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS:


A PREFRONTAL FRAMEWORK FOR DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOPATHOLOGIES

Michele Poletti

Abstract
Objective: the brain undergoes a period of marked development during adolescence, due to the processes of
synaptic pruning and myelination, that improve the efficiency of cortical and cortico-subcortical connectivity. This
brain development has a significant impact on cognitive and emotional processing of adolescents. To describe the
correlates of adolescent brain maturation it’s here proposed a prefrontal framework that highlights anatomical, functional
and developmental differences between executive functions based on the Dorsolateral Prefrontal cortex and executive
functions based on the Orbitofrontal cortex.
Method: a literature review of cognitive and emotional correlates of adolescent brain development in healthy
subjects and in clinical populations was conducted in Electronic databases PubMed and PsycInfo. For the purpose of
this article, Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Major Depression are taken as prototypes of those externalizing
disorders and internalizing disorders, respectively, that may affect adolescents.
Results: executive functions based on the Orbitofrontal cortex mature earlier than executive functions based on
the Dorsolateral Prefrontal cortex. ADHD is characterized by a dysfunction of both types of executive functions, with
differences within clinical subtypes; Major Depression is characterized by a dysfunction of Orbitofrontal Prefrontal
cortex.
Conclusions: a prefrontal framework is useful to describe cognitive and emotional correlates of adolescent brain
development and how this phenomenon may increase the vulnerability of adolescents to psychopathology.

Key Words: adolescence, brain development, prefrontal framework, executive functions, ADHD, major depression

Declaration of interest: None

Michele Poletti
Neurology Unit, Versilia Hospital, Lido Di Camaiore (LU), Italy,
AnSvi, Accademia di Neuropsicologia dello Sviluppo, Parma.

Address for correspondence:


Dr. Michele Poletti
Neurology Unit, Versilia Hospital, via Aurelia 335, Lido di Camaiore, (LU).
+39 0584 6059539, fax: + 39 0584 6059539, michelepoletti79@gmail.com

Introduction portions of the Prefrontal Cortex (PFC) in healthy


adolescents, but also to explain why adolescence
Several psychopathologies may have an onset represents a temporal window of increased vulnerability
during adolescence: recent neuroimaging studies to psychopathology; in order to sustain this hypothesis,
suggest that this phenomenon could be related to the Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Major
marked structural development that involves the Depression are examined as prototypes of externalizing
cerebral cortex in this temporal window (Paus et al. disorders and internalizing disorders, respectively. A
2008). In this article is discussed the contribution of literature review of studies of healthy and clinical
neuropsychology to the assessment of behavioral, adolescent populations was conducted to support this
cognitive and affective correlates of this developmental distinction between DLPFC and OFC during
phenomenon. Empirical findings about correlates of development. Articles published before February 2009
adolescent brain development are framed within a were searched in the electronic databases PubMed and
prefrontal system that highlights anatomical, functional PsycInfo using the following keywords: adolescence,
and developmental differences between executive brain development, prefrontal cortex, dorsolateral
functions (EF), based on the dorsolateral portion of the prefrontal cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, executive
Prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), and EF based on the orbital functions, frontal functions, working memory, response
portion of the Prefrontal cortex (OFC). inhibition, reward processing, decision making, delay
This framework is useful not only to explain the aversion, impulsivity, Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity
different developmental trends of EF based on different Disorder, depression, mood disorder.
SUBMITTED JANUARY 2009, ACCEPTED JULY 2009

© 2009 Giovanni Fioriti Editore s.r.l. 155


Michele Poletti

Adolescent brain development shaped curve during adolescence. MRI studies found
that different cortical regions reach their peak of grey
In recent years, a group of neuroimaging studies matter at different ages. Only occipital lobes follow a
put light on adolescent brain development. This set of linear increase. Frontal lobes reach their peak at 12 years
studies (for example Gogtay et al. 2004, Shaw et al. of age for males and at 11 years for females. Parietal
2008, Sowell et al. 2003, for review see Giedd 2008, lobes reach their peak at 12 years for males, and at 10
Lenroot and Giedd 2006, Thompson et al. 2005) found for females. Temporal lobes reach their peak at 17 years,
that the adolescent brain has a marked development, either for males and females (Shaw et al. 2008). Brain
specially in the PFC. Different developmental patterns development continues until early adulthood, even if
of grey matter and white matter have been discovered with weaker intensity. The redefinition of neural
by the several longitudinal studies with neuroimaging networks, by loss of grey matter, continues, in the PFC
methodologies that followed the development of brain also in the third decade of life (Sowell et al. 2003), and
structure from childhood to early adulthood. DLPFC is the last prefrontal cortical area that reaches
its definitive thickness (Lenroot and Giedd 2006).
Studies in nonhuman animals suggest that cortical
White Matter Increase dimensions during critical periods for the development
of cognitive functions, may reflect experience-
First, structural Magnetic Resonance Imaging dependent molding of the architecture of cortical
(MRI), used to measure the size and shape of brain columns along with dendritic spine and axonal
structure, found that white matter volume increases in remodeling (Chklovskii et al. 2004, Mataga et al. 2004,
a roughly linear pattern, increasing throughout Hensch 2004, 2005, Sur and Rubinstein 2005). The
development into adulthood (Gogtay et al. 2004). These cortical thinning that dominates in adolescence might
changes presumably reflect ongoing myelination of reflect the use-dependent selective elimination of
axons by oligodendrocytes enhancing neuronal synapses (synaptic pruning), a process that enhances
conduction and communication (Casey et al. 2008). the computational capacity of local circuitry, improving
Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI), used to index the ability to modulate activity and implement stop
connectivity of white matter fiber tracts, permitted to mechanisms in a prompt manner (Hensch 2004,
look for specific associations between DTI-based Huttenlocher and Dabholkar 1997, Knudsen 2004).
measures of prefrontal white matter development and To summarize, the elimination of synaptic
cognitive control in children. For example, the redundant connections support more efficient and
development of working memory was found to be complicated regional neural computations, increasing
positively correlated with the fractional anisotropy, an firing stability of prefrontal cortical neurons (Rutherford
indicator of myelination and axonal thickness, of et al. 1998), more able to fire in concerted and sustained
prefrontal-parietal fiber tracts (Nagy et al., 2004). The patterns (Miller and Cohen 2001). At the same time
same indicator, relative to white matter tracts between myelination speeds neural transmissions, allowing
PFC and basal ganglia, was found to be correlated with distant brain regions to participate more efficiently in
the development of inhibitory control, as detected by widely distributed circuitry, that supports top-down
performances in go-no go tasks (Liston et al., 2005). executive control of behavior: communication among
Myelination involves cortico-cortical neural cortical regions can ensure that most refined plans are
connections between different prefrontal regions generated, while cortico-subcortical communication
(including the corpus callosum) and between the enhances top-down modulation of response plan
DLPFC and occipital, temporal and parietal association execution (Luna and Sweeney 2004). This pattern
areas (Gogtay et al. 2004), increasing the efficiency of suggests that adolescent brain development may support
conduction and communication by up to a hundred-fold more effective integration of brain functions across
(Durston et al. 2006). regions, rather than improvements in the computational
As evidenced in the proliferation of projections capacities of local brain function.
of white matter tracts across different brain regions, These researches have given new strength to the
there is an increase not only in connections among
fields of developmental psychopathology (Spessot et
cortical areas, but between cortical and subcortical areas
al. 2004, Toga et al. 2006) and cognitive development
(specially between prefrontal regions and limbic and
(Durston and Casey 2006, Kuhn 2006, Paus 2005,
paralimbic areas, including the amygdala, the nucleus
Steinberg 2005). How cognitive and affective correlates
accumbens and the hippocampus) (Eluvathingal et al.
of this prolonged brain development can be framed?
2007). This anatomical change should be associated
Here is proposed a prefrontal system framework that
with improved coordination of affect and cognition,
facilitated by the increased connectivity of regions distinguishes between EF based on the OFC and EF
involved in the processing of emotional information based on the DLPFC. The efficacy of this prefrontal
(the amygdala, ventral striatum, orbitofrontal cortex, framework is discussed examining different
medial prefrontal cortex, and superior temporal sulcus) maturational patterns of EF, either in healthy
and regions involved in cognitive control (DLPFC, adolescents and in adolescents with psychopathologies.
anterior and posterior cingulate, and temporo-parietal
cortices) (Steinberg 2008).
A framework for Executive Functions

Grey Matter U-Curve Shaped development PFC is one of the latest cortical region to mature
and to reach its definitive thickness during adolescence
The grey matter develops following an inverse U- (Lenroot and Giedd 2006). PFC is involved in several

156 Clinical Neuropsychiatry (2009) 6, 4


Developmental psychopathologies and prefrontal cortex

cognitive functions, like language, movement and high by gambling tasks or decision tasks (Hongwanishkul
level perception, but plays also a fundamental role in et al. 2005), like the Iowa Gambling Task (Bechara et
functions usually defined as EF (Stuss 1992). EF are al. 1994). Overall, it’s important to underline that a clear
usually considered a variety of high level cognitive distinction between DLPFC-EF and OFC-EF is possible
processes as attention, set-shifting, planning, working only distinguishing respective neural correlates; at a
memory, cognitive flexibility, decision-making, behavioral level of analysis, considering performance
feedback use, error detection, response inhibition and in experimental tasks or in real-life situations, it’s
self regulation, that are necessary for an appropriate possible to think only in terms of a prevalence of a
affective and contextual goal-directed behavior typology of EF, never in terms of presence/absence
(Alvarez and Emory 2006, Miyake et al. 2000). (Ardila 2008, Galotti 2007). Does this prefrontal system
framework fit to describe cognitive and affective
correlates of adolescent brain development? Recent
Anatomical distinction experimental results supporting the usefulness of this
approach in healthy and clinical adolescent populations
In order to describe the different cognitive and are reported and discussed in the next sections.
behavioural deficits after injuries to its different portions
(Stuss and Levine 2002), neuroscience actually
distinguishes between DLPFC-related EF (DLPFC-EF) Maturation of DLPFC vs. OFC Executive
and OFC-related EF (OFC-EF) (Ardila 2008). DLPFC Functions during adolescence
comprises the lateral portions of Brodmann’s areas 9,
10, 11, and 12; areas 45 and 46 and the superior part of DLPFC Executive Functions
area 47 (Damasio 1996, Gazzaniga et al. 1998). In
addition to its connections with OFC, DLPFC is An increasing number of researches, using a
connected to a variety of brain areas that allow it to standard neuropsychological approach, revealed that
play an important role in the integration of sensory and DLPFC-EF develop from childhood to adolescence
mnemonic information and in the regulation of (Davies and Rose 1999, Korkman et al. 2001, Levin et
intellectual function and action. These areas include al. 1991, Stuss 1992, Welsh and Pennington 1988, Welsh
thalamus, basal ganglia (the dorsal caudate nucleus), et al. 1991) following different developmental trends,
hippocampus, and primary and secondary associative reaching adult-like levels of performance at different
areas of neocortex, including posterior temporal, ages. A comparison from different studies is difficult
parietal, and occipital areas (Fuster 1989). OFC consists because many tasks are used to assess the same function
of both orbital (ventral) and medial regions of PFC, and few studies assess many EF simultaneously,
including the medial portions of Brodmann’s areas 9, impeding to look for latent common variables (Huizinga
10, 11, and 12; areas 13 and 25; and the inferior portion et al. 2006, Miyake et al. 2000). Recent studies
of area 47 (Damasio 1996, Gazzaniga et al. 1998). OFC reassessed the topic of DLPFC-EF maturation,
is part of a frontostriatal circuit that has strong combining neuropsychological tasks (Brocki and
connections to the amygdala and other parts of the Bohlin 2004, Conklin et al. 2007, Gathercole et al. 2004,
limbic system (Chudasama and Robbins 2006). Hence, Huizinga et al. 2006, Luciana et al. 2005), behavioral
OFC is well suited for the integration of affective and tasks (Luna et al. 2004) and neuroimaging (Crone et
cognitive information, and for the regulation of al. 2006, Durston et al. 2002, O’Hare et al. 2008).
motivated and goal-oriented behavior (Rolls 2004). This new set of combined studies (Blakemore and
Choudhury 2006 for review) confirmed that different
DLPFC-EF develop from childhood through
Functional distinction adolescence to young adulthood at different rates,
reaching mature functional levels at different ages. This
DLPFC-EF have been differently labelled; some is also confirmed by neuroimaging studies, whose
authors label them as “Cool EF”, being based on a results suggest there may be a dissociation within neural
cognitive and controlled elaboration of information correlates of performances between working memory
(Zelazo and Mueller 2002); other authors label them tasks and response inhibition tasks. Working memory
as “Metacognitive EF” (Ardila 2008). DLPFC EF tasks increasingly activate, with age, lateral PFC regions
permit an attentional control on behavior and include (Klingberg et al. 2002, Kwon et al. 2002) and in-
working memory, planning, task or set-switching, creasingly recruit complex neural networks (Crone et
problem solving, strategy development: they are al. 2006); for example, while in children working
assessed by classic executive tasks like the Trail Making memory tasks (verbal and spatial) activate only the left
Test (Reitan 1958), the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test ventral PFC, since adolescence the same tasks activate
(Grant and Berg 1948), the Tower of London (Shallice a complex prefrontal-parietal-cerebellar neural network
1982) and the Stroop Test (Stroop 1935). (Crone et al. 2006, O’Hare et al. 2008). On the other
Also OFC-EF have been differently labelled: some side, response inhibition tasks decreasingly recruit, with
authors label them as “Hot EF”, being based on an age, complex neural networks, showing increasingly
emotional and automatic processing of information focal activation in specific regions (inferior PFC: Aron
(Zelazo and Mueller 2002); other authors label them et al., 2004) thought to play a critical role in response
as “Emotional/Motivational EF” (Ardila 2008). OFC- inhibition (Durston et al. 2002, Tamm et al. 2002).
EF guide a reward-based control of behavior and the A recent meta-analysis of studies published
management of risk; they include reward processing, between 1984 and 2004 on the development of DLPFC-
reversal learning and decision making: they are assessed EF, is useful to provide a general representation of their

Clinical Neuropsychiatry (2009) 6, 4 157


Michele Poletti

maturation from childhood to adolescence (Romine and they were drawing from the good decks about 55% and
Reynolds 2005). General trends included medium to 60% of the time, respectively. By the final block,
large age-related increases in performance between 5 however, the young adults were drawing from the good
and 8 years of age. Similarly, medium to large effects deck nearly 75% of the time, and they began shifting
were found between the span of 8 to 11 years of age. towards the good decks much earlier than the younger
Small to medium age-related increases were evident groups. Another study, of 9 to 17-year-olds, also found
between the range of 11 to 14 years. Changes in per- significant improvement in performance on this task
formance between 14 and 17 years of age, across the with age (Hooper et al. 2004). 14-17 year-olds drew
prefrontal abilities reviewed, ranged from no age- from the good decks more often than 9-10 year-olds
related change to medium-size change. Variability in (although not as often than 11-13 year olds) and began
age-related increase between the 17 years to adulthood shifting to the good decks earlier than either of the
emerges, with some prefrontal functions displaying no younger groups.
age-related increase and others demonstrating a large Successful performances on the decision tasks
increase in mean performance. This meta-analysis require participants to pay attention to the outcomes of
found that across the areas of planning, verbal fluency their choices, using this feedback in their future choices.
and response inhibition the greatest period of Thus, poor performances in decision tasks may result
development was between the ages of 5 and 8 years. from an insensitivity to loss or from an inability to use
During the 8 to 11 age span, significant increases were feedbacks in anticipation of future risk. A recent
evident across all prefrontal functions. A small increase physiological study demonstrated that the ability to
in performance in response inhibition was found anticipate future outcomes of decisions continues to
between 11 and 14 years of age; however, no age-related develop until late adolescence (Crone and van der
increase in performance was evident after this age Molen 2007), supporting the hypothesis that 8-10-year-
period. A continued development of planning and verbal old and 12-14-year-old children perform like OFC-
fluency was noted throughout adolescence, with damaged adult patients (Damasio 1994), because they
improvement in performance even in the 17 years of fail to anticipate outcomes prior to making a decision.
age to early adulthood period. These data rejected the hypothesis that 8-10-year-old
children and 12-14-year-old children perform
disadvantageously because they would have failed to
OFC Executive Functions process the outcomes of their decisions.
Functional neuroimaging has recently been used
With regard to OFC-EF, they are generally to investigate developmental differences in neural
assessed by decision tasks. As a matter of fact, when a correlates of decision making. In a fMRI study, 18
choice has to be made, information about expected adolescents (9-17 year-old) and 20 adults (20-40 year-
outcomes have to be maintained in memory in order to old) underwent brain scan during an economic decision
be compared and integrated with information about task, that directly assessed risk-taking behaviors during
internal states and current subjective goals. This inte- choice selection (Eishel et al. 2007). OFC and dorsal
grative process generates outcome expectancies, that Anterior Cingulate Cortex (ACC) were examined
are internal representations of possible consequences selectively since both have been implicated in reward-
of own actions: OFC plays a crucial role in the related processes and resolution of conflicting
generation of these outcome expectancies (Wallis decisions, respectively (Fleck et al. 2006). Group
2007). comparisons revealed greater activation in the OFC and
While the assessment of decision making in adults dorsal ACC, in adults than in adolescents, when making
has generated an extensive amount of researches, few risky selections. Furthermore, reduced activity in these
studies approached the study of this function in areas correlated with greater risk-taking performances
adolescence. Recent results from developmental studies in adolescents and in the combined group. These data
reported that performances in decision tasks are worse show that adolescents engage prefrontal regulatory
than those of adults until 11-12 years of age, because structures to a lesser extent than adults when making
of a bias for immediate wins, despite possible greater risky economic choices.
future gains (Crone et al. 2005). Using a developmental Another study (Galvan et al. 2006) examined the
version of this task in a sample of subjects from 7 to 15 development of neural correlates of reward-seeking
years of age and varying the frequency and the temporal behaviors. 37 participants (7-29 years of age) underwent
discount of wins and punishments, this study found an fMRI while performing a task that manipulated reward
age-related increase of the sensitivity toward possible values. The results show exaggerated nucleus
future punishments, also in uncertain situations. Up to accumbens activity relative to PFC activity, in
12 years of age, only when the punishment was very adolescents compared to children and adults. Nucleus
probable and heavy it received attention by subjects, accumbens activity in adolescents was similar to adults
while it was ignored in other cases. in both extent of activity and sensitivity to reward
Few studies directly examined the maturation of values, although the magnitude of activity was
decision making during adolescence with the same tasks exaggerated. In contrast, the extent of OFC activity in
used with adults, as the Iowa Gambling Task (Bechara adolescents looked more like children’s extent than the
et al. 1994). In one study (Crone and van der Molen adults’ extent, with less focal patterns of activity.
2004) with four age groups (6-9, 10-12, 13-15, 18-25 All these empirical results, at behavioral and
year-olds), the youngest subjects drew equally from the functional levels, confirm what suggested by
good and bad decks. The two middle groups showed neuroanatomical studies, that is the OFC-limbic system
modest improvement over time; by the final trial block, matures earlier than the cognitive-regulatory system,

158 Clinical Neuropsychiatry (2009) 6, 4


Developmental psychopathologies and prefrontal cortex

becoming disproportionately activated relative to later prefrontal cortex. Diminished activation in a caudate
maturing top-down control system, and biasing the region was correlated with lower subjective positive
adolescent’s action toward immediate over future gains. affect in natural environments, particularly within the
Only when a cognitive-regulatory activity is well depressed group.
functioning, the prefrontal activity is adequately These preliminary studies on OFC-EF in
balanced between reward evaluation and cognitive adolescents with MD demonstrated the usefulness of a
regulation. This maturational mismatch between neurocognitive approach to the study of psycho-
different portions of the PFC is deemed to be one of pathologies with adolescent onset: they gave prelimi-
the causes of affect dysregulation in adolescence, a nary experimental evidence that 1) adolescents with an
phenomenon that may be considered as an increased internalizing disorder like MD may often present a
vulnerability to mood disorders, in particular dysfunction in reward processing; 2) the reduction of
depression. In the next paragraph are reviewed the striatal reward system is directly related to the
neurocognitive studies on adolescent depression. reduction of positive affectivity (Diekhof et al. 2008,
Poletti 2008 for review). The link between an altered
reward processing and MD is of particular interest,
Executive Functions in Adolescent Major because of the high rate of mood disorders with an onset
Depression during adolescence (Pine et al. 1998, Pine et al. 2002,
Salujia et al. 2004). Recently, some theoretical models
While DLPFC-EF are reported within the average have been proposed to explain adolescent vulnerability
range in adolescents with Major Depression (MD) to depression. The “Social information processing
(Fravre et al. 2009), some recent studies, assessing network” (SIPN: Nelson et al. 2004) proposed that
OFC-EF in internalizing developmental psycho- changes in social behaviors during adolescence are
pathologies, reported altered neural responses in choice related with the development of different neural
tasks with rewards varying in magnitude and networks involved in the processing of social
probability. For example, adolescents with MD exhibit information. These three neural networks develop along
weaker neural response than control participants in OFC different trajectories: in particular the affective node
during both anticipation/decision and reward- (almost overlapping the subcortical limbic system and
processing phases of the task (Forbes et al. 2006). the OFC), develops earlier than the cognitive regulatory
Another study investigated reward-related decision node, principally based on the PFC (Eluvatinghal et al.
making within a longitudinal study of 221 11-year-old 2006, Lenroot and Giedd 2006). This maturational
boys, 25 of whom suffered of MD at age 10 or 11 mismatch implies that during adolescence there is a
(Forbes et al. 2007). Participants completed a behavioral temporal window of increased vulnerability to affective
decision-making task varying probability and dysregulation as the activation of the affective node is
magnitude of rewards. Under conditions involving a not adequately counterbalanced by and controlled by
high probability of winning, adolescents with MD failed the PFC (Rubia et al. 2000), resulting in having
to distinguish between options involving small or large difficulty to modulate emotional reactions to social
possible rewards, while subjects with anxiety or stimuli (Yurgelun-Todd 2007). However, the SIPN
externalizing disorders at age 10 or 11 did not differ model of affect dysregulation does not provide an
from others in their reward-related decisions. Low explanation for the specific vulnerability of adolescents
frequency of choosing the high-probability-large reward to depression, considering that affective dysregulation
option at age 11 predicted depressive disorders, anxiety is an important clinical feature also of anxiety disorders
disorders, and depressive symptoms 1 year later. (Ladoucer et al. 2005) and somatoform disorders
Furthermore, reward-related decisions predicted later (Waller and Scheidt 2006).
depressive or anxiety disorders even when adjusting The dysregulated positive affect model (Forbes
for the continuity of such disorders and the presence of and Dahl 2005) examined the relationship between
concurrent externalizing disorders. adolescent depression and the development of the
As reported in studies with adults with MD reward processing systems, framing depression as a
(Epstein et al. 2006, Keedwell et al. 2005, Knutson et reduction in positive affectivity: the remodelling of
al. 2008), the dysfunction of OFC-EF in adolescents neural systems at the basis of reward processing could
with MD is also suggested by the weak activation of result in an higher vulnerability to the dysregulation of
the striatum during reward anticipation and reward affect and could predispose to depression. An
outcome. As a matter of fact, striatum and OFC are advancement of these models on the relationship
strictly connected in a subcortical- prefrontal network between depression and representation of reward in
and involved in reward processing (O’Doherty 2004). adolescence has been recently proposed (Davey et al.
In a recent study (Forbes et al. 2009) 15 adolescents 2008). Brain development and the consequent cognitive
with MD disorder and 28 adolescents with no history maturation allow adolescents to represent abstract and
of psychiatric disorder, ages 8-17 years, underwent complex rewards, for example found in complex
fMRI while performing a guessing task involving relationships: as a matter of fact the susceptibility to
monetary reward. Participants also reported their peer pressure follows an inverted U-shaped curve, that
subjective positive affect in natural environments during has its peak during adolescence, around age 14, and
a 4-day cell-phone-based ecological momentary declines thereafter (Gardner and Steinberg 2005,
assessment. Adolescents with major depressive disorder Steinberg and Monahan 2007). The brain becomes able
exhibited less striatal response than healthy comparison to encode more complex rewards, while social changes
adolescents during reward anticipation and reward see adolescents become more sociable and engaged in
outcome, but more response in dorsolateral and medial more complex social situations and relationships, that

Clinical Neuropsychiatry (2009) 6, 4 159


Michele Poletti

are more instable and need to be more actively pursued. 2005; Halperin and Schulz 2006; Sergeant 2000).
All these changes lead to psychological changes Globally, these studies identified in the poor inhibitory
including an increased sensitivity to social relationships, control, specially at a motor level, the core deficit of
and the ability to see both themselves and the ADHD. Inhibitory control is a EF based on the right
relationships in a more extended temporal context inferior PFC (Aron et al. 2004) and several studies
(Davey et al. 2008). How do these psychological reported that the developmental curve of this function
changes impact on reward system? Recent researches ends around 14-15 years of age, when performance in
have highlighted features of reward system that are inhibitory tasks become similar to those of adult
relevant to understand its role in depression: first, subjects (Romine and Reynolds 2005). A recent meta-
dopaminergic reward system is activated by the analysis put many doubts on the efficacy of the
anticipation of rewards rather then their consummation inhibitory control deficit to describe cognitive deficits
and by stimuli that predict rewards rather then rewards of all ADHD subjects, highlighting the phenotypical
themselves (Knutson et al. 2000, Schultz 1998); heterogeneity of this clinical population (Lijffjt et al.
secondly, the dopaminergic reward system responds to 2005): as a matter of fact, an inhibitory deficit is
novel, intermittent and unexpected rewards rather than reported in many studies but it could be explained also
expected rewards where learning has already occurred by impairments of other functions active during the task
(O’Doherty et al. 2002), and transiently suppressed (Martinussen et al. 2005). Another meta-analysis
when an expected reward is omitted (Hollerman and (Wilcutt et al. 2005), examining results from 83 studies
Schultz 1998). Then the onset of depression during on EF in ADHD, for a total amount of 6700 subjects,
adolescence may result from the frustration, or reported that the only robust finding across studies is a
omission, of highly anticipated social rewards involving spatial working memory deficit: the hypothesis of a
love, belonging to social groups and social agency common inhibitory deficit in ADHD is not confirmed
(Davey et al. 2008). The omission of expected rewards (Bitaskou et al. 2008).
may cause a prolonged suppression of the reward To better describe the heterogeneity of per-
system, resulting in a reduced positive affectivity: this formances of ADHD subjects, it has been recently
phenomenon, combined with other vulnerability factors proposed that executive dysfunction is related to the
(environmental, cognitive and genetic: Yap et al. 2007) attention deficit but not to the hyperactivity/impulsivity
may cause depression (Yap et al. 2008). trait (Diamond 2005, Nigg 2005, Stefanatos and Baron
These theoretical models can be summarized and 2007). A better description of different ADHD
integrated underlining that adolescents received an phenotypes involves an inhibitory control deficit and a
increased stimulation by their environment and because delay aversion (Johansen 2009, Solanto et al. 2001).
their cognitive abilities allow a widen temporal Delay aversion indicates the preference for immediate
perspective. This increased stimulation activates the over delayed rewards and is measured by decision tasks
affective node, in particular the reward system, whose in which subjects choose between small immediate and
activity is not counterbalanced by an adequate activity large delayed rewards (Bitsakou et al. 2006, Muller et
of the regulatory node for a prolonged temporal al. 2006). Choices of the small immediate reward (that
window. Adolescence represents a period of increased is delay aversion) are uncorrelated with inhibitory
potentialities but also increased vulnerabilities: if difficulties, suggesting that inhibitory deficits and delay
complex social and future rewards become available, aversion in ADHD are dissociable processes: so
this may increase the motivation to action; at the same performances on either tasks are only moderately
time, when adolescents fail to achieve these rewards associated with ADHD but together correctly classify
(generating frustration and disappointment, and a nearly 90% of children and adolescents with ADHD
prolonged suppression of reward system) or when (Sonuga-Barke et al. 2003).
rewards overwhelm the affective node, they may In order to explain the presence of these dissociable
develop internalizing psychopathologies like MD. behavioral characteristics in ADHD subjects, it has been
recently proposed that inattention reflects a deficit of
DLPFC-EF (inhibitory control and spatial working
Executive Functions in Attention Deficit/ memory) (Martel et al. 2007), while hyperactivity/
Hyperactivity Disorder impulsivity symptoms reflect a deficit of OFC-EF
(delay aversion): this approach gives rise to the
Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) possibility that some subjects with ADHD manifest
is here taken as a paradigmatic example of externalizing primarily a DLPFC-EF dysfunction, some subjects
developmental psychopathology. ADHD is considered manifest mainly a OFC-EF deficit, whereas others
to have a strong biological aetiology (Swanson et al. manifest both types (Castellanos et al. 2006). For
2007): for example, neuroimaging studies reported a example, it has been reported that a risky decision-
reduced volume of PFC (Yeo et al. 2003), whose making style in the IGT, taken as an index of OFC-EF,
morphology is related to behavioral phenotypes of is associated with hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms
subjects affected by this psychopathology (Dickstein but not with inattentive symptoms in adolescents with
et al. 2006). Neurocognitive approaches to ADHD ADHD (Toplak et al. 2005).
focused on the presence of executive deficits: a poor In summary, whereas studies on cognitive
inhibitory control on internal and external interferences dysfunction in ADHD have mostly focused on DLPFC-
has been proposed as primary deficit, leading to EF, the presence of impairments in incentive,
secondary difficulties of working memory, motivation motivational and reward-related processing based on
and affect regulation, those abilities that enable goal- the OFC suggested that both these types of EF may
directed behaviors (Barkley 1997, 2004; Dobler et al. present deficits and deserve attention during the

160 Clinical Neuropsychiatry (2009) 6, 4


Developmental psychopathologies and prefrontal cortex

assessment (Sonuga-Barke et al. 2008). As a matter of processing related to different developmental


fact, these dissociable deficits continue to differently psychopathologies like MD and ADHD offers a new
influence behavioural performance of ADHD subjects perspective to clinical assessment. First, in clinical
also during young adulthood, for example making practice the measurement of EF may indirectly suggest
distinct contributions to academic skill difficulties the level of functional impairment due to a
(Thorell 2007). developmental psychopathology. This information,
matched with other information available to clinicians,
may help in the diagnostic phase and in the planning of
Discussion a therapy or of a rehabilitation program. Second, the
assessment of EF could be used to evaluate the effects
Longitudinal MRI studies have shown that, during of therapeutic/rehabilitative interventions at the end of
adolescence, the human cerebral cortex undergoes a treatments and in follow-up phases. Referring in
period of marked maturation. This phenomenon is particular to psychopathologies examined in this paper,
particularly evident in the PFC, one of the latest cortical interventions for MD may be usually pharmacological
areas to mature and to reach its definitive shape, at the (Tsapakis et al. 2008), psychotherapeutic (Watanabe et
beginning of adulthood. Brain development supports al. 2007, Weisz et al. 2006) or either. It’s possible to
cognitive and affective maturation of adolescents, leading hypothesize that an improvement of mood due to the
them towards an adult way of thinking and behaving. therapy may go with an improvement of performances
These cognitive and affective correlates of brain in tasks sensitive to EF and this finding could be
development are framed within a prefrontal system- considered as an indirect index of the beneficial effects
approach, that distinguishes DLPFC-EF and OFC-EF. of the treatment. For what concerns ADHD, the finding
DLPFC sustains a cognitive elaboration of information, that this psychopathology is related to specific EF
while OFC, due to its strong connections with limbic dysfunction not only offers a way to assess possible
structures, sustains an interaction between cognition and beneficial effects of treatments, but also suggests the
emotion, motivating goal-directed behaviors. possibility to plan interventions alternative or integrated
The applicability of this framework is demon- to the pharmacological one. As a matter of fact,
strated by studies on healthy adolescents. OFC-EF and preliminary attempts to rehabilitate subjects with
DLPFC-EF mature at different rates: there is some ADHD have been focused on possible beneficial effects
evidence that the maturation of these regions may take of a cognitive training of those EF that have been shown
place along somewhat different timetables (Lenroot and as impaired in this clinical population, in particular
Giedd 2006), with performance on exclusively OFC working memory (Klingberg et al. 2002, 2005). For
tasks reaching adult levels earlier than performance on example these studies found that a brief, intensive trai-
exclusively DLPFC tasks (Hooper et al. 2004, Romine ning of working memory not only improved working
and Reynolds 2005). memory performances, but also improved performances
Also studies on developmental psychopathologies of other non-trained EF (planning, inhibition) and had
highlighted the usefulness of this theoretical framework. beneficial effects at a behavioral level (reduction of
Studies on ADHD adolescents demonstrated that number of head movements).
different symptoms are related to different executive
deficits: the dysfunction of OFC-EF, evidenced by a
delay aversion, is related to hyperactivity/impulsivity Future directions
symptoms, while the dysfunction of DLPFC-EF,
evidenced by deficits in inhibitory control and in spatial Future studies have to investigate the different
working memory, is related to inattentive symptoms. functioning of these two types of EF in other
Studies on adolescents with MD revealed a dysfunction developmental psychopathologies, specially those that
of OFC-EF, in particular reward processing. Recent in adulthood are characterized by an executive deficit
models attempted to explain the specific relationship and an affect dysregulation, for example Obsessive
between an altered reward processing and the increased Compulsive Disorder (Chamberlain et al. 2008) and
vulnerability to depression in adolescents, highlighting Borderline Personality Disorder (Kunert et al. 2003).
that adolescence involves the management of more Considering that adolescents with internalizing
complex and social rewards than in infancy: if on the disorders present a dysfunction of OFC-EF, it could be
one hand this phenomenon raises motivation to action, interesting to investigate if, conversely, subjects with
on the other side it may temporarily suppress the reward externalizing disorders like Conduct Disorder and
system if rewards are not reached or overwhelms reward Oppositional Defiant Disorder present a dysfunction
system, leading to an affect dysregulation. of DLPFC-EF. Preliminary studies suggested that a
This review provided evidence that a framework deficit of inhibitory control, specially of motor
that distinguishes DLPFC-EF and OFC-EF fits to responses, is more frequent in adolescents with Conduct
describe cognitive and affective correlates of adolescent Disorder (Herba et al. 2006) and substance abuse (Nigg
brain development, and to characterize cognitive and et al. 2006) than in healthy age-matched controls.
emotional processing dysfunctions at the basis of some It could then be hypothesized that a dysfunction
developmental psychopathologies. of OFC-EF is involved in internalizing disorders and a
dysfunction of DLPFC-EF is involved in externalizing
disorders. Interestingly ADHD could be considered a
Clinical implications disorder at the intersection of these different clinical
conditions (Swanson et al. 2007), whose subtypes
The evidence about a dysfunctional executive present different executive deficits within this DLPFC-

Clinical Neuropsychiatry (2009) 6, 4 161


Michele Poletti

OFC framework. By this perspective, there is a need of Neuropsychology 11, 245-263.


further attention to the assessment of executive Crone EA, van der Molen MW (2007). Development of decision
functioning in those adolescents that present co- making in school-aged children and adolescents: evidence
from heart rate and skin conductance analysis. Child
occurrence disorders (Boylan et al. 2007, Wolff and
Development 78, 1288-1301.
Ollendick 2006), for example Conduct Disorder and Crone EA, van der Molen MW (2004). Developmental changes
Major Depression. in real life decision making: performance on a gambling
task previously shown to depend on the ventromedial
prefrontal cortex. Developmental Neuropsychology 25,
References 251-279.
Crone EA., Wendelken C, Donohue SE, Bunge SA (2006). Neural
Alvarez JA, Emory E (2006). Executive Function and the Frontal evidence for dissociable components of task-switching.
Lobes: A Meta-Analytic Review. Neuropsychology Review Cerebral Cortex 16, 475-486.
16, 17-42. Crone EA., Wendelken C, Donohue SE, van Leijenhorst L, Bunge
Ardila A (2008). On the evolutionary origins of executive SA (2006). Neurocognitive development of the ability to
functions. Brain and Cognition 68, 92-99. manipulate information in working memory. Proceedings
Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA (2004). Inhibition and the of the National Academy of Sciences of the Unites States
right inferior prefrontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive of America 103, 9315-9320.
Sciences 8, 170-176. Damasio AR (1994). Descartes’s error. Emotion, reason, and
Barkley RA (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, the human brain. New York: Grosset/Putnam.
and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of Damasio H (1996). Human neuroanatomy relevant to decision-
ADHD. Psychological Bulletin 121, 65-94. making. In: AR. Damasio, H. Damasio, Y. Christen (eds)
Barkley RA (2004). Adolescents with attention-deficit/ Neurobiology of decision-making. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
hyperactivity disorder: an overview of empirically based Davey CG, Yucel M, Allen NB (2008). The emergence of
treatments. Journal of Psychiatric Practice 10, 39-56. depression in adolescence: Development of the prefrontal
Bechara A, Damasio, AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW (1994). cortex and the representation of reward. Neuroscience and
Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to Biobehavioral Review 32, 1-19.
human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50, 7-15. Davies PL, Rose JD (1999). Assessment of cognitive
Bitsakou P, Antrop I, Wiersema JR, Sonuga-Barke EJ (2006). development in adolescents by means of neuro-
Probing the limits of delay intolerance: preliminary young psychological tasks. Developmental Neuropsychology 15,
adult data from the Delay Frustration Task (DeFT). Journal 227-248.
of Neuroscience Methods 151, 38-44. Diamond A (2005). Attention-deficit disorder attention-deficit
Bitsakou P, Psychogiou L, Thompson M, Sonuga-Barke EJ hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity: A
(2008). Inhibitory deficits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from
disorder are independent of basic processing efficiency and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder with hyperactivity.
IQ. Journal of Neural Transmission 115, 261-268. Developmental Psychopathology 17, 807-825.
Blakemore SJ, Choudhury S (2006). Development of the Dickstein SG, Bannon K, Castellanos FX, Milham MP (2006).
adolescent brain: implications for executive function and The neural correlates of attention deficit hyperactivity
social cognition. Journal of Child Psychology and disorder: an ALE meta analysis. Journal of Child
Psychiatry 47, 296-312. Psychology and Psychiatry 47, 1051-1062.
Boylan K, Vaillancourt T, Boyle M, Szatmari P (2007). Diekhof EK, Falkai P, Gruber O. (2008). Functional
Comorbidity of internalizing disorders in children with neuroimaging of reward processing and decision making:
oppositional defiant disorder. European Child & a review of aberrant motivational and affective processing
Adolescent Psychiatry 16, 484-494. in addiction and mood disorders. Brain Research Reviews
Brocki KC, Bohlin G (2004). Executive functions in children 59, 164-184.
aged 6 to 13: a dimensional and developmental study. Dobler VB, Anker S, Gilmore J, Robertson IH, Atkinson J, Manly
Developmental Neuropsychology 26, 571-593. T (2005). Asymmetric deterioration of spatial awareness
Casey BJ, Getz S, Galvan A (2008). The adolescent brain. with diminishing levels of alertness in normal children and
Developmental Review 28, 62-77. children with ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology and
Castellanos FX, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Milham MP, Tannock R Psychiatry 46, 1230-1248.
(2006). Characterizing cognition in ADHD: beyond Durston S, Casey BJ (2006). What have we learned about
executive dysfunction. Trends in Cognitive Science 10, cognitive development from neuroimaging?
117-123. Neuropsychologia 44, 2149-2157.
Chamberlain SR, Menzies L, Hampshire A, Suckling J, Durston S, Thomas KM, Yang YH, Ulug AM, Zimmerman RD,
Finenberg NA, Del Campo N, Altken M, Craig K, Owen Casey BJ (2002). A neural basis for the development of
AM, Bullmore ET, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ (2008). inhibitory control. Developmental Science 5, 9-16.
Orbitofrontal dysfunction in patients with obsessive Eluvatinghal T, Hasan K, Kramer L, Fletcher J, Ewing-Cobbs,
compulsive disorder and their unaffected relatives. Science L (2007). Quantitative diffusion tensor tractography of
321, 421-422. association of projection fibers in normally developing
Chklovskii DB, Mel BW, Svoboda K (2004). Cortical rewiring children and adolescents. Cerebral Cortex 17, 2760-2768.
and information storage. Nature 431, 782-788. Epstein J, Pan H, Kocsis JH, Yang Y, Butler T, Chusid J,
Chudasama Y, Robbins TW (2006). Functions of frontostriatal Hochberg H, Murrough J, Strohmayer E, Stern E,
systems in cognition: comparative neuropsycho- Silbersweig DA. (2006). Lack of ventral striatal response
pharmacological studies in rats, monkeys and humans. to positive stimuli in depressed versus normal subjects.
Biological Psychology 73, 19-38. American Journal of Psychiatry 163, 1784-1790.
Conklin MH, Luciana M, Hooper CJ, Yarger RS (2007). Working Eshel N, Nelson EE, Blair RJ, Pine DS, Ernst M (2007). Neural
memory performance in typically developing children and substrates of choice selection in adults and adolescents:
adolescents: behavioral evidence of protracted frontal lobe development of the ventrolateral prefrontal and anterior
development. Developmental Neuropsychology 31, 103- cingulate cortices. Neuropsychologia 45, 1270-1279.
128. Favre T, Hughes C, Emslie G, Stavinoha P, Kennard P, Carmody
Crone EA, Bunge SA, Latenstein H, van der Molen MW (2005). T. (2009). Executive functioning in children and
Characterization of children’s decision making; sensitivity adolescents with Major Depressive Disorder. Child
to punishment frequency, not task complexity. Child Neuropsychology 15, 85-98.

162 Clinical Neuropsychiatry (2009) 6, 4


Developmental psychopathologies and prefrontal cortex

Fleck MS, Darselaar SM, Dobbins IG, Cabeza R (2006). Role of Implications for the development of decision making and
prefrontal and anterior cingulated regions in decision ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Developmental Psychology
making processes shared by memory and non memory 40, 1148-1158.
tasks. Cerebral Cortex 16, 1623-1630. Huizinga M, Dolan CV, van der Molen MW (2006). Age related
Forbes EE, Christopher MJ, Siegle GJ, Ladouceur CD, Ryan changes in executive function. Developmental trends and
ND, Carter CS, Birmaher B, Axelson DA, Dahl RE (2006). a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia 44, 2017-
Reward related decision making in pediatric major 2036.
depressive disorder: an fMRI study. Journal of Child Huttenlocher PR, Dabholkar AS (1997). Regional differences
Psychology and Psychiatry 47, 1031-1040. in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. Journal of
Forbes EE, Dahl RE (2005). Neural systems of positive affect: Comparative Neurology 387, 167-178.
relevance to understanding child and adolescent Johansen EB, Killeen PR, Russell VA, Tripp G, Wickens JR,
depression? Development and Psychopathology 17, 827- Tannock R, Williams J, Sagvolden T (2009). Origins of
850. altered reinforcement effects in ADHD. Behavioral and
Forbes EE, Hariri AR, Martin SL, Silk JS, Moyles DL, Fisher Brain Functions 5, 7.
PM, Brown SM, Ryan ND, Birmaher B, Axelson DA, Dahl Keedwell PA, Andrew C, Williams SC, Brammer MJ, Phillips
RE (2009). Altered striatal activation predicting real-world ML (2005). The neural correlates of anhedonia in major
positive affect in adolescent major depressive disorder. depressive disorder. Biological Psychiatry 58, 843-853.
American Journal of Psychiatry 166, 64-73. Klingberg T, Fernell E, Olesen PJ, Johnson M, Gustafson P,
Forbes EE, Shaw DS, Dahl RE (2007). Alterations in reward- Dahlstrom K, Gillberg CG, Forssberg H, Westerberg H.
related decision making in boys with recent and future (2005). Computerized training of working memory in
depression. Biological Psychiatry 61, 633-639. children with ADHD – a randomized controlled trial.
Fuster J (1989). The prefrontal cortex: Anatomy, physiology and Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
neuropsychology of the frontal lobe (2nd edition). New Psychiatry 44, 177-186.
York: Raven. Klingberg T, Forssberg H, Westerberg H (2002). Increased brain
Galotti KM (2007). Decision structuring in important real life activity in frontal and parietal cortex underlies the
choices. Psychological Science 18, 320-325. development of visuospatial working memory capacity
Galvan A, Hare AH, Parra CE, Penn J., Voss H, Glover G, Casey during childhood. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 14,
BJ (2006). Earlier development of the accumbens relative 1-10.
to orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk-taking behaviour Klingberg T, Forssberg H, Westerberg H. (2002). Training of
in adolescents. Journal of Neuroscience 26, 6885-6892. working memory in children with ADHD. Journal of
Gardner M, Steinberg L (2005). Per influence on risk taking, Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 24, 781-791.
risk preference and risky decision making in adolescence Knudsen EI (2004). Sensitive periods in the development of brain
and adulthood: an experimental study. Developmental and behavior. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 16, 1412-
Psychology 41, 625-635. 1425.
Gathercole SE, Pickering SJ, Ambridge B, Wearing H (2004). Knutson B, Bhanji JP, Cooney RE, Atlas LY, Gotlib IH (2008).
The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of Neural response to monetary incentives in major
age. Developmental Psychology 40, 177-190. depression. Biological Psychiatry 63, 686-692.
Gazzaniga MS, Ivry, RB, Mangun GR (1998). Cognitive Korkman M, Kemp SL, Kirk U (2001). Effects of age on
neuroscience: the biology of the mind. New York: W.W. neurocognitive measures of children ages 5 to 12: A cross-
Norton & Company. sectional study on 800 children from the United States.
Giedd JN (2008). The teen brain: insights from neuroimaging. Developmental Neuropsychology 20, 331–354.
Journal of Adolescent Health 42, 335-343. Kuhn D (2006). Do cognitive changes accompany developments
Gogtay N, Giedd JN, Lusk L, Hayashi KM, Greenstein D, in the adolescent brain? Perspective in Psychological
Vaituzis AC, Nugent TF 3rd, Herman DH, Clasen LS, Toga Science 1, 59-67.
AW, Rapoport JL, Thompson PM (2004). Dynamic Kunert HJ Druecke HW, Sass H, Herpertz SC (2003). Frontal
mapping of human cortical development during childhood lobe dysfunctions in borderline personality disorder?
through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Neuropsychological findings. Journal of Personality
Academy of Sciences of the Unites States of America 101, Disorders 17, 497-509.
8174-8179. Kwon H, Reiss AL, Menon V (2002). Neural basis of protracted
Grant DA, Berg EA (1948). A behavioral analysis of degree of developmental changes in visuo-spatial working memory.
reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
Weigl-type card sorting problem. Journal of Experimental Unites States of America 99, 13336-13341.
Psychology 34, 404-411. Ladoucer CD, Dahl RE, Williamson DE, Birmaher B, Ryan ND,
Halperin JM, Schulz KP (2006). Revisiting the role of the Casey BJ (2005). Altered emotional processing in pediatric
prefrontal cortex in the pathophysiology of attention-defi- anxiety, depression and comorbid anxiety-depression.
cit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychological Bulletin 132, 560- Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 33, 165-177.
581. Lenroot RK, Giedd JN (2006). Brain development in children
Hensch TK (2004). Critical period regulation. Annual Review of and adolescents: insights from anatomical magnetic
Neuroscience 27, 549-579. resonance imaging. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral
Hensch TK (2005). Critical period plasticity in local cortical Review 30, 718-729.
circuits. Nature Review Neuroscience 6, 877-888. Levin HS, Culhane KA, Hartmann J, Evankovich K, Mattson
Herba CM, Tranah T, Rubia K, Yule W (2006). Conduct problems AJ, Harward H (1991). Developmental changes in perfor-
in adolescence: three domains of inhibition and effect of mance on tests of purported frontal lobe functioning.
gender. Developmental Neuropsychology 30, 659-695. Developmental Neuropsychology 7, 377-395.
Hollerman JR, Schultz W (1998). Dopamine neurons reports an Lijffjt M, Kenemans JL, Verbaten MN, van Engeland H (2005).
error in the temporal prediction of reward during learning. A meta analytic review of stopping performance in
Nature Neuroscience 1, 304-309. attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: deficient inhibitory
Hongwanishkul D, Happaney KR, Lee WSC, Lee, Zelazo PD motor control? Journal of Abnormal Psychology 114, 216-
(2005). Assessment of hot and cool executive function in 222.
young children: Age-related changes and individual Liston C, Watts R, Tottenham N, Davidson MC, Niogi S, Ulug
differences. Developmental Neuropsychology 28, 617-644. AM, Casey BJ (2005). Frontostriatal microstructure
Hooper CJ, Luciana M, Conklin HM, Yarger RS (2004). modulates efficient recruitment of cognitive control.
Adolescents’ performance on the Iowa Gambling Task: Cerebral Cortex 16, 553-560.

Clinical Neuropsychiatry (2009) 6, 4 163


Michele Poletti

Luciana M, Conklin MH, Hooper CJ, Yarger RS (2005). The events as predictors of adult depression. Journal of
development of non verbal working memory and executive Affective Disorders 68, 49-57.
control processes in adolescents. Child Development 76, Poletti M (2008). Reward processing in mood disorders. Minerva
697-712. Psichiatrica 49, 297-306.
Luman M, Oosterlan J, Sergeant JA (2005). The impact of Reitan RM (1958). Validity of the Trail Making Test as an
reinforcement contingencies on AD/HD; a review and indication of organic brain damage. Perceptual and Motor
theoretical appraisal. Clinical Psychology Review 25, 183- Skills 8, 271-276.
213. Rolls ET (2004). The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. Brain
Luna B, Garver KE, Urban TA, Lazar NA, Sweeney JA (2004). and Cognition 55, 11-29.
Maturation of cognitive processes from late childhood to Romine C, Reynolds C (2005). A Model of the Development of
adulthood. Child Development 75, 1357-1372. Frontal Lobe Functioning Findings From a Meta-Analysis.
Luna B, Sweeney JA. (2004). The emergence of collaborative Applied Neuropsychology 12, 190-201.
brain function. fMRI studies of the development of Rubia K, Overmeyer S, Taylor E, Brammer E, Williams SC,
response inhibition. Annals of the New York Academy of Simmons A, Andrew C, Bullmore ET (2000). Functional
Sciences 1021, 296-309. frontalization with age: mapping neurodevelopmental
Martel MM, Nikolas M, Nigg JT (2007). Executive function in trajectories with fMRI. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral
adolescents with ADHD. Journal of the American Academy Reviews 24, 13-19.
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 46, 1437-1444. Rutheford LC, Nelson SB, Turrigiano GG (1998). BDNF has
Martinussen R, Hayden J, Hogg-Johnson S, Tannock R (2005). opposite effects on the quantal amplitude of pyramidal
A meta analysis of working memory impairments in neuron and interneuron excitatory synapses. Neuron 21,
children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. 521-530.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Saluja G, Iachan R, Scheidt PC, Overpeck MD, Sun W, Giedd
Psychiatry 44, 377-384. JN (2004). Prevalence of and risk factors for depressive
Mataga N, Mizuguchi Y, Hensch TK (2004). Experience- symptoms among young adolescents. Archives of Pediatric
dependent pruning of dendritic spines in visual cortex by and Adolescent Medicine 158, 760-765.
tissue plasminogen activator. Neuron 44, 1031-1041. Schultz W (1998). Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons.
Miller EK, Cohen JD (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal Journal of Neurophysiology 80, 1-27.
cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience 24, 167- Sergeant J (2000). The cognitive-energetic model: an empirical
202. approach to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 24, 7-12
Wager TD (2000). The unity and diversity of executive Shallice T (1982). Specific impairments in planning.
functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London.
tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology 41, B. Biological Sciences 298, 199-209.
49-100. Shaw P, Kabani NJ, Lerch JP, Eckstrand K, Lenroot R, Gogtay
Muller UC, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Brandeis D, Steinhausen HC N, Greenstein D, Clasen L, Evans A, Rapoport JL, Giedd
(2006). Online measurement of motivational processes: JN, Wise SP (2008). Neurodevelopmental trajectories of
introducing the Continuous Delay Aversion Test the human cerebral cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 28,
(ConDAT). Journal of Neuroscience Methods 151, 45-51. 3586-3594.
Nagy Z, Westerberg, H, Klingberg T (2004). Maturation of white Solanto MV, Abikoff H, Sonuga-Barke E, Schachar R, Logan
matter is associated with the development of cognitive GD, Wigal T, Hechtman L, Hinshaw S, Turkel E (2001).
functions during childhood. Journal of Cognitive The ecological validity of delay aversion and response
Neuroscience 16, 1227-1233. inhibition as measures of impulsivity in AD/HD: a
Nelson EE, Leibenluft E, McClure EB, Pine DS (2004). The supplement to the NIMH multimodal treatment study of
social re-orientation of adolescence: a neuroscience AD/HD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 29, 215-
perspective on the process and its relation to 228.
psychopathology. Psychological Medicine 35, 163-174. Sonuga-Barke, EJ., Dalen L, Remington B (2003). Do executive
Nigg JT (2005). Neuropsychologic theory and findings in deficits and delay aversion make independent contributions
attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder: the state of the field to preschool attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder
and salient challenges for the coming decade. Biological symptoms? Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Psychiatry 57, 1424-1435. Adolescent Psychiatry 42, 335-342.
Nigg JT, Wong MM, Martel MM, Jester JM, Puttler LI, Glass Sonuga-Barke EJ, Sergeant JA, Nigg J, Wilcutt E (2008).
JM, Adams KM, Fitzgerald HE, Zucker RA (2006). Poor Executive dysfunction and delay aversion in Attention
response inhibition as a predictor of problem drinking and Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: nosologic and diagnostic
illicit drug use in adolescents at risk for alcoholism and implications. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of
other substance use disorders. Journal of the American North America 17, 367-384.
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 45, 468-475. Sowell ER, Peterson BS, Thompson PM, Welcome SE,
O’Doherty JP (2004). Reward representations and reward-related Henkenius AL, Toga AW (2003). Mapping cortical change
learning in the human brain: insights from neuroimaging. across the life span. Nature Neuroscience 6, 309-315.
Current Opinion in Neurobiology 14, 769-776. Spessot AL, Plessen KJ, Peterson BJ (2004). Neuroimaging of
O’Hare ED, Lu LH, Houston SM (2008). Neurodevelopmental developmental psychopathologies: the importance of self-
changes in verbal working memory load-dependency: an regulatory and neuroplastic processes in adolescence.
fMRI investigation. Neuroimage 42, 1678-1685. Annals of the New York Academy of Science 1021, 86-104.
Paus T (2005). Mapping brain maturation and cognitive Stefanatos GA, Baron IS (2007). Attention deficit hyperactivity
development during adolescence. Trends in Cognitive disorder: a neuropsychological perspective towards DSM-
Sciences 9, 60-68. V. Neuropsychology Review 17, 39-59.
Paus T, Keshavan M, Giedd JN (2008). Why do many psychiatric Steinberg L (2004). Risk taking in adolescence: what changes
disorders emerge during adolescence? Nature and why? Annals of the New York Academy of Science
Neuroscience 9, 947-957. 1021, 51-58.
Pine DS, Cohen P, Gurley D, Brook JS, Ma Y (1998). The risk Steinberg L (2005). Cognitive and affective development in
of early adulthood anxiety and depressive disorders in adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9, 69-74.
adolescents with anxiety and depressive disorders. Archives Steinberg L (2008). A neurobehavioral perspective on adolescent
of General Psychiatry 55, 56-64. risk taking. Developmental Review 28, 78-106.
Pine DS, Cohen P, Johnson JG, Brook JS (2002). Adolescent life Steinberg L, Monahan KC (2007). Age differences in resistance

164 Clinical Neuropsychiatry (2009) 6, 4


Developmental psychopathologies and prefrontal cortex

to peer influence. Developmental Psychology 43, 1531- Watanabe N, Hunot V, Omori IM, Churchill R, Furukawa TA.
1543. (2007). Psychotherapy for depression among children and
Stroop JR (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal adolescents: a systematic review. Acta Psychiatrica
reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology 18, 643- Scandinavica 116, 84-95.
662. Weisz JR, McCarthy MC, Valeri SM. (2006). Effects of
Stuss DT (1992). Biological and psychological development of psychotherapy for depression in children and adolescents:
executive functions. Brain and Cognition 20, 8-23. a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 132, 132-149.
Stuss D, Levine B (2002). Adult clinical neuropsychology: Welsh MC, Pennington BF (1988). Assessing frontal lobe
lessons from studies of the frontal lobes. Annual Review functioning in children: Views from developmental
of Psychology 53, 401-433. psychology. Developmental Neuropsychology 4, 199-230.
Sur M, Rubinstein JL (2005). Patterning and plasticity of cerebral Welsh MC, Pennington BF, Groisser DB (1991). A normative
cortex. Science 310, 805-810. developmental study of executive function: A window on
Swanson JM, Kinsbourne M, Nigg J, Lanphear B, Stefanatos prefrontal function in children. Developmental
GA, Volkow N, Taylor E, Casey BJ, Castellanos FX, Neuropsychology 7, 131-149.
Wadhwa PD (2007). Etiologic subtypes of Attention-De- Wilcutt EG, Doyle AE, Nigg JT, Faraone SV, Pennington BF
ficit/ Hyperactivity disorder: brain imaging molecular (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of
genetic and environmental factors and the dopamine attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta analytic
hypothesis. Neuropsychology Review 17, 39-59. review. Biological Psychiatry 57, 1336-1346.
Tamm L, Menon V, Reiss AL (2002). Maturation of brain function Wolff JC, Ollendick TH (2006). The comorbidity of conduct
associated with response inhibition. Journal of the problems and depression in childhood and adolescence.
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Clinical Child and Family Psychology, Review 9, 201-220.
41, 1231-1238. Yap MB, Allen NB, Sheeber L (2007). Using an emotion
Thompson PM, Sowell ER, Gogtay N (2005). Structural MRI regulation framework to understand the role of
and brain development. International Review of temperament and family processes in risk for adolescent
Neurobiology 67, 285-323. depressive disorders. Clinical Child and Family
Thorell LB (2007). Do delay aversion and executive function Psychology 10, 180-196.
deficits make distinct contributions to the functional impact Yap MB, Whittle S, Yucel M, Sheeber L, Pantelis C, Simmons
of ADHD symptoms? A study of early academic skill JG, Allen NB (2008). Interaction of parenting experiences
deficits. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 48, and brain structures in the prediction of depressive
1061-1070. symptoms in adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry
Toga AW, Thompson PM, Sowell ER (2006). Mapping brain 65, 1377-1385.
maturation. Trends in Neuroscience 29, 148-159. Yeo R, Hill D, Campbell R, Vigil J, Petropoulos H, Hart B,
Toplak ME, Jain U, Tannock R (2005). Executive and Zamora L, Brooks WM (2003). Proton magnetic resonance
motivational processes in adolescents with Attention De- spectroscopy investigation of the right frontal lobe in
ficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Behavioral and children with ADHD. Journal of the American Academy
Brain Functions 27, 1-8. of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 42, 303-310.
Tsapakis EM, Soldani F, Tondo L, Baldessarini RJ. (2008). Yurgelun Todd DA (2007). Emotional and cognitive changes
Effects of antidepressants in juvenile depression: meta- during adolescence. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 17,
analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry 193, 10-17. 251-257.
Waller E, Scheidt CE (2006). Somatoform disorders as disorders Zelazo PD, Mueller U (2002). Executive function in typical and
of affect regulation: a development perspective. atypical development. In U. Goswami (ed) Handbook of
International Review of Psychiatry 18, 13-24. childhood cognitive development. Oxford: Blackwell.
Wallis JD (2007). The orbitofrontal cortex and its contribution
to decision making. Annual Review of Neuroscience 30,
31-56.

Clinical Neuropsychiatry (2009) 6, 4 165

Вам также может понравиться