Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 17

Developmental Psychology In the public domain

2010, Vol. 46, No. 6, 1677–1693 DOI: 10.1037/a0020677

Parenting Knowledge: Experiential and Sociodemographic Factors in


European American Mothers of Young Children
Marc H. Bornstein, Linda R. Cote, O. Maurice Haynes, Chun-Shin Hahn, and Yoonjung Park
Child and Family Research, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development

Knowledge of child rearing and child development is relevant to parenting and the well-being of children.
Using a sociodemographically heterogeneous sample of 268 European American mothers of 2-year-olds,
we assessed the state of mothers’ parenting knowledge; compared parenting knowledge in groups of
mothers who varied in terms of parenthood and social status; and identified principal sources of mothers’
parenting knowledge in terms of social factors, parenting supports, and formal classes. On the whole,
European American mothers demonstrated fair but less than complete basic parenting knowledge; age,
education, and rated helpfulness of written materials each uniquely contributed to mothers’ knowledge.
Adult mothers scored higher than adolescent mothers, and mothers improved in their knowledge of
parenting from their first to their second child (and were stable across time). No differences were found
between mothers of girls and boys, mothers who varied in employment status, or birth and adoptive
mothers. The implications of variation in parenting knowledge and its sources for parenting education
and clinical interactions with parents are discussed.

Keywords: parenting, knowledge, toddlers

Trust yourself . . . . You know more than you think you do. develop; parents’ understanding of normative child development
—Benjamin Spock, Baby and Child Care (i.e., both developmental processes and the abilities and accom-
plishments of children as they grow); and parents’ awareness of
A major focus of contemporary parenting studies is cognitions, practices and strategies for maintaining and promoting children’s
broadly construed to include parents’ values, goals, beliefs, and health and coping effectively with children’s illness (Bornstein,
attitudes as well as actual parenting knowledge of child rearing 2006; Goodnow & Collins, 1990; MacPhee, 1981). In turn, par-
and child development. Whereas values, goals, beliefs, attitudes, enting knowledge about such topics as proper parenting, norms
and other like cognitions may or may not be factual, parenting and milestones, and health and safety is believed to shape parents’
knowledge is thought to draw on the science base, in addition to its other cognitions and practices, to influence child development, and
social construction, and is regarded as valid and reliable by mem- to portend consequences for children’s health and well-being. The
bers of the clinical and research communities. The present study literature in parenting knowledge is largely based on studies that
concerns itself with what European American mothers of young include participation of varying ethnicities and children of various
children in different walks of life know substantively about child ages. These factors moderate their results. In this study (as we
rearing and child development, how levels of parenting knowledge point out), we sampled from the European American population of
vary among mothers of different parenthood and social statuses, mothers with young children. Most of the studies we review (but
and which factors relate to variation in parenting knowledge. not all) had a similar sample composition.
First, insights into the patterns and processes of child rearing
The Significance of Parenting Knowledge and child development are thought to shape parenting. For exam-
Parenting knowledge of child rearing and child development ple, parenting knowledge is associated with enhanced parental
encompasses many domains: parents’ cognitions about various self-perceptions of competence, satisfaction, and investment in
approaches appropriate to fulfilling the biological and physical as parenting (Bornstein, Hendricks, et al., 2003). The relation be-
well as socioemotional and cognitive needs of children as they tween parental self-efficacy and parenting competence is also
moderated by parenting knowledge: Parental self-efficacy and
parenting competence are positively associated when parenting
knowledge is high; by contrast, self-efficacy and competence are
This article was published Online First September 13, 2010. inversely associated when knowledge is low (Hess, Teti, &
Marc H. Bornstein, Linda R. Cote, O. Maurice Haynes, Chun-Shin Hussey-Gardner, 2004). Parenting knowledge has also been im-
Hahn, and Yoonjung Park, Child and Family Research, Eunice Kennedy plicated in parents’ more accurate interpretations of children’s
Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.
behaviors (Bugental & Happaney, 2002). In these senses, parent-
We thank Melanie Sandoval, Joan T. D. Suwalsky, and Teresa Taylor.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Marc H.
ing knowledge may underpin parents’ global cognitive organiza-
Bornstein, Child and Family Research, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National tion for adapting to or anticipating developmental changes in
Institute of Child Health and Human Development, National Institutes of children (Stoiber & Houghton, 1993).
Health, Suite 8030, 6705 Rockledge Drive, Bethesda, MD 20892-7971. Beyond cognitions, parenting knowledge may inform parenting
E-mail: Marc_H_Bornstein@nih.gov practices. It has been argued that acting appropriately as a parent

1677
1678 BORNSTEIN, COTE, HAYNES, HAHN, AND PARK

depends, at least in part, on knowledge of principles related to tively to substantial variation in parenting knowledge (e.g., Civitas
child development (Donahue, Pearl, & Herzog, 1997). Mothers Initiative, Zero to Three, & Brio Corporation, 2000; Clarke-
who know more about child development report more positive Stewart, 1978, 1998; Oldershaw, 2002; Young, 1991). However,
interactions with and give more positive descriptions of their empirical studies of parenting knowledge have largely concen-
children (Chamberlin, Szumowski, & Zastowny, 1979; Grusec & trated on nonnormative risk samples (e.g., mothers of low birth
Goodnow, 1994), and they score higher in parenting skills, even weight and preterm infants), and most studies have been limited to
controlling for income and education (Booth, Mitchell, Barnard, & knowledge of developmental milestones or parents’ perceptions of
Spieker, 1989; Conrad, Gross, Fogg, & Ruchala, 1992; Reis, relations between caregiving practices and developmental out-
Barbera-Stein, & Bennett, 1986; Stevens, 1984). Variation in comes (Parks & Smeriglio, 1983; Sistler & Gottfried, 1990; Smeri-
maternal knowledge about child play relates to mothers’ style of glio & Parks, 1983; Stevens, 1984). We attempted to extend this
play with children (Damast, Tamis-LeMonda, & Bornstein, 1996; tradition by exploring parenting knowledge in a wide variety of
Tamis-LeMonda, Damast, & Bornstein, 1994; see also Gross, mothers in different walks of life. Our first question asked, How
Conrad, Fogg, Willis, & Garvey, 1993) and to children’s positive does parenting knowledge vary with family sociodemographic
development (Benasich & Brooks-Gunn, 1996; Goodnow, 1988; characteristics, specifically, gender of the child and maternal age,
Miller, 1988; Sigel, 1992); knowledge of developmental mile- education, socioeconomic and employment status, parity, and birth
stones also relates to when parents introduce new experiences, versus adoptive status as a parent? For this study, we recruited and
such as telling stories or reading to their child (Frankel & Roer- tested a large, sociodemographically heterogeneous sample of
Bornstein, 1982; Hess, Kashiwagi, Azuma, Price, & Dickson, mothers in the United States to discern how these prominent
1980; Ninio, 1979). characteristics relate to parenting knowledge. Here we briefly
Because parents are the main caregivers of young children, the review relevant literature on how parenting knowledge varies with
extent and quality of their parenting knowledge is often considered each characteristic.
vital to improving children’s development and health. Mothers’ Is there reason to believe that rearing a child of one or the other
parenting knowledge is related to children’s performance on the gender is regularly associated with parenting knowledge? Little is
Bayley Scales of Infant Development (Dichtelmiller et al., 1992), known about whether mothers of girls know more or less than
and mothers with greater parenting knowledge have children who mothers of boys about child rearing and child development, but
display fewer behavior problems (Benasich & Brooks-Gunn, 1996; there is evidence that child gender systematically moderates other
Huang, Caughy, Genevro, & Miller, 2005; McGillicuddy-DeLisi, parental cognitions and decision making. For example, Monds-
1982a, 1982b; Stevens, 1984); reciprocally, parents’ inaccurate chein, Adolph, and Tamis-LeMonda (2000) found that mothers of
beliefs and misconceptions are thought to undermine child devel- 11-month-old male infants tended to overestimate how well their
opment (Miller, 1989; Miller, Manhal, & Mee, 1991; Stoiber, babies would crawl down a sloped pathway, whereas mothers of
1992). Better informed parents might recognize earlier and more 11-month-old female infants tended to underestimate how well
effectively address developmental problems. This is important, their babies would do (when subsequent objective tests of crawling
because early intervention is key to preventing long-term adverse ability on the slope revealed no gender differences in infant crawl-
outcomes in children. Moreover, parenting knowledge is thought ing). Similarly, girls with language delays are 60% more likely
to guide parents’ decisions about how to maintain child health, than boys with the same diagnosis to be referred for audiological
how to prevent illness, when to seek health care for children, and assessment (Sices, Feudtner, McLaughlin, Drotar, & Williams,
how to use medical services effectively (Hickson & Clayton, 2002; 2004). Findings such as these suggest that it is important to
Melamed, 2002). Finally, parenting knowledge is relevant to clin- separate empirical and scientific knowledge about child rearing
ical practice with children (Bornstein & Cote, 2004; Pachter & and child development from social constructions.
Dworkin, 1997). The Institute of Medicine (1994) formally recog- How does maternal age relate to parenting knowledge? Teenage
nized the importance of children’s primary care physicians “de- motherhood is epidemic in the United States (Martin et al., 2009;
veloping a sustained partnership with patients and practicing in Singh & Darroch, 2000; see also Williams & Decouflé, 1999), as
the context of family and community” (p. 16). During child health approximately 34% of all girls become pregnant by the end of their
visits, clinicians ask about and must interpret parents’ (typically 19th year (National Campaign to Prevent Teen Pregnancy, 2004).
mothers’) observations, expectations, concerns, and opinions about At the same time, increasing numbers of adult women are delaying
children’s health and development. Accurate and complete devel- conception (Martin et al., 2009; Mirowsky, 2002; Paulsen &
opmental anamnesis and surveillance thus depend in large measure Sachs, 1998). These twin demographics expand the age range for
on parents’ knowledge base. Parents have the most experience pregnancy and birth and raise questions about which associations
with their child, are believed to know their child best, and are might obtain between mothers’ age and parenting knowledge. To
routinely the clinician’s primary source of outside information date, individual studies have suggested that adult mothers possess
about the child. more parenting knowledge than do adolescent mothers (Benasich
& Brooks-Gunn, 1996; de Lissovoy, 1973; Dichtelmiller et al.,
Two Questions About Parenting Knowledge 1992; Field, Widmayer, Stringer, & Ignatoff, 1980; Hammond-
Ratzlaff & Fulton, 2001; Karraker & Evans, 1996; McGillicuddy-
These global considerations about the significance of parenting DeLisi, 1982a, 1982b; Roosa & Vaughan, 1984; Ruchala & James,
knowledge to parenting cognitions and practices, children’s devel- 1997; Stevens, 1984) as have most reviews (e.g., Brooks-Gunn &
opment and well-being, as well as clinical decision making and Furstenberg, 1986; Moore & Brooks-Gunn, 2002; Whitman,
health gave rise to the two general questions that motivated this Borkowski, Schellenbach, & Nath, 1987). However, other studies
research. Past small- and large-scale surveys have pointed descrip- have failed to uncover systematic relations between parenting
MOTHERS’ KNOWLEDGE OF CHILD DEVELOPMENT 1679

knowledge and age among adult mothers (Conrad et al., 1992; Extant investigations comparing primiparous and multiparous
Schilmoeller & Baranowski, 1985). Is the critical difference be- parents suggest (surprisingly) that neither parity (number of chil-
tween teens and adults? Unfortunately, many group comparisons dren) nor birth order (position of child) is strictly associated with
of adolescent versus adult mothers have confounded socioeco- parenting knowledge (Conrad et al., 1992; Dichtelmiller et al.,
nomic status as well as ethnicity, focused on low-income, inner- 1992; MacPhee, 1981, 1983; McGillicuddy-DeLisi, 1982a,
city minority women, and treated age dichotomously (e.g., Bav- 1982b), even though firstborns are reared by presumably less
olek, 1989; Parks & Smeriglio, 1983; Stern & Alvarez, 1992; experienced parents who may be less at ease in their new role and
Tamis-LeMonda, Shannon, & Spellman, 2002). In the present later borns are reared by parents who are more experienced and,
design and analyses, we disentangle socioeconomic status and presumably, more competent (Kreppner, 1992). Other aspects of
ethnicity from age by controlling socioeconomic status (SES) and primiparous and multiparous parenting differ. For example, par-
focusing on parenting knowledge in a single (European American) ents with one child tend to underestimate the age at which children
ethnic group (practicably significant, too, because European begin to speak a complete sentence or sleep through the night
Americans are numerically and proportionately the most populous (Waddell & Ball, 1980); parents with two children reportedly have
group of adolescent mothers; Martin et al., 2009). We address the higher achievement expectations for their firstborns in comparison
age question in two ways: by comparing adolescent and adult to their later borns (Kammeyer, 1967). Unfortunately, many stud-
mothers residing in the same ecological geographic context and by ies of parenting first- versus later borns have been plagued by
correlating age as a continuous variable with parenting knowledge methodological shortcomings (Michalski & Shackelford, 2001;
in a larger sociodemographically heterogeneous sample. Price & Hare, 1969): Not only was the ordinal position of later
The “knowledge gap hypothesis” proposes that, as information borns often heterogeneous or not specified, but most studies have
flows into a social system, higher SES groups are more likely to compared firstborns and later borns from different families. For
acquire it at a faster rate than lower SES groups (Tichenor, these reasons, we utilized a rarer but more refined, within-family
Donohue, & Olien, 1970). SES is generally influential in parent- design asking about parenting knowledge in the same mothers of
ing: SES orders the home environment and other practices of two children born into the same family, and we limited our
parents, such as the likelihood that parents read books on child care comparison to first- versus secondborns.
No studies comparing the parenting knowledge of parents who
and serve their children healthy food (Bornstein & Bradley, 2003;
give birth and those who adopt have been reported to date. A few
Hoff, Laursen, & Tardif, 2002). Parents in higher socioeconomic
studies that have compared other aspects of parenting and child
strata change more flexibly and more rapidly in response to
development in these groups suggest that, if any differences exist
changes in developmental theory than do parents in lower socio-
in the early years, they may be small: For example, microanalysis
economic strata (Alwin, 1990; Bronfenbrenner, 1958). Parental
of interactions has shown that birth and adoptive mothers behave
knowledge of child rearing and child development has been found
similarly with their young infants (Suwalsky, Hendricks, & Born-
to moderate the relation between SES and parent behavior (Rowe,
stein, 2008a, 2008b), and attachment relationships of adoptive
2008), and low SES is associated with lower levels of maternal
mothers with their children appear not to differ from those of birth
knowledge (Benasich & Brooks-Gunn, 1996; Conrad et al., 1992;
mothers with their children (Juffer & Rosenboom, 1997). None-
Stevens, 1984). Education is an integral component of SES (Born-
theless, some nonbiological mothers are thought to struggle in
stein & Bradley, 2003), and educational achievement is related to identifying with the maternal role, which birth mothers may em-
parenting knowledge (Conrad et al., 1992; McGillicuddy-DeLisi, brace more readily (Gabb, 2005). Here, we asked whether biolog-
1982a, 1982b; Reich, 2005; Richman, Miller, & LeVine, 1992). ical connection to a young child moderates mothers’ parenting
When mothers of preschool-age children were asked to estimate knowledge.
the ages at which their child would acquire a variety of perceptual– Our second question asked, What are the primary sources of
motor, cognitive, and psychosocial skills (Williams, Williams, parenting knowledge? This study investigated maternal parenting
Lopez, & Tayko, 2000), mothers with higher educational attain- knowledge in U.S. groups varying in sociodemographic status as
ment correctly voiced earlier expectations. Accurate information well as the selective contributions of various sources of support to
about infant development correlates with amounts of formal ex- maternal parenting knowledge. Research has identified multiple
perience with young children: MacPhee (1981) reported that par- antecedents of parenting knowledge (Goodnow & Collins, 1990;
enting knowledge relates to degree of practical and professional MacPhee, 1981). Parenting information and advice vary in types of
experience. In short, parents higher in SES and those with more sources, frequency of use, reasons for use, and perceived benefit,
education may possess more knowledge of child rearing and child and mothers commonly rely on their own experiences as well as
development (Gross et al., 1993; Palacios, 1990; Parks & Smeri- lay and professional resources for information (Civitas Initiative et
glio, 1986). al., 2000; McKim, 1987; Vukelich & Kliman, 1985; Wolf, 1998).
We could find no research that compared parenting knowledge By contrast with formal, objective sources— books and classes
per se in mothers who are employed full-time outside the home about child rearing and child development—informal or subjective
and mothers who are full-time homemakers. However, more moth- experiences with children (through, e.g., babysitting) have been
ers participate in the labor force than ever before, relations be- found to correlate not at all or negatively with parenting knowl-
tween employment status and parenting are of increasing moment, edge (Frankel & Roer-Bornstein, 1982; MacPhee, 1981). Coch-
and studies comparing parenting principles and practices relative ran’s model of parenting and personal social networks (Cochran,
to employment status have pointed to mixed results that invite 1990; Cochran & Niego, 2002) suggests that parents generally rely
additional research (e.g., Gottfried, Gottfried, & Bathurst, 2002; on familiar and proximal sources for parenting information, turn-
Waldfogel, 2006). ing initially to family and friends (Belsky, Youngblade, & Pensky,
1680 BORNSTEIN, COTE, HAYNES, HAHN, AND PARK

1989; Sparling & Lowman, 1983). For example, in the Civitas source most often consulted about emergent or specific medical
Initiative et al. (2000) national survey, mothers of children under problems (Hickson & Clayton, 2002; Hulbert, 2003). In the Civitas
3 years reported that they relied on their spouse (70%) and their Initiative et al. (2000) survey, mothers reported that they relied
mothers (66%). The parenting knowledge of a mother relates to her frequently on their child’s doctor/pediatrician (54%), nurses
own mother’s knowledge, even when contact between the two is (25%), and child-care providers (20%) for information and advice.
minimal (Sistler & Gottfried, 1990). New parents less frequently At the same time, parents reported that many anticipatory guidance
mention child-care advice from in-laws (Belsky & Kelly, 1994; topics are not covered in well-child visits, and that even after such
Civitas Initiative et al., 2000). visits, they can still use more information (Sanghavi, 2005). When
When faced with a need for special information, parents sup- parents were asked whether they had any developmental, behav-
plement advice from their personal social networks with additional ioral, or health concerns about their child and whether they shared
sources, such as a written materials, education programs, or pro- those concerns with a professional, 29% of parents reported at least
fessionals (Geboy, 1981). Clarke-Stewart (1978, 1998) reported one serious concern, and more than 20% had not shared that
that books and magazines were popular sources of information concern (Golan et al., 2008).
about general child development, but their most frequent users
were first-time and middle-class parents (Deutsch, Ruble, Fleming, Overview
Brooks-Gunn, & Stangor, 1988). In the Civitas Initiative et al.
(2000) survey, only 34% of mothers reported relying frequently on We assessed the state of mothers’ parenting knowledge in
books; 15%–22% reported relying on the news and magazines. groups of mothers who varied in terms of parenthood and socio-
Mothers with greater amounts of formal education normally ex- demographic status, and we identified principal sources of moth-
press a preference for education materials (Vukelich & Kliman, ers’ parenting knowledge in terms of sociodemographics, diverse
1985). Newsletters have proved effective in improving parenting parenting supports, and formal classes. We addressed these ques-
among mothers who are younger, are less experienced, have less tions in a large, sociodemographically diverse sample of U.S.
education, and are more socially isolated (Riley, Salisbury, European American mothers of 2-year-olds.
Walker, & Steinberg, 1996). Parenting books have long had mass
appeal (Spock, 1946), and in contemporary times parents have Method
turned to related media sources, such as CDs, TV, and the World
Wide Web (Wolf, 1998). Friends and neighbors are another com- Participants
mon source of information cited by parents (Clarke-Stewart, 1978;
Ninio, 1979), including 23% of mothers in the Civitas Initiative et Altogether, 269 European American mothers of 20-month-olds,
al. (2000) survey. In a comparative evaluation of 10 sources of 123 mothers of girls and 146 mothers of boys, participated. One
information, mothers reported an average of four very useful mother’s Knowledge of Infant Development Inventory score was
sources, with the baby’s grandparent the most used informal more than three standard deviations from the mean, and so she was
source and newsletters the most used professional source (Walker, omitted from all analyses. Sociodemographic information on the
2007). participating 268 mothers and their families appears in Table 1.
Formal education programs offer yet another way for parents to Mothers ranged in age from 16.39 to 47.26 years. Twenty-two
improve their parenting knowledge. Although health care profes- mothers had not completed high school, 30 had completed high
sionals are less immediately accessible, they constitute an impor- school, 53 had some college, 84 had completed college, and 79 had
tant reserve for all ages and social classes of parent and are the enrolled in or completed university graduate programs. Of the

Table 1
Sample Sociodemographic Data and Correlations With KIDI Proportion Correct

Correlation with KIDI proportion correct

Sociodemographic M SD Zero-order Controlling for mothers’ age Controlling for mothers’ education

Mother
Age (years) 31.11 6.27 .46ⴱⴱⴱ .15ⴱ
Educationa 5.59 1.35 .49ⴱⴱⴱ .21ⴱⴱⴱ
Occupational status 6.9 1.6 .19ⴱⴱ .06 .02
No. hours employed weeklyb 20.43 19.04 .05 .02 ⫺.03
Child
Age (months) 20.08 .23 ⫺.10 ⫺.06 ⫺.03
Birth weight (grams) 3,522.94 447.18 .04 .06 .04
Family
Hollingshead Index 49.96 13.23 .43ⴱⴱⴱ .14ⴱ .02

Note. N ⫽ 268. KIDI ⫽ Knowledge of Infant Development Inventory.


a
The education factor in the Four-Factor Index of Social Status (Hollingshead, 1975) was measured with a 7-point scale (1 ⫽ less than 7th grade; 2 ⫽
7th, 8th, 9th grade; 3 ⫽ 10th or 11th grade; 4 ⫽ high school graduate or GED; 5 ⫽ partial college or specialized training; 6 ⫽ standard college or
university graduate; 7 ⫽ graduate professional training). b Includes homemaker mothers.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
MOTHERS’ KNOWLEDGE OF CHILD DEVELOPMENT 1681

mothers, 167 were employed part time to full time outside of the collection on parenting knowledge to avoid these transient and
home; their occupational statuses ranged from menial labor to temporal vicissitudes.
higher executives or major professionals. Families were of low to
high SES (Hollingshead, 1975; range ⫽ 14 – 66). All mothers were
Instruments
living with the baby’s father (whether they were married or not).
As child rearing and child development are known to vary with Parenting knowledge. The Knowledge of Infant Develop-
ethnicity and culture (Bornstein, 1991; Garcı́a Coll & Pachter, ment Inventory (KIDI; MacPhee, 1981; see also Miller, 1988) is a
2002), we recruited an ethnically homogeneous community sample 75-item, criteria-referenced questionnaire that assesses evidence-
as a first step in understanding the matrix of associations surround- based knowledge of parental practices, developmental processes,
ing maternal parenting knowledge before embarking on more health and safety guidelines, and norms and milestones relevant to
complex studies and analyses with ethnically diverse samples. children’s growth from birth to 2 years of age. Each KIDI item
Approximately 75% of the population of the United States self- targets information that pediatric professionals believe all mothers
identifies as European American (Schmidley, 2001; Tilton-Weaver should know. Thus, the instrument measures a professional-based
& Kakihara, 2008; U.S. Census Bureau, 2004). By including only criterion of knowledge, minimizing subjectivity and social con-
European American mothers, we intentionally avoided an ethnicity struction, instead of comparing a sample’s knowledge to that of
confound that has plagued the existing literature, as it would cloud other respondents (as a normative assessment would; Nunnally &
our findings with respect to parenting knowledge. Children aver- Bernstein, 1994). The KIDI contains 48 items that ask respondents
aged 20.08 months (SD ⫽ 0.23, range ⫽ 19.45–20.93) and were to indicate whether they agree, disagree, or are unsure about a
healthy. They were born at term, weighed at least 2,500 g at birth, series of statements (e.g., “Baby girls are fragile and sick more
and were firstborn (with the exception of the n ⫽ 45 within-family often, so they need to be treated more carefully than boys”). An
firstborn–secondborn comparison). Approximately equal numbers additional 20 items ask parents to choose among four responses—
of mothers of girls and boys participated. We recruited mothers agree, younger, older, or unsure—in relation to statements about
from the Washington, DC, metropolitan area, including Maryland when a child should be able to achieve particular milestones (e.g.,
and Virginia, and from West Virginia using methods common to “Most infants are ready to be toilet trained by one year of age”).
developmental research with young children, including mass mail- The remaining seven items either are questions or are sentence
ings, hospital birth notifications, patient lists of medical groups, completions with five possible answers (e.g., “Select the most
and newspapers. appropriate game for a one-year-old: (a) stringing small beads, (b)
We collected data about parenting knowledge and its sources cutting out shapes with scissors, (c) rolling a ball back and forth
directly from mothers. Research on parenting attitudes and activ- with an adult, (d) sorting things by shape and color, (e) not sure”).
ities has historically concentrated on mothers, acknowledging that All items are worded to be accessible to individuals with no more
they have traditionally (and across cultures) assumed primary—if than a sixth-grade education and to be free of sociocultural biases
not exclusive—responsibility for early child care; that they partic- (MacPhee, 1981). Responses were scored as 1 if the respondent
ipate in child-rearing activities at significantly higher rates and agreed with the dominant response of clinicians and developmen-
garner more experience and knowledge than do fathers or other tal scientists in the standardization sample and as 0 if the respon-
caregivers; and that, beginning prenatally, mothering is unequiv- dent did not agree. We used proportion correct of total items as our
ocally principal for the health and development of infants and primary dependent measure.
young children (Barnard & Solchany, 2002; Geary, 2000; Leider- As reported in MacPhee’s (1981) technical manual, the KIDI
man, Tulkin, & Rosenfeld, 1977; Parke, 2002; Weisner & Galli- was standardized on four groups: college students, mothers,
more, 1977). Given societal dictates that it is mothers who are doctoral-level psychologists, and pediatricians. The proportion of
ultimately responsible for their very young children’s lives and total items that were correct was positively and monotonically
health, women have normally become more expert than men and related to the degree of practical and professional experience with
actively seek child-relevant information during the early stages of children: Mean proportions correct equaled .62, .72, .86, and .87
child rearing (Stern, 1995). Not unexpectedly, maternal parenting for college students, mothers, developmental psychologists, and
is most consistently associated with developmental and health pediatricians, respectively (MacPhee, 1981). The split-half reli-
outcomes in children (e.g., Crouter, Helms-Erickson, Updegraff, & ability coefficient of the KIDI for mothers is .85; the two-week
McHale, 1999; Rothbaum & Weisz, 1994). retest coefficient is r(58) ⫽ .92; and the KIDI is also highly stable
We studied mothers’ parenting knowledge toward the end of the (r ⫽ .61 from 12 to 24 months; Benasich & Brooks-Gunn, 1996).
child’s second year (at 20 months), first, because the instrument Among our total sample of mothers, alpha was .67.
we used to assess parenting knowledge covered the child’s first 2 The KIDI is arguably the most widely used measure of parent-
years (so mothers would have had recent or current experience ing knowledge (see, e.g., Conrad et al., 1992; Dichtelmiller et al.,
with the items on the measure). Second, the samples consisted 1992; Gross et al., 1993; MacPhee, 1984; Pachter & Dworkin,
almost exclusively of first-time mothers, and soon after parturition 1997; Roggman, Benson, & Boyce, 1999; Ruchala & James, 1997;
many women experience fluctuations in mood (Cooper & Murray, Thompson, Gustafson, Bonner, & Ware, 2002; Wacharasin, 2001).
1998) and marital satisfaction (Belsky, Hertzog, & Rovine, 1986; It is a principal measure used in the Early Head Start Research and
Cowan et al., 1985) as well as stresses associated with caring for Evaluation Project (U.S. Department of Health and Human Ser-
a very young infant (Copeland & Harbaugh, 2005; Paris & Dubus, vices, Administration for Children and Families, n.d.) conducted
2005). Because we wanted information from mothers who were by the Administration for Children and Families, U.S. Department
settled in the maternal role and whose cognitions about parenting of Health and Human Services (http://www.acf.hhs.gov/), as well
had had time (since parturition) to stabilize, we scheduled data as the Infant Health and Development Program (Gordon, Chase-
1682 BORNSTEIN, COTE, HAYNES, HAHN, AND PARK

Lansdale, & Brooks-Gunn, 2004) and Healthy Steps for Young significantly ( p ⬍ .05) with parenting knowledge and the effects
Children (http://www.healthysteps.org), a national (15-site), of the covariates were significant. For each covariate, the assump-
pediatric-based parent program designed to prospectively address tion of homogeneity of regression slopes was checked, but the
behavioral and developmental support needs of young families in assumption was never violated. Partial eta squared (␩2p) was used
pediatric clinical care settings (Caughy, Miller, Genevro, Huang, as an effect size, with ␩2p ⬇ .01 interpreted as a small effect, ␩2p ⬇
& Nautiyal, 2003; Guyer, Hughart, Strobino, Jones, & Scharfstein, .06 interpreted as a medium effect, and ␩2p ⬇ .14 interpreted as a
2000; Johnston, Huebner, Tyll, Barlow, & Thompson, 2004; large effect (Cohen, 1988).
Minkovitz et al., 2003). The KIDI has demonstrated construct Differences in parenting knowledge were investigated in two
validity: It is related to other measures of mothers’ knowledge of ways relative to maternal sociodemographic characteristics. First,
children’s development (the Concepts of Development Question- mothers were compared to see whether differences in parenting
naire; Benasich & Brooks-Gunn, 1996) and to children’s actual knowledge emerged based on grouping variables of child gender
development (e.g., Dichtelmiller et al., 1992). (mothers of girls vs. mothers of boys), maternal age (adolescent vs.
Sociodemographic information. Data on maternal education adult), employment (full-time employed vs. part-time employed
and occupation and family SES were gathered with the Family vs. homemakers), parity (primiparas vs. multiparas), and parent-
Description Questionnaire (Bornstein, Tamis-LeMonda, Suwal- hood genesis (adoptive vs. birth). Second, relations between moth-
sky, & Haynes, 1991). These data were used to calculate the ers’ sociodemographic characteristics and sources of parenting
Four-Factor Index of Social Status (Hollingshead, 1975; see Born- supports for parenting knowledge were analyzed. The predictive
stein, Hahn, Suwalsky, & Haynes, 2003). validity of mothers’ sociodemographic characteristics and moth-
Parenting support. Using the Parenting Support Measure ers’ supports to KIDI scores was then investigated in a three-step
(Suwalsky & Bornstein, 1991), mothers rated the helpfulness of process: First, zero-order correlations of KIDI scores with moth-
nine parenting resources, including the child’s father, books or ers’ sociodemographic characteristics and supports were evalu-
other written materials, mother’s mother, spouse’s mother, other ated, with r ⬇ .10 interpreted as a small effect, r ⬇ .25 interpreted
relatives, pediatrician, friends or neighbors, another significant as a medium effect, and r ⬇ .40 interpreted as a large effect
person, or an organized group. Parenting support was measured on (Cohen, 1988). Because mothers’ knowledge was expected to be
a 5-point Likert scale (0 ⫽ Of little or no help or not applicable to correlated with mothers’ age and education and because, as ex-
4 ⫽ Very helpful). This information was collected when children pected, mothers’ age and education were highly correlated,
were 5 months of age, because the demands of an infant on a new r(266) ⫽ .68, p ⬍ .001, correlations of the KIDI with the other
mother at home are compelling and the mother’s early parenting sociodemographic measures and supports were evaluated with
support has been shown to be critical both to child development residuals of the KIDI partialed for mothers’ age and separately for
and to parenting (Crockenberg, 1981). In addition, in the Family mothers’ education. Finally, if significant relations between moth-
Description Questionnaire mothers were asked about their preg- ers’ sociodemographic characteristics or supports and KIDI scores
nancy and childbirth experiences, including whether or not they were found, their unique predictive ability was assessed with
had taken a childbirth class (“Did you prepare for labor and hierarchical regression analyses.
delivery by attending a prepared childbirth course [for example,
Lamaze, etc.?”] or parenting class (“Have you taken a formal class
or course dealing with parenting issues and skills?”). Mothers
Results
responded to each of these questions by circling either yes or no.
Whole Sample
Preliminary Data Analysis On average, in the total U.S. sample mothers answered .81 of
We examined all data for univariate and influential bivariate the KIDI items correctly (SD ⫽ .07, range ⫽ .52–.95).
outliers using scatter plots and numeric statistics: the studentized
deleted residual, leverage, and Cook’s D. Initial estimates of Parenting Knowledge by Parenting and
optimal transformations were sought by evaluating inverse fitted- Social Status Groups
value plots and output from Box–Cox methods (Cook & Weisberg,
1999). The KIDI proportion correct score was raised to the third As shown in Table 1, significant and meaningful correlations
power to approximate normality and reduce the number and influ- between mothers’ KIDI scores and age, education, and SES
ence of outliers (Cook & Weisberg, 1999; Fox, 1997; Tabachnick emerged. Mothers’ age and education (but not SES) were therefore
& Fidell, 2001). Transformed variables were used in all analyses; used as covariates for comparisons involving all participating
however, untransformed values are reported to facilitate interpre- mothers (between mothers of girls and mothers of boys and among
tation. Not all mothers provided complete data on measures of mothers of varying employment status). Mothers’ education level,
parenting support; in all the analyses, the maximum available n rather than family SES, was used as a covariate, because mothers’
was used. education level is a weighted component of SES and education is
a better predictor of parenting (Bornstein et al., 2003). In compar-
Analytic Strategy isons that involved only subgroups of mothers (between adoptive
and birth mothers and for mothers when primiparous then multip-
Analyses of variance (ANOVA) and covariance were used to arous), we evaluated mothers’ age and/or education as covariates
test group differences. Mothers’ age and education level were by examining their correlations with KIDI scores in the relevant
screened as covariates; they were used only if they correlated subgroup.
MOTHERS’ KNOWLEDGE OF CHILD DEVELOPMENT 1683

Mothers of girls and mothers of boys. Mothers of girls SES, and maternal hours of employment, considered in that order.
(estimated marginal means [EMM] ⫽ .54, SE ⫽ .01) and mothers A repeated-measures ANOVA showed no difference on KIDI
of boys (EMM ⫽ .54, SE ⫽ .01) did not differ in their KIDI scores, between adoptive mothers (M ⫽ .83, SD ⫽ .07) and birth mothers
controlling for mother age and education, F(1, 264) ⫽ 0.24, ns. (M ⫽ .83, SD ⫽ .04), F(1, 27) ⫽ 0.01, ns.
Adolescent and adult mothers. There were 33 adolescent
mothers (M ⫽ 19.84 years, SD ⫽ 1.49) in the original sample. A Relations Between Sociodemographic Characteristics
randomly selected subsample of 32 adult mothers (M ⫽ 28.63 and Parenting Knowledge
years, SD ⫽ 4.65) who lived in the same cultural and geographic
contexts as the adolescent mothers served as the comparison At the zero-order level, mothers’ age, education, and occupa-
group. Adult mothers (M ⫽ .82, SD ⫽ .06) scored higher than tional status and family SES were significantly positively related
adolescent mothers (M ⫽ .70, SD ⫽ .08) on the KIDI, F(1, 63) ⫽ to mothers’ KIDI scores (see Table 1). Older mothers, mothers
47.13, p ⬍ .001, ␩2p ⫽ .43. Mothers’ education was correlated with with more education or higher occupational status, and mothers
KIDI scores in these 65 mothers, r(63) ⫽ .49, p ⬍ .001, but the from higher SES families generally scored higher on the KIDI. In
group difference in KIDI scores held after controlling for maternal addition, mothers’ education and family SES were positively cor-
education (adult mothers, EMM ⫽ .53, SE ⫽ .03; adolescent related with residual KIDI scores, controlling for mothers’ age:
mothers, EMM ⫽ .37, SE ⫽ .02), F(1, 62) ⫽ 18.43, p ⬍ .001, Mothers with more education and mothers from higher SES fam-
␩2p ⫽ .23. ilies scored higher on the KIDI when the variance in KIDI scores
Full-time and part-time employed mothers and homemak- that was associated with mothers’ age was removed. The signifi-
ers. Of the 268 participating mothers, 87 were employed outside cant correlation between mothers’ occupational status and KIDI
the home on a full-time basis (working ⱖ 40 hr/week, M ⫽ 42.67, scores attenuated to nonsignificance when the variance in KIDI
SD ⫽ 6.1), 80 were employed part time (M ⫽ 22.03, SD ⫽ 10.18), scores that was associated with mothers’ age was removed. Fi-
and 101 were full-time homemakers. No group differences in KIDI nally, mothers’ age was positively correlated with residual KIDI
scores emerged when controlling for mother age and education scores when we controlled for mothers’ education: Older mothers
(full-time employed mothers, EMM ⫽ .53, SE ⫽ .01; part-time scored higher on the KIDI when the variance in KIDI scores that
employed mothers, EMM ⫽ .54, SE ⫽ .01; homemakers, EMM ⫽ was associated with mothers’ education was removed. The signif-
.54, SE ⫽ .01), F(2, 263) ⫽ 0.45, ns. icant correlations of KIDI score with mothers’ occupational status
Primiparous, multiparous mothers. A subsample of 45 and family SES attenuated to nonsignificance, however, when the
mothers completed the KIDI twice, when their first child was 20 variance in KIDI score that was associated with mothers’ educa-
months old and again approximately three years later when their tion was removed (see Bornstein et al., 2003).
second child was 20 months old. A repeated-measures ANOVA
showed that mothers scored significantly higher the second time Relations Between Sources of Parenting Supports and
(M ⫽ .84, SD ⫽ .06) than they did the first time (M ⫽ .82, SD ⫽ Parenting Knowledge
.05), F(1, 44) ⫽ 7.67, p ⫽ .008, ␩2p ⫽ .15, and so as a group
mothers improved in parenting knowledge between their first and Descriptive statistics for mothers’ reported sources of parenting
second child. Mothers’ KIDI scores were also highly stable be- support are shown in Table 2 and are ordered by prominence of the
tween the two children, r(43) ⫽ .52, p ⬍ .001, and so mothers reported sources. Altogether, 89.92% (214 of the 238 mothers who
tended to maintain their rank order over time. answered the question) indicated having attended childbirth
Adoptive and birth mothers. A subsample of 28 birth moth- classes, whereas only 28.46% (76 mothers out of the 267 who
ers was selected to compare with the 28 adoptive mothers. Birth answered the question) reported having taken a parenting class.
mothers were the closest individual matches in the whole sample Table 2 also presents correlations between various sources of
on the basis of child gender, maternal age and education, family parenting support and mothers’ KIDI scores. At the zero-order

Table 2
Parenting Sources of Support and Correlations With KIDI Proportion Correct

Correlation with KIDI proportion correct

Parenting support M SD Zero-order Controlling for mothers’ age Controlling for mothers’ education

Child’s father 3.40 1.04 .07 .01 ⫺.00


Written materials 2.85 1.14 .32ⴱⴱⴱ .20ⴱⴱ .20ⴱⴱ
Pediatrician 2.65 1.10 .04 ⫺.01 .05
Mother’s mother 2.36 1.56 ⫺.16ⴱ ⫺.05 ⫺.08
Friends/neighbors 2.06 1.31 .01 ⫺.03 ⫺.03
Other relatives 1.76 1.42 ⫺.13ⴱ ⫺.04 ⫺.03
Mother-in-law 1.45 1.49 ⫺.13ⴱ ⫺.07 ⫺.09
Childbirth class (% yes) 89.92 .23ⴱⴱⴱ .14ⴱ .06
Parenting class (% yes) 28.46 .08 .05 .02

Note. N ⫽ 268; sample sizes for some analyses are slightly smaller due to missing data. KIDI ⫽ Knowledge of Infant Development Inventory.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
1684 BORNSTEIN, COTE, HAYNES, HAHN, AND PARK

level, written materials and having taken childbirth classes were not, controlling for all other variables in the model. Given con-
positively related to mothers’ accuracy on the KIDI: Mothers who cerns expressed that parenting knowledge may be a proxy for
reported that written materials were helpful in their role as parent educational attainment, we tested a mediational model to discern if
during the first half-year of their child’s life and mothers who took formal experience (i.e., written materials) explained the associa-
a childbirth class during pregnancy scored higher on the KIDI tion between education and parenting knowledge. Results of a
when their children were almost 2 years of age. Correlations of Sobel test (Sobel, 1982) indicated that both direct and indirect
written materials and attendance at childbirth classes with residual (mediated by written materials; z ⫽ 3.02, p ⫽ .001) effects of
KIDI scores controlling for mothers’ age remained significant. maternal education on parenting knowledge were significant (see
Only written materials were significantly correlated with residual Figure 1), suggesting that formal experience (i.e., written materi-
KIDI scores controlling for mothers’ education, however. At the als) partially explained the association between education and
zero-order level, a mother’s reliance on her own mother, other parenting knowledge.
relatives, or mother-in-law as sources for support was negatively
related to her KIDI scores. A moment’s speculation on this coun- Discussion
terintuitive result is in order. Perhaps the mother’s own mother,
other relatives, and mother-in-law provide support, but they also Parenting knowledge— understanding the substance and pro-
constitute stresses, contribute criticism, and compete for authority cesses of child rearing and child development—is vital to the
in parenting and so relate negatively to the mother’s KIDI scores. well-being of children, parents, and society. For this reason, advice
Or, perhaps mothers rely on others for parenting knowledge be- to parents dates back in printed form more than 20 centuries to the
cause they themselves lack knowledge, either intentionally (they Code of Hammurabi (Dangel & Polster, 1984; French, 2002).
do not believe that women need to mother independently and Moreover, a majority of U.S. Americans feel that it is more
perceive advice seeking as positive) or unintentionally (they never difficult to be a parent today than in the past and express a pressing
took the time to learn about children’s development and age- need to know more about child rearing and child development
appropriate parenting strategies). Investigating the cause of this (Civitas Initiative et al., 2000). In this study, we compared parent-
finding merits future research attention. No other significant cor- ing knowledge in a large sample of European American mothers
relations emerged between any of the other sources of parenting who varied sociodemographically, and we also analyzed the rela-
support (including pediatricians) and residual KIDI scores when tive contributions of a variety of sociodemographic factors and
we controlled for mothers’ age or education. parenting supports and classes to parenting knowledge. The dis-
cussion of what mothers know, which mothers are in the know,
Predictive Ability of Mothers’ Sociodemographic and how mothers know that ensues should be understood in the
Characteristics and Parenting Supports for context of this referent sample.
Parenting Knowledge
What Mothers Know
The foregoing analyses indicated significant associations of
mothers’ age, education, access to books and other written mate- Knowledge is one key component of parents’ general instru-
rials, and attendance at childbirth classes with mothers’ knowledge mental cognitive system. The items from the KIDI battery were
of child development and child rearing. The overall regression developed for parents of children through 2 years, and the mothers
with all four predictor variables in the model was significant: total we tested all had children approximately that age. Maternal knowl-
adjusted R2 ⫽ .32, F(4, 233) ⫽ 27.07, p ⬍ .001. To evaluate the edge in some developmental domains has been shown to be quite
unique predictive validity of each factor, we conducted four sets of good (see Bornstein & Haynes, 1998). Here we found that on the
hierarchical regression analyses (see Table 3). In each, three of the whole European American mothers demonstrated a fair (81%) but
four variables were entered first as a block and then the fourth not complete understanding of basic parenting knowledge. The
variable was entered alone; the fourth variable could then predict five individual items on which mothers in the current study scored
parenting knowledge after we controlled for the other three vari- the highest (at or near 100%) covered child health and well-being,
ables in the model. Mothers’ age, education, and access to books safety, language abilities, gender differences, and socioemotional
and other written materials all uniquely contributed to mothers’ development, but the five at the bottom (all ⱕ50%) likewise
parenting knowledge, whereas attendance at childbirth classes did included physical growth, socioemotional development, language

Table 3
Prediction of Proportion KIDI Correct From Mothers’ Age, Education, Access to Books and Written Materials, and Attendance at a
Childbirth Class
2
Model Variable added Rincrement Fincrement(1, 233)

Mothers’ education, written materials, childbirth class Mothers’ age .012 4.21ⴱ
Mothers’ age, written materials, childbirth class Mothers’ education .055 18.90ⴱⴱⴱ
Mothers’ age, mothers’ education, childbirth class Written materials .027 9.34ⴱⴱ
Mothers’ age, mothers’ education, written materials Childbirth class .001 0.44

Note. KIDI ⫽ Knowledge of Infant Development Inventory.



p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
MOTHERS’ KNOWLEDGE OF CHILD DEVELOPMENT 1685

age, their knowledge about children at that age may be accurate;


.31***
Written materials .19*** however, once their children move beyond the ages they are asked
to judge, parents fall back on generalized stereotypes. That said,
the items in the KIDI are generic, general, and evidence based and
Mother education Parenting Knowledge
.44*** do not rely on specific memories of specific children; thus, many
problems thought to be associated with parental retrospective
Figure 1. Mediation effect of a source of parenting knowledge (written reporting (Yarrow, Campbell, & Burton, 1968) and inaccurate
materials) on the relation between mother education and parenting knowl- estimation of milestones of their own children’s distant past
edge. Numbers associated with the single-headed arrows are standardized (Tamis-LeMonda, Chen, & Bornstein, 1998) are not at issue. For
coefficients from the Sobel test. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
this reason we looked at group membership and the role of par-
enting resources and supports to understand variation in parenting
knowledge.
abilities, and perception. MacPhee (1981) divided the KIDI con-
ceptually into four domains; looked at from that vantage, mothers
scored highest on parenting items (instrumental beliefs, strategies, Which Mothers Are in the Know
management, responsibilities; 92.5%), then health and safety (nu-
trition, health care, accident prevention, treatment of ailments; This study provides converging evidence that older mothers and
88.5%) and principles (developmental processes, general child more educated mothers possess more parenting knowledge, results
abilities; 80.5%), and lowest on norms and milestones (child that comport with other findings (e.g., Karraker & Evans, 1996).
behavior at a particular point in time; 72.8%). Berlin, Brady-Smith, and Brooks-Gunn (2002) speculated that
What mothers know is important. To understand parenting, one adolescent mothers lack cognitive readiness to parent, and adoles-
must consider not only observations of caregiving but also internal cent parents’ gaps in parenting knowledge have led service pro-
processes thought to underlie caregiving. Thus, a positive cascade viders to recommend programs in parent education for pregnant
is hypothesized to obtain from parenting knowledge to parenting and parenting teens. Although (as we discuss below) attendance at
behaviors to children’s development. Some evidence supports this parenting classes was unrelated to parenting knowledge, atten-
view. Parks and Smeriglio (1986) reported that parenting knowl- dance at childbirth classes was related (at the zero-order level).
edge was positively related to quality of stimulation in the home, Adolescent mothers should attend these, at a minimum. Parenting
which in turn was associated with children’s scores on a develop- information is also typically less available to less educated than to
mental scale. In the prospective longitudinal data set of a low- more educated mothers (see also Viswanath, Kahn, Finnegan,
birth-weight preterm cohort from the multisite Infant Health and Hertog, & Potter, 1993). Although most between-family studies
Development Program, maternal parenting knowledge (of the kind have failed to unearth parity effects (Conrad et al., 1992; Dichtel-
we measured here) was found to affect the quality and structure of miller et al., 1992; MacPhee, 1981), we found, using a more
the home environment mothers provided, which in turn affected sensitive, within-family design, that being a mother for the second
child cognitive and behavioral outcomes (Benasich & Brooks- time appeared to (marginally) improve mothers’ parenting knowl-
Gunn, 1996). (For similar patterns of findings, see McGillicuddy- edge vis-à-vis the first time. Mothers may learn on the job and
DeLisi, 1982a, 1982b; Seefeldt, Denton, Galper, & Younoszai,
remember, or they may prepare better the second time around.
1999.) Reciprocally, mothers lacking parenting knowledge dem-
Firsthand experience or experience of the first 2 years of life with
onstrate poor parenting competence (Conrad et al., 1992; Hess et
a previous child appears to profit parenting knowledge, if only
al., 2004) and may fail to encourage their children’s development
marginally.
and perceive their children as less competent (de Lissovoy, 1973;
By contrast, mothers of girls and boys, homemaker and em-
Jarrett, 1982; Linde & Engelhardt, 1979; Ragozin, Basham, Crinic,
ployed mothers, and mothers by birth and adoption did not sys-
Greenberg, & Robinson, 1982). A dearth of parenting knowledge
tematically differ in their parenting knowledge. Separating social
is also believed to play itself out in unrealistic developmental
expectations. Mothers who harbor unrealistic expectations tend to constructions of knowledge about children’s development from
experience greater stress, possibly as a result of mismatches be- evidence-based knowledge, we conclude that if mothers treat their
tween expectations and behaviors (Crnic & Low, 2002), and their young girls and boys differently (Leaper, 2002) it is apparently not
interactions with their children are often marked by greater discord because their knowledge about parenting or development differs,
and conflict (Teti & Gelfand, 1991). and so gender attitudes or stereotypes seem to be distinct from
Why is mothers’ parenting knowledge sometimes lacking or objective knowledge per se. At least for parenting knowledge as
incorrect? It could be that experiences surrounding child develop- we assessed it, working outside the home does not on average
ment do not automatically lead to parenting knowledge, or, if they augment or undermine a mother’s knowing how to care for a
do, mothers may forget what they know about early child devel- young child, and staying at home with a child does not necessarily
opment by the time their children reach toddlerhood. Thus, with ensure or enhance greater knowledge vis-à-vis being employed
each new phase of development, the onrush of novel requirements outside the home. Homemaker and employed mothers (who aver-
supplants those of past developmental phases (retroactive inhibi- aged half-time) scored equivalently in terms of their knowledge
tion). Alternatively, modifications to parental cognitions may be base. That parenting knowledge is equivalent among adoptive and
temporary, and so parents revert to preconceptions once their birth mothers also suggests that a biological bond with one’s child
experiences with a child have passed (DeGrada & Ponzo, 1971). In does not automatically confer, motivate, or foster greater parenting
other words, when parents are involved with a child of a particular knowledge (Leon, 2002).
1686 BORNSTEIN, COTE, HAYNES, HAHN, AND PARK

How Mothers Know Axia, & Bornstein, 2009). Part of mothers’ parenting knowledge
was explained by mothers’ education acquired through the use of
Resources and supports to parenting can have meaningful con- written materials. However, because the direct effect of maternal
sequences. Parents with high levels of resources and supports tend education on parenting knowledge was significant over and above
to display more sensitive and responsive parenting (Cutrona, 1984) this indirect effect, mothers’ schooling, apart from her reading of
and greater parental efficacy (Izzo, Weiss, Shanahan, & written materials, also significantly informed her parenting knowl-
Rodriguez-Brown, 2000). Nonetheless, we could identify no single edge as measured by the KIDI.
source that all parents trust and want information from that af- Science is dynamic, and scientific knowledge concerning child
fected mothers’ parenting knowledge; mothers varied in their rearing and child development is constantly changing and expand-
sources of parenting knowledge. These findings accord with the ing (Alwin, 1990; Bronfenbrenner, 1958; Elder, 1974; French,
extant literature (Golan et al., 2008). Cochran’s model of parenting 2002). As developmental science, pediatrics, and related fields
and personal social networks attempts to explain how parents use continue to advance, we become increasingly aware, on the one
and evaluate various informational sources in child rearing (Coch- hand, that children possess abilities more advanced and more
ran, 1990; Cochran & Niego, 2002). This model suggests that diverse than formerly believed and, on the other, that parents
parents generally rely on familiar and proximal sources to provide interact with and influence children’s growth and development in
needed parenting information, child care, and other support; that is, sophisticated and subtle ways (Bornstein, 2006; Collins, Maccoby,
parents turn first and most often to family and friends. Then, they Steinberg, Hetherington, & Bornstein, 2000). Consequently, par-
supplement advice from personal social networks with additional enting knowledge differs for the next generation, and media of
formal sources of information, such as professionals or educational books and other written materials that store, convey, and dissem-
programs. Of course, the nature and breadth of parents’ social inate that knowledge will remain key to acquisition of parenting
networks must condition access to information inside and outside knowledge.
this framework. Parents belonging to tightly knit, smaller networks
may, for example, have less exposure to novel, outside information
and be more influenced by the opinions of those close by. As Limitations and Future Directions
geographic mobility and age segregation have increased, the avail-
ability of grandparents, other relatives, and close neighbors— This study addresses some basic questions about mothers’ par-
traditional sources of guidance to young parents— has decreased. enting knowledge. Nonetheless, it has limitations that raise addi-
Another potential explanation for a lack of knowledge of devel- tional questions. Mothers were surveyed about their parenting
opment may be diminishing and curtailed interactions with health knowledge at one point in time: when the mother had a child 20
care providers. months of age. What do mothers know about child rearing and
In this study, mothers’ age and education and mothers’ access to child development before they have their first child? or immedi-
books and other written materials related to greater parenting ately afterward? or by the time their children are adolescents?
knowledge. The significant association between maternal age and Although we studied a number of sociodemographically varied
parenting knowledge might underscore the importance of general groups of mothers, additional research with more diverse samples,
life experience or the fact that mature mothers treat information fathers, and other child caregivers is needed to determine the full
relevant to parenting with more gravitas than do younger mothers. scope of parenting knowledge surrounding young children as well
The teenage mother is very likely to have been a poorer student as whether other individuals in the child’s life rely on similar or
than the older mother (Furstenberg, Brooks-Gunn, & Chase- different sources of child-rearing information. Multiple sources of
Lansdale, 1989; Hofferth & Hayes, 1987; Moore et al., 1993), information are available to parents today: physicians, books,
making it less likely that she will turn to books and other such nurses, phone advice services, Internet websites, community health
materials very readily. Education generally improves parents’ per- centers, news reports, magazines, child-care providers, TV shows,
spectives on their lives, enhances their cognitive and literacy skills, religious groups, classes, and social workers. How will new
and likely engenders feelings of mastery and competence sources of information influence parents? What do adults who are
(Michael, 1972; Reich, 2005). Maternal education bears a close responsible for child health and well-being do in the face of
and pervasive relation to individual variation in parenting (Alwin, conflicting information? Rapid social changes, including distance
1984; Bornstein, Hendricks, et al., 2003; Kelly, Sanchez-Hucles, from kin and friends and scarcity of time, fuel greater reliance on
& Walker, 1993; Wright & Wright, 1976) and, through this rela- the World Wide Web for child-rearing information. As suggested
tion, may exert an impact on child development, health, and by Rothbaum, Martland, and Jannsen (2008), the Web is attractive
well-being (Dollaghan et al., 1999; Green, 1970; Hitchcock & to parents seeking child-rearing information, because it is “always
Oliver, 1976; Hoff et al., 2002) across a wide range of child age available, it is up to date, and it is fast” (p. 119). Future studies
and ethnic group membership (Ramey & Ramey, 1999; Sandqvist, should explore parents’ use of all media as they grow in popularity.
1995; Sonnander & Claesson, 1999; Terrisse, Roberts, Palacio- Furthermore, received support is less strongly associated with a
Quintin, & MacDonald, 1998). Youth and poor education are risk variety of psychological phenomena than is perceived support
factors for parental limited knowledge of child development (Pick- (Wethington & Kessler, 1986). Parents’ perceptions of the value of
ett, Marlenga, & Berg, 2003; Reich, 2005). Higher SES parents, different resources, therefore, may be a better predictor than actual
more than lower SES parents, have access to, seek out, and absorb support; parents who receive a lot of support from a given source
expert advice about child rearing and child development (Lightfoot may still perceive this support to be inadequate to their knowledge
& Valsiner, 1992). Print materials are convenient to more educated needs. We evaluated only parenting knowledge. Finally, research-
mothers, who also rate media influence as important (Bonichini, ers need to attend more to assessing relations among parenting
MOTHERS’ KNOWLEDGE OF CHILD DEVELOPMENT 1687

knowledge, other parenting cognitions, parenting practices, and For their part, physicians regularly draw the information they
child development. need to treat their child patients from multiple sources, parents
(normally mothers) in particular. Child health supervision visits
are central to the work of primary health care providers, who see
Implications for Parenting Education and virtually all U.S. children in the first few years of life. Young
Clinical Practice children average more than two visits per year to office-based
clinicians (Williams, Whitlock, Edgerton, Smith, & Bell, 2007).
Some aspects of parenting are learned and some parenting skills Practitioners routinely rely on parents during interviews, and so
can be taught, and lack of knowledge of basic child care can be they need to be aware that parents’ replies to their questions are
detrimental (Feldman, 1998). Thus, our findings have explicit moderated by parents’ knowledge about child rearing and chil-
implications, first, for parenting education. “Parenting education dren’s development (Cheng, DeWitt, Savageau, & O’Connor,
describes a range of teaching and support programmes which focus 1999; Cheng, Savageau, Bigelow, et al., 1996; Cheng, Savageau,
on the skills, feelings, and tasks of being a parent” (Einzig, 1996, DeWitt, Bigelow, & Charney, 1996). This kind of information may
p. 222); parent education aims to enhance or change parental role facilitate or critically handicap attempts to advise parents and treat
performance (Smith, Perou, & Lesesne, 2002), and parenting ed- child patients (Civitas Initiative et al., 2000). As health care
ucators are concerned, among other topics, with knowledge of professionals cope with deciding, in the diminishing time available
child development and child rearing (e.g., DeBord, Smith, Mulroy, in office visits, which health supervision issues to cover from the
Tanner, & Silliman, 1997). Parent education is therefore posi- growing list of useful topics (Cheng et al., 1999; Galuska et al.,
tioned opportunely to enhance the satisfaction and functioning of 2002; Olson et al., 2004; Reisinger & Bires, 1980; Schuster, Duan,
families and development of children by communicating knowl- Regalado, & Klein, 2000; Stevens et al., 2003; Young, Davis,
edge about child rearing and child development that increases Schoen, & Parker, 1998), reports from parents set priorities and
understanding and by providing alternative models of parenting might improve practice delivery.
that widen parents’ choices, teaching new skills, and facilitating
access to community services. There is much information about
child development and child rearing available today, but it is often
Conclusions
poorly explained, frequently unusable, and commonly delivered in Parents have the job of absorbing and understanding novel,
confidence-undermining ways. Our data show that childbirth, and complex, and rapidly changing, uncertain information. At the same
not parenting, classes relate to mothers’ parenting knowledge (at time they are called on to parent consistently, appropriately, and
least at the zero-order level). Moreover, many more mothers effectively. In other words, what parents need to know about
reported attending childbirth than parenting classes (3:1). The need parenting, children, and childhood is complicated and has real-life
for readily accessible information that will teach parents about consequences every day. The findings about parenting knowledge
their child’s development and help parents cope with the chal- from this study have implications for parenthood and for children’s
lenges of parenting cannot be overstated. What may be required to well-being. Insofar as parents are more knowledgeable about child
improve this record is to promote attendance at childbirth and rearing and child development, they are likely to act and respond
parenting classes and to revisit their curricula to refine and specify more positively, skillfully, and effectively in executing their child-
more exactly the areas of parenting knowledge that need to be rearing roles and responsibilities, feel more competent in their
taught and learned. parenting (Bornstein, 2003; Bornstein, Hendricks, et al., 2003),
A second issue is the role of parenting knowledge vis-à-vis have more positive interactions with their children (Conrad et al.,
clinical practice, a consideration that itself has twin reciprocal 1992; Reis et al., 1986), and behave in developmentally more
aspects: how parents inform and what parents want to know from appropriate ways with them (Grusec & Goodnow, 1994; Putnam,
their children’s health care providers. Parent participation in deci- Sanson, & Rothbart, 2002).
sion making and information sharing, two elements of family- The construct of “cognitive readiness” to parent has been
centered health care, are widely promulgated as desirable and proposed as an influential factor in the transition and adjust-
beneficial. However, “health literacy” generally is remarkably ment to parenting (Miller, Miceli, Whitman, & Borkowski,
poor, and poor health literacy affects parenting and may have 1996; O’Callaghan, Borkowski, Whitman, Maxwell, & Keogh,
far-reaching psychological and medical consequences. Caregivers 1999; Sommer et al., 1993). Put broadly, cognitive readiness
determine whether to bring children for acute, as well as preven- represents parents’ knowledge of child rearing and child devel-
tive, care appointments, and they bear the responsibility of follow- opment from formal and informal sources and parents’ ability to
ing through with treatment plans. One study assessed the associ- synthesize information and apply it. Cognitive readiness to
ation between caregiver health literacy and the use and cost of parent predicts parenting and children’s developmental out-
child health services in children ages 12 months to 12 years comes. Adolescent mothers exhibit lower levels of cognitive
presenting to the pediatric emergency department of an urban, readiness than do adult mothers, and in a sample of adolescent
public hospital (Sanders, Thompson, & Wilkinson, 2007). One in mothers, cognitive readiness to parent measured during preg-
five caregivers had low health literacy, and (in accord with the nancy predicted children’s attachment status at 12 months as
results we report here) caregivers with low health literacy were well as the quality of mother– child interactions when children
more likely to have less than a high school education. Mothers who were 3 years of age (Whitman, Borkowski, Weed, & Keogh,
bring their children to clinics differ in knowledge of child devel- 2001). Thus, inadequate knowledge and expectations about
opment norms from mothers who do not (Rickard, Graziano, & children’s development and behaviors infect problematic par-
Forehand, 1984). enting and have been associated with nonoptimal caregiving
1688 BORNSTEIN, COTE, HAYNES, HAHN, AND PARK

even among already at-risk parents (Azar & Rohrbeck, 1986; Bornstein, M. H. (2003). Positive parenting and positive development in
Chance & Scannapieco, 2002; Miller et al., 1996; Twentyman children. In R. M. Lerner, F. Jacobs, & D. Wertlieb (Eds.), Handbook of
& Plotkin, 1982). applied developmental science: Promoting positive child, adolescent,
The epigraph for our paper opened Benjamin Spock’s Baby and family development through research, policies, and programs:
and Child Care, first published in 1946. For decades, Dr. Vol. 1. Applying developmental science for youth and families: Histor-
ical and theoretical foundations (pp. 187–209). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Spock’s advice on child rearing was gospel for mostly middle-
Sage.
class European American parents. But common sense alone— Bornstein, M. H. (2006). Parenting science and practice. In K. A. Ren-
that mixture of folk wisdom, gut feelings or intuitions, counsel ninger, I. E. Sigel, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child
by relatives and strangers, and vague memories of one’s own psychology: Vol. 4. Child psychology in practice (6th ed., pp. 893–949).
upbringing— can be mistaken, misguided, or outmoded and Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
does not always constitute a sound basis for decisions and Bornstein, M. H., & Bradley, R. H. (Eds.). (2003). Socioeconomic status,
actions that affect children’s lives. Parents need and are entitled parenting, and child development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
to information based on evidence, not merely belief, and pre- Bornstein, M. H., & Cote, L. R. (2004). Mothers’ parenting cognitions in
sented in a manner reflecting an understanding of the signifi- cultures of origin, acculturating cultures, and cultures of destination.
cance of their task and acknowledgment of their capabilities. To Child Development, 75, 221–235.
be fair, when Dr. Spock wrote “Trust yourself,” the science of Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C. S., Suwalsky, J., & Haynes, O. M. (2003).
Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development: The Holling-
child development was in its infancy. Neither pediatricians like
shead Four-Factor Index of Social Status and the Socioeconomic Index
Spock nor parents had the body of concrete, reliable develop-
of Occupations. In M. H. Bornstein & R. H. Bradley (Eds.), Socioeco-
mental science we more fortunately possess today. It behooves nomic status, parenting, and child development (pp. 29 – 82). Mahwah,
thinking parents to take full advantage of this newfound, NJ: Erlbaum.
evidence-based knowledge on how to parent and why children Bornstein, M. H., & Haynes, O. M. (1998). Vocabulary competence in
develop as they do. early childhood: Measurement, latent construct, and predictive validity.
Child Development, 69, 654 – 671.
Bornstein, M. H., Hendricks, C., Hahn, C.-S., Haynes, O. M., Painter,
References K. M., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2003). Contributors to self-perceived
competence, satisfaction, investment, and role balance in maternal par-
Alwin, D. F. (1984). Trends in parental socialization values: Detroit,
enting: A multivariate ecological analysis. Parenting: Science and Prac-
1958 –1983. American Journal of Sociology, 90, 359 –381.
tice, 3, 285–326.
Alwin, D. F. (1990). Historical changes in parental orientations to children.
Bornstein, M. H., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Suwalsky, J. T. D., & Haynes,
Sociological Studies of Child Development, 3, 65– 86.
O. M. (1991). Family Description Questionnaire. Unpublished manu-
Azar, S. T., & Rohrbeck, C. A. (1986). Child abuse and unrealistic
script, National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.
expectations: Further validation of the Parent Opinion Questionnaire.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1958). Socialization and social class through time and
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 867– 868.
Barnard, K. E., & Solchany, J. E. (2002). Mothering. In M. H. Bornstein space. New York, NY: Holt.
(Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3. Status and social conditions of Brooks-Gunn, J., & Furstenberg, F. F. (1986). The children of adolescent
parenting (2nd ed., pp. 3–25). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. mothers: Physical, academic, and psychological outcomes. Developmen-
Bavolek, S. (1989). Assessing and treating high-risk parenting attitudes. tal Review, 6, 224 –251.
Early Child Development and Care, 42, 99 –112. Bugental, D. B., & Happaney, K. (2002). Parental attributions. In M. H.
Belsky, J., Hertzog, C., & Rovine, M. (1986). Casual analyses of multiple Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3. Status and social
determinants of parenting: Empirical and methodological advances. In conditions of parenting (2nd ed., pp. 509 –535). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
M. E. Lamb, A. L. Brown, & B. Rogoff (Eds.), Advances in develop- Caughy, M. O., Miller, T. L., Genevro, J. L., Huang, K., & Nautiyal, C.
mental psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 153–202). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (2003). The effects of Healthy Steps on disciplines strategies of parents
Belsky, J., & Kelly, J. (1994). The transition to parenthood: How a first with toddlers. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 24, 517–
child changes a marriage. New York, NY: Bantam. 534.
Belsky, J., Youngblade, L., & Pensky, E. (1989). Child-rearing history, Chamberlin, R. W., Szumowski, E. K., & Zastowny, T. R. (1979). An
marital quality, and maternal affect: Intergenerational transmission in a evaluation of efforts to educate mothers about childhood development in
low-risk sample. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 291–304. pediatric office practices. American Journal of Public Health, 69, 875–
Benasich, A. A., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1996). Maternal attitudes and knowl- 886.
edge of child-rearing: Associations with family and child outcomes. Chance, T., & Scannapieco, M. (2002). Ecological correlates of child
Child Development, 67, 1186 –1205. maltreatment: Similarities and differences between child fatality and
Berlin, L. J., Brady-Smith, C., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2002). Links between nonfatality cases. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 19, 139 –
childbearing age and observed maternal behaviors with 14-month-olds 161.
in the Early Head Start Research and Evaluation Project. Infant Mental Cheng, T. L., DeWitt, T. G., Savageau, J. A., & O’Connor, K. G. (1999).
Health Journal, 23, 104 –129. Determinants of counseling in primary care pediatric practice. Archives
Bonichini, S., Axia, G., & Bornstein, M. H. (2009). Validation of the of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 153, 629 – 635.
parent health locus of control scales in an Italian sample. Italian Journal Cheng, T. L., Savageau, J. A., Bigelow, C., Charney, E., Kumar, S., &
of Pediatrics, 35, 1–5. DeWitt, T. G. (1996). Assessing mother’s attitudes about the physician’s
Booth, C. L., Mitchell, S. K., Barnard, K. E., & Spieker, S. J. (1989). role in child health promotion. American Journal of Public Health, 86,
Development of maternal social skills in multiproblem families: Effects 1809 –1812.
on the mother– child relationship. Developmental Psychology, 25, 403– Cheng, T. L., Savageau, J. A., DeWitt, T. G., Bigelow, C., & Charney, E.
412. (1996). Expectations, goals, and perceived effectiveness of child health
Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.). (1991). Cultural approaches to parenting. Hills- supervision: A study of mothers in a pediatric practice. Clinical Pedi-
dale, NJ: Erlbaum. atrics, 35, 129 –137.
MOTHERS’ KNOWLEDGE OF CHILD DEVELOPMENT 1689

Civitas Initiative, Zero to Three, & Brio Corporation. (2000). What grown- to the development of extremely low birth weight infants. Journal of
ups understand about child development: A national benchmark survey. Early Intervention, 16, 210 –220.
Washington, DC: Zero to Three, National Center for Infants, Toddlers, Dollaghan, C. A., Campbell, T. F., Paradise, J. L., Feldman, H. M.,
and Families. Janosky, J. E., Pitcairn, D. N., & Kurs-Lasky, M. (1999). Maternal
Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (1978). Popular primers for parents. American education and measures of early speech and language. Journal of
Psychologist, 33, 359 –369. Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 1432–1443.
Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (1998). Historical shifts and underlying themes in Donahue, M. L., Pearl, R., & Herzog, A. (1997). Mothers’ referential
ideas about rearing young children in the United States: Where have we communication with preschoolers: Effects of children’s syntax and
been? Where are we going? Early Development and Parenting, 7, mothers’ beliefs. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 18,
101–117. 133–147.
Cochran, M. (1990). Personal social networks as a focus of support. Einzig, H. (1996). Parenting education and support. In R. Bayne & I.
Prevention in Human Services, 9, 45– 67. Horton (Eds.), New directions in counseling (pp. 220 –234). London,
Cochran, M., & Niego, S. (2002). Parenting and social networks. In M. H. England: Routledge.
Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 4. Applied parenting (2nd Elder, G. H. (1974). Children of the Great Depression: Social change in
ed., pp. 123–148). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. life experience. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences Feldman, M. A. (1998). Parents with intellectual disabilities. In J. R.
(2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Jutzker (Ed.), Handbook of child abuse research and treatment (pp.
Collins, W. A., Maccoby, E. E., Steinberg, L., Hetherington, E. M., & 401– 420). New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Bornstein, M. H. (2000). Contemporary research on parenting: The case Field, T. M., Widmayer, S. M., Stringer, S., & Ignatoff, E. (1980). Teen-
for nature and nurture. American Psychologist, 55, 218 –232. age, lower-class, Black mothers and their preterm infants: An interven-
Conrad, B., Gross, D., Fogg, L., & Ruchala, P. (1992). Maternal confi- tion and developmental follow-up. Child Development, 51, 426 – 436.
dence, knowledge, and quality of mother–toddler interactions: A pre- Fox, J. (1997). Applied regression analysis, linear models and related
liminary study. Infant Mental Health Journal, 13, 353–362. methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Cook, R. D., & Weisberg, S. (1999). Applied regression including com- Frankel, D. G., & Roer-Bornstein, D. (1982). Traditional and modern
puting and graphics. New York, NY: Wiley. contributions to changing infant-rearing ideologies of two ethnic com-
Cooper, P. J., & Murray, L. (1998). Postnatal depression. British Medical munities. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Develop-
Journal, 316, 1884 –1886. ment, 47(4, Serial No. 196), 1–51.
Copeland, D., & Harbaugh, B. L. (2005). Differences in parenting stress French, V. (2002). History of parenting: The ancient Mediterranean world.
between married and single first time mothers at six to eight weeks after In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 2. Biology and
birth. Issues in Comprehensive Pediatric Nursing, 28, 139 –152. ecology of parenting (2nd ed., pp. 345–376). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cowan, C. P., Cowan, P. A., Heming, G., Garrett, E., Coysh, W. S., Furstenberg, F. F., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Chase-Lansdale, L. (1989). Teen-
Curtis-Boles, H., & Boles, A. J. (1985). Transitions to parenthood: His, aged pregnancy and childbearing. American Psychologist, 44, 313–320.
hers, and theirs. Journal of Family Issues, 6, 451– 481. Gabb, J. (2005). Lesbian m/otherhood: Strategies of familial–linguistic
Crnic, K., & Low, C. (2002). Everyday stresses and parenting. In M. H. management in lesbian parent families. Sociology, 39, 585– 603.
Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 5. Practical parenting Galuska, D. A., Fulton, J. E., Powell, K. E., Burgeson, C. R., Pratt, M.,
(2nd ed., pp. 243–267). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Elster, A., & Griesemer, B. A. (2002). Pediatrician counseling about
Crockenberg, S. B. (1981). Infant irritability, mother responsiveness, and preventive health topics: Results from the Physicians’ Practices Survey,
social support influences on the security of infant–mother attachment. 1998 –1999. Pediatrics, 109, e83.
Child Development, 52, 857– 865. Garcı́a Coll, C. T., & Pachter, L. M. (2002). Ethnic and minority parenting.
Crouter, A. C., Helms-Erickson, H., Updegraff, K., & McHale, S. M. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 4. Applied
(1999). Conditions underlying parents’ knowledge about children’s parenting (2nd ed., pp. 1–20). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
daily lives in middle childhood: Between- and within-family compari- Geary, D. C. (2000). Evolution and proximate expression of human pater-
sons. Child Development, 70, 246 –259. nal investment. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 55–77.
Cutrona, C. (1984). Social support and stress in the transition to parent- Geboy, M. J. (1981). Who is listening to the experts? The use of child care
hood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 378 –390. material by parents. Family Relations, 30, 205–210.
Damast, A. M., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. H. (1996). Golan, S., Spiker, D., Peterson, D., Mercier, B., Snow, M., & Williamson,
Mother– child play: Sequential interactions and the relation between C. (2008, June). Parent voices: A statewide look. Washington State
maternal beliefs and behaviors. Child Development, 67, 1752–1766. Department of Early Learning Parent Needs Assessment: Phone survey
Dangel, R. E., & Polster, R. A. (Eds.). (1984). Parent training. New York, (SRI Project 18252). Menlo Park, CA: SRI International.
NY: Guilford Press. Goodnow, J. J. (1988). Parents’ ideas, actions, and feelings: Models for
DeBord, K., Smith, C., Mulroy, M., Tanner, P., & Silliman, B. (1997). The developmental and social psychology. Child Development, 59, 286 –320.
emergence of the parenting profession. Arlington, VA: National Council Goodnow, J. J., & Collins, W. A. (1990). Development according to
for Family Relations. parents: The nature, sources, and consequences of parents’ ideas. Lon-
DeGrada, E., & Ponzo, E. (1971). La normalita del bambino come pregi- don, England: Erlbaum.
udizio dell’adulto [Adult prejudices about infant normality]. Rome, Gordon, R. A., Chase-Lansdale, P. L., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2004). Extended
Italy: Bulzoni. households and the life course of young mothers: Understanding the
de Lissovoy, V. (1973). Child care by adolescent parents. Children Today, associations using a sample of mothers with premature, low birthweight
2, 22–25. babies. Child Development, 75, 1013–1038.
Deutsch, F. M., Ruble, D. N., Fleming, A., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Stangor, C. Gottfried, A. E., Gottfried, A. W., & Bathurst, K. (2002). Maternal and
(1988). Information-seeking and self-definition during the transition to dual-earner employment status and parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.),
motherhood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 420 – Handbook of parenting: Vol. 2. Biology and ecology of parenting (2nd
431. ed., pp. 207–229). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Dichtelmiller, M., Meisels, S. J., Plunkett, J. W., Bozynski, M. A., Claflin, Green, I. W. (1970). Manual for scoring socioeconomic status for research
C., & Mangelsdorf, S. C. (1992). The relationship of parental knowledge on health behaviors. U.S. Public Health Reports, 85, 815– 827.
1690 BORNSTEIN, COTE, HAYNES, HAHN, AND PARK

Gross, D., Conrad, B., Fogg, L., Willis, L., & Garvey, C. (1993). What Kelly, M. L., Sanchez-Hucles, J., & Walker, R. (1993). Correlates of
does the NCATS (Nursing Child Assessment Teaching Scale) measure? disciplinary practices in working- to middle-class African-American
Nursing Research, 42, 260 –265. mothers. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 39, 252–264.
Grusec, J. E., & Goodnow, J. J. (1994). Impact of parental discipline Kreppner, K. (1992). Development in a developing context: Rethinking the
methods on the child’s internalization of values: A reconceptualization family’s role for children’s development. In L. T. Winegar & J. Valsiner
of current points of view. Developmental Psychology, 30, 4 –19. (Eds.), Children’s development within social context: Vol. 2. Research
Guyer, B., Hughart, N., Strobino, D., Jones, A. S., & Scharfstein, D. and methodology (pp. 161–182). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
(2000). Assessing the impact of pediatric-based, developmental services Leaper, C. (2002). Parenting girls and boys. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.),
on infants, families and clinicians: Challenges to evaluating the Healthy Handbook of parenting: Vol. 1. Children and parenting (2nd ed., pp.
Steps Program. Pediatrics, 105, e33. 189 –225). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hammond-Ratzlaff, A., & Fulton, A. (2001). Knowledge gained by moth- Leiderman, P. H., Tulkin, S. R., & Rosenfeld, A. (Eds.). (1977). Culture
ers enrolled in a home visitation program. Adolescence, 36, 435– 442. and infancy: Variations in the human experience. New York, NY:
Hess, C. R., Teti, D. M., & Hussey-Gardner, B. (2004). Self-efficacy and Academic Press.
parenting of high-risk infants: The moderating role of parent knowledge Leon, I. G. (2002). Adoption losses: Naturally occurring or socially con-
of infant development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, structed? Child Development, 73, 652– 663.
24, 423– 437. Lightfoot, C., & Valsiner, J. (1992). Parental belief systems under the
Hess, R. D., Kashiwagi, K., Azuma, H., Price, G. G., & Dickson, W. P. influence: Social guidance of the construction of personal cultures. In
(1980). Maternal expectations for mastery of developmental tasks in I. E. Sigel, A. V. McGillicuddy-DeLisi, & J. J. Goodnow (Eds.), Pa-
Japan and the United States. International Journal of Psychology, 15, rental belief systems: The psychological consequences for children (2nd
259 –271. ed., pp. 393– 414). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hickson, G. B., & Clayton, E. W. (2002). Parents and their children’s Linde, D. B., & Engelhardt, K. F. (1979). What do parents know about
doctors. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 5. infant development? Pediatric Nursing, 5(6), 32–36.
Practical parenting (2nd ed., pp. 439 – 462). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. MacPhee, D. (1981). Manual for the Knowledge of Infant Development
Hitchcock, D. C., & Oliver, L. I. (1976). Intellectual development and Inventory. Unpublished manuscript, University of North Carolina.
school achievements of youths 12–17 years: Demographic and socio- MacPhee, D. (1983, April). The nature of parents’ experience with and
economic factors. Vital and Health Statistics, 158, 1– 65. knowledge about infant development. In I. Sigel (Chair), The cognitive
Hoff, E., Laursen, B., & Tardif, T. (2002). Socioeconomic status and experience of parents: The study and implications of parental knowl-
parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 2. edge, perceptions, reasoning and beliefs. Symposium conducted at the
Biology and ecology of parenting (2nd ed., pp. 231–252). Mahwah, NJ: meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Detroit, MI.
Erlbaum. MacPhee, D. (1984). The pediatrician as source of information about child
Hofferth, S. L., & Hayes, C. D. (Eds.). (1987). Risking the future: Ado- development. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 9, 87–100.
lescent sexuality pregnancy, and childbearing. Washington, DC: Na- Martin, J. A., Hamilton, B. E., Sutton, P. D., Ventura, S. J., Menacker, F.,
tional Academy Press. Kimeyer, S., & Mathews, T. J. (2009). Births: Final data for 2006.
Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). The Four-Factor Index of Social Status. National Vital Statistics Reports, 57, 1–27.
Unpublished manuscript, Yale University. McGillicuddy-DeLisi, A. B. (1982a). Parental beliefs about developmental
Huang, K., Caughy, M. O., Genevro, J. L., & Miller, T. L. (2005). Maternal processes. Human Development, 25, 192–200.
knowledge of child development and quality of parenting among White, McGillicuddy-DeLisi, A. B. (1982b). The relationship between parental
African-American, and Hispanic mothers. Journal of Applied Develop- beliefs and children’s cognitive development. In L. M. Laosa & I. E.
mental Psychology, 26, 149 –170. Sigel (Eds.), Families as learning environments for children (pp. 7–24).
Hulbert, A. (2003). Raising America: Experts, parents, and a century of New York, NY: Plenum.
advice about children. New York, NY: Knopf. McKim, M. K. (1987). Transition to what? New parents’ problems in the
Institute of Medicine. (1994). Defining primary care: An interim report. first year. Family Relations, 36, 22–25.
Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Retrieved from http:// Melamed, B. G. (2002). Parenting the ill child. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.),
books.nap.edu/openbook.php?record_id⫽9153&page⫽15 Handbook of parenting: Vol. 5. Practical parenting (2nd ed., pp. 329 –
Izzo, C., Weiss, L., Shanahan, T., & Rodriguez-Brown, F. (2000). 348). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Parental self-efficacy and social support as predictors of parenting Michael, R. T. (1972). The effect of education on efficiency in consumption.
practices and children’s socioemotional adjustment in Mexican im- New York, NY: Columbia University Press.
migrant families. Journal of Prevention and Intervention in the Michalski, R. L., & Shackelford, T. K. (2001). Methodology, birth order,
Community, 20, 197–213. intelligence, and personality. American Psychologist, 56, 520 –524.
Jarrett, G. E. (1982). Childrearing patterns of young mothers: Expectations, Miller, C. L., Miceli, P. J., Whitman, T. L., & Borkowski, J. G. (1996).
knowledge and practices. American Journal of Maternal Child Nursing, Cognitive readiness to parent and intellectual– emotional development in
7, 119 –124. children of adolescent mothers. Developmental Psychology, 32, 533–541.
Johnston, B. D., Huebner, C. E., Tyll, L. T., Barlow, W. E., & Thompson, Miller, S. A. (1988). Parents’ beliefs about children’s cognitive develop-
R. S. (2004). Expanding developmental and behavioral services for ment. Child Development, 59, 259 –285.
newborns in primary care: Effects on parental well-being, practice, and Miller, S. A., Manhal, M., & Mee, L. L. (1991). Parental beliefs, parental
satisfaction. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 26, 356 –366. accuracy, and children’s cognitive performance: A search for causal
Juffer, F., & Rosenboom, L. G. (1997). Infant–mother attachment of relations. Developmental Psychology, 27, 267–276.
internationally adopted children in the Netherlands. International Jour- Miller, T. W. (Ed.). (1989). Stressful life events. Madison, CT: Interna-
nal of Behavioral Development, 20, 93–107. tional Universities Press.
Kammeyer, K. (1967). Birth order as a research variable. Social Forces, Minkovitz, C. S., Hughart, N., Strobino, D., Scharfstein, D., Grason, H.,
46, 71– 80. Hou, W., & Guyer, B. (2003). A practice-based intervention to enhance
Karraker, K. H., & Evans, S. L. (1996). Adolescent mothers’ knowledge of quality of care in the first 3 years of life: The Healthy Steps for Young
child development and expectations for their own infants. Journal of Children program. Journal of the American Medical Association, 290,
Youth and Adolescence, 2, 651– 666. 3081–3091.
MOTHERS’ KNOWLEDGE OF CHILD DEVELOPMENT 1691

Mirowsky, J. (2002). Parenthood and health: The pivotal and optimal age Reis, J., Barbera-Stein, L., & Bennett, S. (1986). Ecological determinants
at first birth. Social Forces, 81, 315–349. of parenting. Family Relations, 35, 547–554.
Mondschein, E. R., Adolph, K. E., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2000). Reisinger, K. F., & Bires, J. (1980). Anticipatory guidance in pediatric
Gender bias in mothers’ expectations about infant crawling. Journal of practice. Pediatrics, 66, 889 – 892.
Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 304 –316. Richman, A. L., Miller, P. M., & LeVine, R. A. (1992). Cultural and
Moore, K. A., Myers, D. E., Morrison, D. R., Nord, C. W., Brown, B., & educational variations in maternal responsiveness. Developmental Psy-
Edmonston, B. (1993). Age at first childbirth and later poverty. Journal chology, 28, 614 – 621.
of Research on Adolescence, 3, 393– 422. Rickard, K. M., Graziano, W., & Forehand, R. (1984). Parental expecta-
Moore, M. R., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2002). Adolescent parenthood. In M. H. tions and child deviance in clinic-referred and non-clinic children. Jour-
Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3. Status and social nal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 13, 179 –186.
conditions of parenting (2nd ed., pp. 173–214). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Riley, D., Salisbury, M. J., Walker, S. K., & Steinberg, J. (1996). Parenting
National Campaign to Prevent Teen Pregnancy. (2004). How is the 3 in 10 in the first year: Wisconsin statewide impact report. Madison: Univer-
statistic calculated? Retrieved from http://www.thenationalcampaign.org/ sity of Wisconsin, School of Human Ecology.
resources/pdf/FactSheet_3in10_Apr2008.pdf Roggman, L. A., Benson, B., & Boyce, L. (1999). Father with infants:
Ninio, A. (1979). The naı̈ve theory of the infant and other maternal Knowledge and involvement in relation to psychosocial functioning and
attitudes in two subgroups in Israel. Child Development, 50, 976 –980. religion. Infant Mental Health Journal, 20, 257–277.
Nunnally, J. C., & Bernstein, I. H. (1994). Psychometric theory (3rd ed.). Roosa, M. W., & Vaughan, L. (1984). A comparison of teenage and older
New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. mothers with preschool age children. Family Relations, 33, 259 –265.
O’Callaghan, M. F., Borkowski, J. G., Whitman, T. L., Maxwell, S. E., & Rothbaum, F., Martland, N., & Jannsen, J. (2008). Parents’ reliance on the
Keogh, D. (1999). A model of adolescent parenting: The role of cognitive Web to find information about children and families: Socio-economic
readiness to parent. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 9, 203–225. differences in use, skills and satisfaction. Journal of Applied Develop-
Oldershaw, L. (2002). A national survey of parents of young children. mental Psychology, 29, 118 –128.
Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Invest in Kids. Rothbaum, F., & Weisz, J. R. (1994). Parental caregiving and child
Olson, L. M., Inkelas, M., Halfon, N., Schuster, M. A., O’Connor, K. G., externalizing behavior in nonclinical samples: A meta-analysis. Psycho-
& Mistry, R. (2004). Overview of the content of health supervision for logical Bulletin, 116, 55–74.
young children: Reports from parents and pediatricians. Pediatrics, 113, Rowe, M. L. (2008). Child-directed speech: Relation to socioeconomic
1907–1916. status, knowledge of child development, and child vocabulary skill.
Pachter, L. M., & Dworkin, P. H. (1997). Maternal expectations about Journal of Child Language, 35, 185–205.
normal child development in four cultural groups. Archives of Pediatrics Ruchala, P., & James, D. C. (1997). Social support, knowledge of infant
& Adolescent Medicine, 151, 1144 –1150. development, and maternal confidence among adolescent and adult
Palacios, J. (1990). Parents’ ideas about the development and education of mothers. Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic, & Neonatal Nursing, 26,
their children: Answers to some questions. International Journal of 685– 689.
Behavioral Development, 13, 137–155. Sanders, L. M., Thompson, V. T., & Wilkinson, J. D. (2007). Caregiver
Paris, R., & Dubus, N. (2005). Staying connected while nurturing an infant: health literacy and the use of child health services. Pediatrics, 119,
A challenge of new motherhood. Family Relations, 54, 72– 83. e86 – e92.
Parke, R. D. (2002). Fathers and families. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Sandqvist, K. (1995). Verbal boys and mathematical girls: Family back-
Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3. Status and social conditions of parenting ground and educational careers. Scandinavian Journal of Educational
(2nd ed., pp. 27–73). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Research, 39, 5–36.
Parks, P. L., & Smeriglio, V. L. (1983). Parenting knowledge among Sanghavi, D. M. (2005). Taking well-child care into the 21st century: A
adolescent mothers. Journal of Adolescent Health Care, 4, 163–167. novel, effective method for improving parent knowledge using comput-
Parks, P. L., & Smeriglio, V. L. (1986). Relationships among parenting erized tutorials. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 159,
knowledge, quality of stimulation in the home and infant development. 482– 485.
Family Relations, 35, 411– 416. Schilmoeller, G. L., & Baranowski, M. D. (1985). Childrearing of first-
Paulsen, R. J., & Sachs, J. (1998). Rewinding your biological clock: borns by adolescent and older mothers. Adolescence, 2, 805– 822.
Motherhood late in life. New York, NY: Freeman. Schmidley, A. D. (2001). Profile of the foreign-born population in the
Pickett, W., Marlenga, B., & Berg, R. L. (2003). Parental knowledge of United States, 2000. Retrieved from the U.S. Bureau of the Census
child development and the assignment of tractor work to children. website: http://www.census.gov/prod/2002pubs/p23-206.pdf
Pediatrics, 112, 11–16. Schuster, M. A., Duan, N., Regalado, M., & Klein, D. (2000). What
Price, J. S., & Hare, E. H. (1969). Birth order studies: Some sources of bias. information do parents receive? What information do they want? Ar-
British Journal of Psychiatry, 115, 633– 646. chives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 154, 1191–1198.
Putnam, S. P., Sanson, A. V., & Rothbart, M. K. (2002). Child tempera- Seefeldt, C., Denton, K., Galper, A., & Younoszai, T. (1999). The relation
ment and parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: between Head Start parents’ participation in a transition demonstration,
Vol. 1. Children and parenting (2nd ed., pp. 255–277). Mahwah, NJ: education, efficacy, and their children’s academic abilities. Early Child-
Erlbaum. hood Research Quarterly, 14, 99 –109.
Ragozin, A. S., Basham, R. B., Crinic, K. A., Greenberg, M. T., & Sices, L., Feudtner, C., McLaughlin, J., Drotar, D., & Williams, M. (2004).
Robinson, N. M. (1982). Effects of maternal age on parenting role. How do primary care physicians manage children with possible devel-
Developmental Psychology, 18, 627– 634. opmental delays? A national survey with an experimental design. Pedi-
Ramey, C. T., & Ramey, S. L. (1999). Prevention of intellectual disabili- atrics, 113, 274 –282.
ties: Early interventions to improve cognitive development. In S. J. Ceci Sigel, I. E. (1992). The belief– behavior connection: A resolvable di-
& W. M. Williams (Eds.), The nature/nurture debate: Essential readings lemma? In I. E. Sigel, A. V. McGillicuddy-DeLisi, & J. J. Goodnow
(pp. 147–163). Walden, MA: Blackwell. (Eds.), Parental belief systems: The psychological consequences for
Reich, S. (2005). What do mothers know? Maternal knowledge of child children (2nd ed., pp. 433– 456). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
development. Infant Mental Health Journal, 26, 143–156. Singh, S., & Darroch, J. E. (2000). Adolescent pregnancy and childbearing:
1692 BORNSTEIN, COTE, HAYNES, HAHN, AND PARK

Levels and trends in developed countries. Family Planning Perspectives, Teti, D. M., & Gelfand, D. M. (1991). Behavioral competence among
32, 14 –23. mothers of infants in the first year: The mediational role of maternal
Sistler, A. K., & Gottfried, N. W. (1990). Shared child development knowl- self-efficacy. Child Development, 62, 918 –929.
edge between grandmother and mother. Family Relations, 39, 92–96. Thompson, R. J., Jr., Gustafson, K. E., Bonner, M. J., & Ware, R. E.
Smeriglio, V. L., & Parks, P. L. (1983). Measuring mothers’ perceptions (2002). Neurocognitive development of young children with sickle cell
about the influence of infant caregiving practices. Child Psychiatry and disease through three years of age. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 27,
Human Development, 13, 237–244. 235–244.
Smith, C., Perou, R., & Lesesne, C. (2002). Parent education. In M. H. Tichenor, P. J., Donohue, G. A., & Olien, C. N. (1970). Mass media flow and
Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 4. Applied parenting (2nd differential growth in knowledge. Public Opinion Quarterly, 34, 159 –170.
ed., pp. 389 – 410). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Tilton-Weaver, L. C., & Kakihara, F. (2008). United States of America. In
Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic intervals for indirect effects in structural J. J. Arnett (Ed.), International encyclopedia of adolescence (pp. 1061–
equations models. In S. Leinhart (Ed.), Sociological methodology 1982 1076). New York, NY: Routledge.
(pp. 290 –312). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Twentyman, C. T., & Plotkin, R. C. (1982). Unrealistic expectations of
Sommer, K., Whitman, T. L., Borkowski, J. G., Schellenbach, C., Max- parents who maltreat their children: An educational deficit that pertains
well, S., & Keogh, D. (1993). Cognitive readiness and adolescent to child development. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38, 497–503.
parenting. Developmental Psychology, 29, 389 –398. U.S. Census Bureau. (2004). Table 1. Total population by age, race and
Sonnander, K., & Claesson, M. (1999). Predictors of developmental delay Hispanic or Latino origin for the United States: 2000. Retrieved from
at 18 months and later school achievement problems. Developmental http://www.census.gov/population/cen2000/phc-t9/tab01.pdf
Medicine & Child Neurology, 41, 195–202. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Chil-
Sparling, J., & Lowman, B. (1983). Parent information needs as revealed dren and Families. (n.d.). Early Head Start Research and Evaluation
through interests, problems, attitudes, and preferences. In R. Haskins & Project (EHSRE), 1996–Current. Retrieved from http://www.acf.hhs
D. Adams (Eds.), Parent education and public policy (pp. 304 –323). .gov/programs/opre/ehs/ehs_resrch/
Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Viswanath, K., Kahn, E., Finnegan, J. R., Jr., Hertog, J., & Potter, J. D.
Spock, B. (1946). Baby and child care. New York, NY: Pocket Books. (1993). Motivation and the “knowledge gap”: Effects of a campaign to
Stern, D. (1995). The motherhood constellation: A unified view of parent– reduce diet-related cancer risk. Communication Research, 20, 546 –563.
infant psychotherapy. New York, NY: Basic Books. Vukelich, C., & Kliman, D. S. (1985). Mature and teenage mothers’ infants
Stern, M., & Alvarez, A. (1992). Knowledge of child development and growth expectations and use of child development information sources.
caretaking attitudes: A comparison of pregnant, parenting, and nonpreg- Family Relations, 34, 189 –196.
nant adolescents. Family Relations: Interdisciplinary Journal of Applied Wacharasin, C. (2001). Predicting child cognitive development in low-
Family Studies, 41, 297–302. income families. Dissertation Abstracts International, 62(5-B), 2263.
Stevens, G. D., Inkelas, M., Kuo, A. A., Peek-Asa, C., Olson, L. M., & Waddell, K. J., & Ball, F. L. (1980, August). Parental Knowledge Ques-
Halfon, N. (2003). Child injury prevention: Disparities in physician tionnaire: The first two years. Paper presented at the meeting of the
guidance and parent practice. Pediatric Research, 53, 218A. American Psychological Association, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.
Stevens, J. H., Jr. (1984). Child development knowledge and parenting Waldfogel, J. (2006). What children need. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni-
skills. Family Relations, 33, 237–244. versity Press.
Stoiber, K. C. (1992). Parents’ beliefs about their children’s cognitive, Walker, M. (2007). International breastfeeding initiatives and their rele-
social, and motor functioning. Early Education and Development, 3, vance to the current state of breastfeeding in the United States. Journal
244 –257. of Midwifery & Women’s Health, 52, 549 –555.
Stoiber, K. C., & Houghton, T. G. (1993). The relationship of adolescent Weisner, T. S., & Gallimore, R. (1977). My brother’s keeper: Child and
mothers’ expectations, knowledge and beliefs to their young children’s sibling caretaking. Current Anthropology, 18, 169 –190.
coping behavior. Infant Mental Health Journal, 14, 61–79. Wethington, E., & Kessler, R. C. (1986). Perceived support, received
Suwalsky, J. T. D., & Bornstein, M. H. (1991). Parenting support measure. support, and adjustment to stressful life events. Journal of Health and
Unpublished manuscript, National Institute of Child Health and Human Social Behavior, 27, 78 – 89.
Development. Whitman, T. L., Borkowski, J. G., Schellenbach, C. J., & Nath, P. S.
Suwalsky, J. T. D., Hendricks, H., & Bornstein, M. H. (2008a). Families by (1987). Predicting and understanding developmental delay of children of
adoption and birth: I. Mother–infant socio-emotional interactions. Adop- adolescent mothers: A multidimensional approach. American Journal of
tion Quarterly, 11, 101–125. Mental Deficiency, 92, 40 –56.
Suwalsky, J. T. D., Hendricks, H., & Bornstein, M. H. (2008b). Families by Whitman, T. L., Borkowski, J. G., Weed, K., & Keogh, D. (2001).
adoption and birth: II. Mother–infant cognitive interactions. Adoption Interwoven lives: Adolescent mothers and their children. Mahwah, NJ:
Quarterly, 11, 126 –151. Erlbaum.
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics. Williams, L. O., & Decouflé, P. (1999). Is maternal age a risk factor for
Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. mental retardation among children? American Journal of Epidemiology,
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Chen, L. A., & Bornstein, M. H. (1998). Mothers’ 149, 814 – 823.
knowledge about children’s play and language development: Short-term Williams, P. D., Williams, A. R., Lopez, M., & Tayko, N. P. (2000).
stability and interrelations. Developmental Psychology, 34, 115–124. Mothers’ developmental expectations for young children in the Philip-
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Damast, A. M., & Bornstein, M. H. (1994). What pines. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 37, 291–301.
do mothers know about the developmental nature of play? Infant Be- Williams, S. B., Whitlock, E. P., Edgerton, E. A., Smith, P. R., & Bell,
havior & Development, 17, 341–345. T. L. (2007). Counseling about proper use of motor vehicle occupant
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Shannon, J., & Spellman, M. (2002). Low-income restraints and avoidance of alcohol use while driving: A systematic
adolescent mothers’ knowledge about domains of child development. evidence review for the U.S. Preventive Services Task Force. Annals of
Infant Mental Health Journal, 23, 88 –103. Internal Medicine, 147, 194 –206.
Terrisse, B., Roberts, D. S. L., Palacio-Quintin, E., & MacDonald, B. E. Wolf, A. (1998). Exposing the great equalizer: Demythologizing Internet
(1998). Effects of parenting practices and socioeconomic status on child equity. In B. Ebo (Ed.), Cyberghetto or cybertopia? Race, class, and
development. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 57, 114 –123. gender on the Internet (pp. 15–32). Westport, CT: Praeger.
MOTHERS’ KNOWLEDGE OF CHILD DEVELOPMENT 1693

Wright, J. D., & Wright, S. R. (1976). Social class and parental values for Young, K. T., Davis, K., Schoen, C., & Parker, S. (1998). Listening to
children: A partial replication and extension of the Kohn thesis. Amer- parents: A national survey of parents with young children. Archives of
ican Sociological Review, 41, 527–537. Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 152, 255–262.
Yarrow, M. R., Campbell, J. D., & Burton, R. V. (1968). Child rearing: An
inquiry into research and methods. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Young, K. T. (1991). What parents and experts think about infants. In F. S. Received November 2, 2009
Kessel, M. H. Bornstein, & A. J. Sameroff (Eds.), Contemporary con- Revision received June 14, 2010
structions of the child (pp. 79 –90). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Accepted June 21, 2010 䡲

Вам также может понравиться