Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 31

American Sociological Review

XX(X) 1–31
Courtesy Stigma and Ó American Sociological
Association 2011
Monetary Sanctions: Toward DOI: 10.1177/0003122411400054
http://asr.sagepub.com

a Socio-Cultural Theory of
Punishment

Alexes Harris,a Heather Evans,a and


Katherine Becketta

Abstract
Recent research suggests that the use of monetary sanctions as a supplementary penalty in
state and federal criminal courts is expanding, and that their imposition creates substantial
and deleterious legal debt. Little is known, however, about the factors that influence the dis-
cretionary imposition of these penalties. This study offers a comprehensive account of the
role socio-cultural factors, especially race and ethnicity, have in this institutional sanctioning
process. We rely on a multilevel statistical analysis of the imposition of monetary sanctions
in Washington State courts to test our theory. The theoretical framework emphasizes the need
to treat race and ethnicity as complex cultural categories, the meaning and institutional
effects of which may vary across time and space. Findings indicate that racialized crime
scripts, such as the association of Latinos and drugs, affect defendants whose wrong-doing
is stereotype congruent. Moreover, all individuals accused of committing racially and ethni-
cally stigmatized offenses in racialized contexts may experience the courtesy stigma that
flows from racialization. We find that race and ethnicity are not just individual attributes
but cultural categories that shape the distribution of stigma and the institutional consequen-
ces that flow from it.

Keywords
stigma, monetary sanctions, punishment

Across the United States, financial penalties factors, especially race and ethnicity, in this
are now a common, although largely discre- institutional sanctioning process.
tionary, supplement to confinement sentences Because monetary sanctions in state and
in misdemeanor and felony cases (Harris, federal courts are a discretionary supplement
Evans, and Beckett 2010). Despite their ubiq- to traditional criminal justice sanctions,
uity and discretionary nature, little is known
about the factors that influence the imposition a
University of Washington
of these substantial and consequential penal-
ties. Drawing on a multilevel analysis of the Corresponding Author:
Alexes Harris, Department of Sociology, Box
assessment of monetary sanctions in Wash- 353340, University of Washington, Seattle, WA
ington State courts, we develop a comprehen- 98195-3530
sive account of the role of socio-cultural E-mail: yharris@uw.edu
2 American Sociological Review XX(X)

analysis of their imposition provides theoret- In addition to yielding theoretical insight


ical insight regarding institutional sanction- regarding the role of race and ethnicity in
ing processes. Our theoretical framework sanctioning decisions, monetary sanctions
suggests that particular behaviors and racial are also of substantive importance. Many
and ethnic characteristics are associated studies show that the massive U.S. penal
with enhanced punishment because they trig- apparatus has become an important compo-
ger negative emotions (e.g., fear, anger, or nent of the stratification system. Researchers
resentment) that intensify punitive responses. have identified several mechanisms by which
Yet, as recent scholarship on race and ethnic- the growing penal system fuels disadvantage
ity suggests, these effects are likely contex- and inequality: the impact of criminal con-
tual and interactive. Indeed, because particu- viction and incarceration on employment
lar crimes and contexts (i.e., places) are and earnings (Pager 2003, 2005, 2007;
themselves racialized, the degree to which Western 2006; Western and Beckett 1999;
a defendant’s behavior is stereotype congru- Western and Pettit 2005); the effects of con-
ent may matter more than ethnic or racial finement on inmates’ mental and physical
background alone. Similarly, the courtesy health (Haney 2006; Massoglia and
stigma that flows from association with a stig- Schnittker 2009); and the widespread imposi-
matized minority group or behavior may tion of collateral or invisible sanctions that
extend to white defendants accused of transform punishment from a temporally lim-
racially stigmatized crimes. Moreover, race ited experience to a long-term status (Manza
and ethnicity may matter differently in and Uggen 2006; Uggen, Manza, and
diverse locales. In short, we argue that race Thompson 2006). This literature also sug-
and ethnicity are best understood not just as gests that the adverse effects of criminal con-
individual attributes, but rather as complex viction are not limited to the criminally sanc-
cultural categories, the meaning and institu- tioned but extend to the families and
tional effects of which vary across time and communities from which they are drawn
space (see also Omi and Winant 1986). (Braman 2004; Clear 2007; Clear, Rose,
In this article, we employ hierarchical lin- and Ryder 2001; Comfort 2007; Foster and
ear modeling techniques to analyze the fac- Hagan 2007; Johnson and Raphael 2006;
tors that shape the imposition of monetary Massoglia 2008; Massoglia and Schnittker
penalties and to test hypotheses derived 2009; McLanahan 2009; Sykes and Piquero
from this theoretical framework. Results con- 2009; Travis 2005; Western 2006; Western
firm that race and ethnicity matter in varied, and McLanahan 2000). These studies have
complex, and contextually specific ways. made an enormous contribution to our under-
The interaction of defendants’ racial and eth- standing of the role of the penal system in the
nic identity, the type of offense of which they stratification process, but they ignore an
are convicted, and the demographic context additional mechanism through which the
in which they are sentenced significantly criminal justice system fuels poverty and
influence the allocation of penalties. Presum- inequality, namely, the widespread imposi-
ably, this is because the activation of racial- tion of monetary sanctions.
ized crime scripts via stereotype congruence
or courtesy stigma triggers negative emotions
that are associated with punitiveness. We MONETARY SANCTIONS IN
also find evidence that ethnicity matters THE U.S. CRIMINAL JUSTICE
more directly than race in Washington State,
where the black population is very small and
SYSTEM
stable in size, but the Latino and immigrant Monetary sanctions, sometimes called Legal
populations are growing rapidly. Financial Obligations (LFOs), include fees,
Harris et al. 3

fines, restitution orders, and other financial (some of which are for specific offenses,
obligations that courts and other criminal jus- such as drug offense or domestic violence)
tice agencies may impose on persons accused and fees (e.g., for the cost of a bench warrant,
of crimes. Use of monetary sanctions in the a court appointed attorney, or a jury trial)
criminal process is not new. In many Euro- may be imposed at the discretion of the sen-
pean countries, restitution was the primary tencing judge (Beckett, Harris, and Evans
criminal penalty for centuries (Mullaney 2008). Similarly, in New York State, judges
1988). In the United States and its colonial may choose to impose 19 statutorily autho-
territories, monetary sanctions became com- rized fees (Rosenthal and Weissman 2007).
mon with the arrival of European settlers The widespread adoption of monetary sanc-
(Merry 2000; Miethe and Lu 2005). In the tions in U.S. state and federal courts repre-
Northern states, monetary penalties were sents a significant enhancement of judicial
used mainly in minor criminal cases; serious discretionary power.
crimes warranted physical punishments such Administration of these sanctions in the
as flogging (Miethe and Lu 2005). In some United States bears little resemblance to the
cases, fines were coupled with corporal pen- use of fines in many European countries,
alties; individuals who could not afford to for two reasons. First, in many European
pay their fines were subjected to additional countries, fines serve as an alternative rather
physical penalties or penal servitude (Miethe than a supplement to incarceration (Bureau
and Lu 2005). Monetary sanctions were also of Justice Assistance 1996; Hillsman and
common in the southern states, where their Greene 1992; O’Malley 2009; Tonry 1998;
imposition was the foundation of the convict Tonry and Lynch 1996). Second, fines are
lease system that lasted from emancipation based on two ideas in Europe: fines should
through the 1940s (Adamson 1983; Black- correspond to the seriousness of the offense,
mon 2008; Oshinsky 1996; Perkinson 2008). and fines should have a similar impact on
In recent decades, policymakers at the people with different incomes (Bureau of
federal, state, county, and city levels have Justice Assistance 1996; Hillsman and
authorized judges to impose a growing num- Greene 1992). To achieve these ends, fines
ber of monetary sanctions on people who are are determined by only two factors: offense
convicted—and sometimes just accused1—of seriousness and offenders’ (actual) daily
crimes (Anderson 2009; Levingston 2008; income (hence the term ‘‘day-fine’’). The
McLean and Thompson 2007; Rosenthal imposition of day-fines by European judges
and Weissman 2007). Some of these newly is highly constrained and predicated on the
authorized monetary sanctions are manda- collection of information regarding
tory, but many are not, and most legislatures offenders’ actual (rather than hypothetical)
did not create guidelines to structure their earnings. By contrast, monetary sanctions
imposition.2 In Washington State, for exam- imposed in the United States are highly dis-
ple, only two fees are mandatory: a $500 cretionary and may be shaped by factors
Victim Penalty Assessment and a $100 fee other than offense seriousness; fines are
to cover the cost of including a first-time also supplements to other criminal justice
defendant’s DNA in a statewide banking sys- sanctions. Moreover, we are unaware of any
tem.3 Although some fees and fines may be statutory requirement that fee and fine
imposed only on certain groups of offenders, assessment be based on offenders’ earnings.
judges retain discretion in deciding whether Recent research suggests that U.S. state
to impose them. For example, some fines and federal courts are increasingly imposing
may be assessed only to defendants who go supplementary monetary sanctions. Indeed,
to trial, but judges need not assess the trial two-thirds (66 percent) of prison inmates sur-
fee in all such cases. A host of other fines veyed in 2004 had been assessed monetary
4 American Sociological Review XX(X)

sanctions by the courts, a dramatic increase of expected monthly earnings for formerly
from 25 percent in 1991 (U.S. Department incarcerated white, Hispanic, and black
of Justice n.d.). Misdemeanants and felons men, respectively) toward a typical (median)
not sentenced to prison were even more legal debt will still possess legal debt 10
likely than felons sentenced to prison to years later due to the accumulation of interest
receive monetary sanctions. Specifically, (Harris et al. 2010). Felons who consistently
courts ordered 84.2 percent of felons sen- pay $50 a month will still possess legal debt
tenced to probation to pay fees or fines in after 30 years of regular monthly payments
1995; 39.7 percent were also required to (ibid). Interviews with 50 debtors from four
pay restitution to victims. Similarly, 85 per- Washington State counties indicate that legal
cent of misdemeanants sentenced to proba- debt has a variety of adverse consequences,
tion were assessed fees, fines, or court costs; including lost income, diminished occupa-
17.6 percent were also assessed restitution tional opportunities, depressed credit ratings,
(Harris et al. 2010). As a result of the and heightened housing instability (ibid).
increased use and proliferation of monetary Rates of payment are low, but non-payment
sanctions, millions of poor residents of the also has negative consequences for debtors,
United States now possess legal debt. including lost income (through garnishment),
This legal debt is often substantial. Anal- worsened credit ratings, prolonged court
ysis of Washington State Superior Court supervision, and issuance of an arrest war-
data, which include all cases sentenced in rant, which can trigger loss of social security
the first two months of 2004, indicates that benefits and even incarceration (ibid).
the median dollar value of the LFO assessed In short, legal debt imposes a variety of
per felony conviction was $1,110; the mean costs; it is also an especially injurious type
LFO assessment was $1,398. These figures of debt. Unlike consumer debt, legal debt is
are clearly an underestimate: they capture not offset by the acquisition of goods or
the fiscal cost of only a single conviction, property, it is not subject to relief through
omit other sources of legal debt, and do not bankruptcy proceedings, and it may trigger
show how legal debt accumulates over the an arrest warrant, an arrest, or incarceration.
life course of persons with criminal histories. Monetary sanctions are thus another mecha-
Toward these ends, we calculated total LFO nism by which criminal justice expansion
amounts imposed on 500 randomly selected fuels inequality.
defendants included in our sample by juve-
nile, district, and superior courts over the
life course. On average, these 500 individuals PUNISHMENT AS A SOCIAL
were assessed $11,471 by these courts by
2008 (Harris et al. 2010). Even this much
AND MORAL ACT
higher figure very likely underestimates the Socio-cultural perspectives on punishment
magnitude of legal debt because it does not suggest that official sanctions for criminal
include fees assessed by other criminal jus- wrong-doing are never solely about crime
tice agencies such as departments of control but are inherently expressive and
corrections. symbolic. Indeed, theorists ranging from
Because legal debt is typically substantial Durkheim (1984) to Mead (1918) to
relative to expected earnings and is often Garfinkel (1956) to Goffman (1956) empha-
subject to interest, it tends to grow over size the emotional and morally expressive
time, even if legal debtors make regular pay- dimension of penal rituals and the role of
ments toward their financial obligations. emotions in the judgment, condemnation,
Washington State felons who make payments and punishment of criminal offenders. Dur-
of $100 a month (i.e., 11, 12, and 15 percent kheim (1984:52), for instance, emphasized
Harris et al. 5

that punishment is far more than an adminis- its rituals and execution. This insight may
trative response to a particular instance help to explain why recent legislative
of law-breaking: ‘‘punishment constitutes attempts to constrain judicial discretion in
essentially a reaction of passionate feeling, confinement sentencing have been only par-
graduated in intensity.’’ Durkheim called tially successful and why they have been
the normative order that gives direction to accompanied by the expansion of judges’
these passionate feelings the conscience col- discretionary authority to impose supplemen-
lective. Although many critics persuasively tary monetary sanctions.
argue that Durkheim overstates the extent
to which the conscience collective and penal
norms and practices reflect widely shared The Socio-Cultural Theory of
and spontaneously held beliefs (see Garland Punishment and the Limits of
1990; Lukes and Scull 1983), his insights Sentencing Reform
regarding the expressive and symbolic
dimensions of punishment constitute the Since the late 1970s, at least 21 states
foundation upon which many other theorists (including Washington State, from which
build. For example, Mead (1918:583) also our data are drawn), the District of Columbia,
highlights the emotional nature of criminal and the federal system have enacted sentenc-
punishment, arguing that the condemnation ing guidelines in an attempt to curb judicial
and punishment of criminal offenders pro- discretion and reduce racial and other dispar-
vides an opportunity to express sublimated ities in confinement sentencing outcomes
and destructive hostilities. For Mead, it is (Frase 2005; Tonry 1996, 1998).4 Research
the release of this ‘‘righteous indignation’’ indicates, however, that the guidelines have
that reinforces in-group identity and gener- failed to create what Weber (1968:657) calls
ates punishment’s characteristic heat and a ‘‘gapless’’ system of rules. Substantive
intensity (see also Garland 1990). rationality persists even where guidelines
Analysts of courtroom rituals also empha- have been adopted, for several reasons. First,
size punishment’s symbolic content and determinant sentencing guidelines still allow
emotional nature. Hay’s (1975) classic for the exercise of judicial discretion in cer-
account of eighteenth-century English court tain cases. Second, guidelines require judges
proceedings shows how these rituals fore- to impose confinement sentences that fall
ground the heinousness of the criminal act within a certain range, but variation within
while simultaneously celebrating the power this range may be substantial. In Washington
of religion, the importance of local hierar- State, for example, judges may impose any-
chies, and the majesty of the law. Similarly, where from 0 to 12 months of jail time for
Garfinkel (1956:420–24) interprets court- some nonviolent drug offenses. Third, the
room proceedings as a ‘‘degradation cere- adoption of sentencing guidelines shifts dis-
mony’’ in which ‘‘moral indignation serves cretionary power from judges to prosecutors
to effect the ritual destruction of the person (Engen and Steen 2000; Harris 2007; Miethe
denounced.’’ Other ethnographic studies pro- 1987; Tonry 1996). Finally, the guidelines
vide empirical support for this perspective, themselves have been the subject of intense
showing that court actors allocate blame political negotiation, in part because determi-
based on offenders’ perceived moral charac- nations of offense seriousness and the rele-
ter (Bridges and Steen 1998; Emerson 1969; vance of offenders’ criminal histories are
Frohmann 1991, 1997; Harris 2008). inherently social and political judgments
From the socio-cultural perspective, pun- (Savelsberg 1992).
ishment is driven by moral outrage; passion The socio-cultural perspective also helps
and indignation provide the motivation for to explain why efforts to eliminate
6 American Sociological Review XX(X)

substantive rationality from the courts have Kramer 2008; Steffensmeier and Demuth
been only partially successful, and why 2001).5 In some cases, race is more salient
they have been accompanied by the expan- than ethnicity; in other cases, the opposite
sion of judicial discretion in the imposition is true.
of monetary sanctions. As Garland (1990) In short, adoption of sentencing guide-
argues, the trend toward formalization and lines moderated but did not eliminate the
rationalization is nowhere complete, least effect of extra-legal factors on custodial
of all in the courtroom, where the recent sentencing outcomes. The courts’ largely
embrace of formal rationality coexists discretionary imposition of monetary sanc-
uneasily with the expressive and moral tions provides an additional and largely
dimension of criminal punishment. Indeed, unfettered opportunity for the exercise of
the decline of public displays of penal discretionary power. The following sections
power means that the expression of moral identify the factors we expect will shape its
outrage and emotion central to penal rituals expression.
is increasingly confined to the courtroom
(Garland 1990). From the socio-cultural
perspective, efforts to formalize criminal Violence, Drugs, and the Socio-
sentencing can be only partially successful Cultural Perspective on
because discretion persists in the cracks Punishment
and crevices of an otherwise formally
rational system and punishment has an Our socio-cultural theory of punishment
inherently expressive and moral dimension, highlights the role of emotions—especially
the expression of which demands discretion- anger, fear, and disgust—in the judgment,
ary power. condemnation, and punishment of offenders.
Indeed, although the enactment of sen- Evidence shows that particular types of crim-
tencing guidelines did reduce the impact of inal offenses trigger especially strong ver-
non-legal variables on sentencing outcomes sions of the negative emotions associated
(Moore and Miethe 1986; Nagel and with punitiveness. Not surprisingly, violent
Schulhofer 1992), many studies find that crime is most strongly linked to fear (Liska,
defendant’s race or ethnicity continue to Lawrence, and Sanchirico 1982; Warr
influence confinement sentencing outcomes, 2000). This finding suggests that violent
even where guidelines have been adopted offenders may be more severely punished
(Albonetti 1997; Bontrager, Bales, and than nonviolent offenders, even after control-
Chiricos 2005; Engen et al. 2003; Johnson ling for offense seriousness. Evidence also
2006; Kramer and Steffensmeier 1993; shows that drug offenders trigger high levels
Spohn and Holleran 2000; Steffensmeier of disgust. For example, experimental
and Demuth 2000, 2001; Ulmer and Kramer research indicates that drug addicts are seen
1996; Wooldredge and Thistlewaite 2004). as incompetent and unsympathetic, a pattern
For example, judges are more likely to associated with this emotional response
depart from sentencing guidelines and (Harris and Fiske 2006). As Miller (1997)
impose harsher penalties when a defendant notes, disgust figures prominently in moral
is African American or Latino (and when discourse; indeed, moral judgment is often
a defendant opts for a trial); judges are conveyed through the idiom of disgust.
also less likely to select the lower incarcer- Because fear and disgust are centrally related
ation length identified in the guidelines to punitiveness, our first hypothesis predicts
when a defendant is black or Latino (Engen that violent and drug offenders will be sanc-
and Gainey 2000; Engen, Gainey et al. tioned more severely than other (mainly
2003; Johnson 2005; Johnson, Ulmer, and property) offenders.
Harris et al. 7

Race, Ethnicity, and the Socio- indicates that the perceived risk of victimiza-
Cultural Perspective on tion is the proximate cause of fear (Warr
2000). Similarly, the perception that blacks
Punishment
and Latinos are more violent than whites
Research suggests that race and ethnicity are appears to explain why a neighborhood’s
strongly linked to the emotions that underlie racial composition influences residents’ per-
the impulse to punish. Studies find that racial ceptions of dangerousness and levels of fear
antipathy and resentment persist among (Chiricos, McEntine, and Gertz 2001;
whites despite widespread acceptance of Quillian and Pager 2001). Cognition, percep-
abstract principles of racial equality. In this tion, and emotion are thus highly interrelated.
literature, symbolic racism is thought to In the context of sanctioning, we argue that
‘‘stem from a blend of anti-Black affect and decision-making is primarily affected by
traditional values. . . . Anti-Black affect is emotional reactions to defendants with par-
a spontaneous and often unacknowledged ticular characteristics. It seems likely that
negative emotion that reflects fear, anger, blacks and Latinos accused of committing
distaste, or simple dislike’’ (Green, Staerkle, criminal offenses will be especially likely
and Sears 2006:438; see also Sears 1988). to trigger the negative emotions that underlie
This antipathy stems in part from the percep- the impulse to punish. Our second hypothesis
tion that blacks violate traditional American therefore predicts that defendants’ race and
values such as self-reliance, the work ethic, ethnicity will shape the exercise of judicial
and respect for authority (Feldman and discretion in the imposition of monetary
Huddy 2005; Green et al. 2006). Similarly, sanctions, with black and Latino defendants
hostility to Latino immigrants stems in large receiving more severe penalties than simi-
part from the belief that they are compara- larly situated white defendants.
tively crime-prone and a drain on non- This hypothesis suggests that blacks and
immigrant taxpayers (Chavez 2008). Latinos will receive similar treatment in the
By contrast, some researchers highlight courts. However, some studies find that
racism’s cognitive dimensions and uncon- race is more salient than ethnicity, while
scious impact on perceptual processes (see others, especially more recent studies, find
Correll, Benard, and Paik 2007; Eberhardt the opposite (Demuth 2003; Mann and Zatz
and Goff 2005; Eberhardt et al. 2004; Pager 1998; Schlesinger 2005; Steffensmeier and
and Karafin 2009; Schram et al. 2009; Soss, Demuth 2000, 2001). A prior study of Wash-
Fording, and Schram 2008). From this per- ington State court outcomes suggests that
spective (sometimes referred to as the Racial Latinos, but not blacks, receive significantly
Classification Model or RCM), race may longer confinement sentences than do whites
affect decision-making even in the absence (Engen and Gainey 2000). Given this find-
of racial animus or antipathy (Schram et al. ing, as well as statewide controversy sur-
2009; Soss et al. 2008). Indeed, some schol- rounding the rapidly increasing immigration
ars refer to the unconscious impact of race on from Latin America and the comparatively
perception as implicit bias, to differentiate it small and stable size of the black population
from conscious racial animus (Quillian 2008; in Washington State, we suspect that the
Sampson and Raudenbush 2004). presence of Latinos in a jurisdiction and
We agree that cognition plays a key role a courtroom may have an especially pro-
in decision-making processes, and that bias nounced effect on judicial decision-making.6
may be unconscious, but it is also clear that Racial threat literature helps to explain
perception often triggers particular emotions how the demographic context in which
that are central to punishment. Research on decisions about punishment are made might
fear of crime, for example, consistently also affect penal outcomes. This literature
8 American Sociological Review XX(X)

highlights the connection between emotions than this. Popular and political discourse
and the racial/ethnic composition of the pop- around violence, drugs, and race are not sep-
ulation in which sentencing occurs. Studies arate but mutually constitutive; contempo-
explore the effect of minority population rary debates about race take place in the con-
size on criminal justice outcomes; disparities text of, and partially through, political
are believed to reflect (white) fear or anxiety discussions of crime, drugs, and punishment
triggered by an increasing or comparatively (Beckett 1997; Bobo and Johnson 2004;
large minority population (see Blalock Edsall and Edsall 1991). As noted previ-
1967; Blumer 1958; King 2007; Liska et al. ously, studies indicate that crime is racially
1982; Spohn and Holleran 2002; Steffensme- and ethnically coded and that the presence
ier and Demuth 2001). Many studies find that of blacks increases fear and perceptions of
a population’s racial composition is a signifi- danger. The relationship between race and
cant predictor of enhanced penalty (see, e.g., crime is bi-directional: blacks and Latinos
Beckett and Western 2001; Bridges and are associated with crime, and crime is asso-
Crutchfield 1988; Bridges, Crutchfield, ciated with blackness and brownness
and Simpson 1987; Mosher 2001; Spohn (Chiricos and Eschholz 2002; Devine and
and Holleran 2002; Steffensmeier and Elliot 1995; Eberhardt et al. 2004; Sampson
Demuth 2001; for negative findings see Britt and Raudenbush 2004). The association
2000; Myers and Talarico 1986). In short, between blacks and violence is particularly
a population’s racial and ethnic composition long-standing.
often influences criminal justice outcomes, Discussions and images of drug problems
presumably because the presence of blacks in the United States are also intimately con-
and Latinos triggers fear in many residents. nected to discussions of race and ethnicity
For this reason, our third hypothesis predicts (Beckett 1997; Reinarman and Levine
that judges in jurisdictions with compara- 1997). Many studies of media representations
tively large black or Latino populations will of crack cocaine in the 1980s and 1990s, for
impose harsher penalties than judges in juris- instance, suggest that these images were
dictions with comparatively small black or highly racialized (Beckett and Sasson 1998;
Latino populations. Our fourth hypothesis Reeves and Campbell 1994; Reinarman and
predicts that these effects will be larger for Levine 1997). In recent years, media atten-
black and Latino defendants sentenced in tion has shifted to other drugs, especially
jurisdictions with comparatively large black methamphetamine, and the popular represen-
and Latino populations than for non-black tation of methamphetamine as a white drug
and non-Latino defendants in jurisdictions has given way to an emphasis on the role
with the same population characteristics. of Latinos, particularly Mexican nationals
and immigrants, in its distribution. In Wash-
ington State, officials emphasize that nar-
Racialization of Crime and cotics are often manufactured or cultivated
Criminalization of Race in Mexico and then imported via Mexican
drug organizations (U.S. DEA 2009; see
We have argued that punishment is an inher- also Eitle and Taylor 2008). In this context,
ently moral and expressive act, and that Latino men are commonly associated with
blackness, brownness, violence, and drugs the illegal drug trade in the popular press
are emotionally and morally laden topics. (Chavez 2008; Delgado and Stefancic 1998;
We therefore expect defendants who are Hurwitz and Pefley 1997).
associated with these qualities to be sanc- In short, racialized crime scripts are to
tioned more severely than defendants who some extent offense-specific. Recent studies
are not. Yet matters are more complicated indicate that defendants who conform to
Harris et al. 9

Race and
Ethnicity of
Individual

Imposition of
Racial and Emotional Reaction
Enhanced
Ethnic and Assessment of
Institutional
Context Moral Character
Sanction

Behavior

Figure 1. Theoretical Diagram of the Socio-Cultural Theory of Punishment: Factors that


Influence Institutional Sanctioning

such scripts receive more severe sanctions. experience courtesy stigma (Goffman 1963)
Experimental studies, for example, suggest that results from racialization. Given the con-
that black and white defendants receive temporary association between blackness and
harsher penalties when they are convicted violence, and brownness and drugs, our sixth
of stereotype-congruent offenses (Jones and hypothesis predicts that defendants of all
Kaplan 2003). Similarly, Schlesinger (2005) races and ethnicities convicted of violent
finds that black defendants charged with vio- crimes in counties with relatively large black
lent offenses, and Latinos charged with drug populations will receive more severe penal-
offenses, are more likely to receive pre-trial ties than will violent offenders sentenced in
incarceration than either white defendants counties with small black populations. Simi-
or black or Latino defendants charged with larly, defendants of all races and ethnicities
less stereotypically-linked crimes (see also convicted of drug offenses in counties with
Steen, Engen, and Gainey 2005). These find- relatively large Latino populations will
ings, which suggest that stereotype congru- receive more severe penalties than will drug
ence activates certain crime scripts and asso- offenders sentenced in counties with small
ciated emotional responses, are the basis of Latino populations. Figure 1 depicts the the-
our fifth hypothesis: black defendants oretical model from which we derive these
charged with violent crimes, and Latino six hypotheses.
defendants charged with drug offenses, will
receive more severe penalties than white Alternatives to the Socio-Cultural
defendants charged with these offenses or Perspective
black and Latino defendants charged with
other offenses. The socio-cultural theory of punishment con-
The literatures discussed thus far highlight trasts with two alternative perspectives. First,
the need to conceive of race and ethnicity as formal rationality in sentencing is the ideal to
historically-specific cultural categories, the which many legislatures subscribe and that
meaning of which varies over time and motivates the adoption of sentencing guide-
across space. They also suggest that behav- lines. In a formally rational system, only
iors and places, not just people, may be legal variables such as offender history and
racially and ethnically coded. That is, the offense seriousness affect sentencing out-
association of a stigmatized minority group comes; extra-legal factors such as race and
with a particular place or behavior may ethnicity do not. If judges consider only
mean that white people who commit racially legally relevant factors (i.e., offense serious-
stigmatized offenses in places associated ness and offender history), and the extra-
with stigmatized minority groups may legal factors and interactions among them
10 American Sociological Review XX(X)

emphasized in our hypotheses do not influ- Washington State Administrative Office of


ence penal outcomes, we would conclude the Courts (AOC). The sample contains
that judicial decision-making in Washington 3,256 cases without missing data on the var-
State conforms to the ideal of formal ratio- iables analyzed.
nality, and we would reject the socio-cultural
theory.
Our socio-cultural perspective can also be Diagnostics and Model
contrasted with an organizational efficiency Specification
model, which emphasizes the institutional
need for an orderly sentencing system and Exploratory data analysis using Ordinary
the importance of the economic sustenance Least Squares (OLS) techniques on the
of the institution itself (Dixon 1995; Engen pooled conviction data showed systematic
and Steen 2000). In studies of confinement variation in monetary sanctions across coun-
sentencing, researchers interpret evidence ties. This suggests that the data are structured
that defendants convicted at trial receive lon- in nested groups and lend themselves to mul-
ger prison sentences than defendants who tilevel analysis. A two-level hierarchical
plead guilty as evidence of an organizational linear model (HLM) nests cases (i.e., convic-
need to encourage efficiency. According to tions) within groups (i.e., counties) to statis-
this interpretation, defendants who exercise tically isolate the impact of offense and
their right to a trial consume significant court offender characteristics from the effects of
resources and imperil the efficient operation county-level factors. As a result, this method-
of the courts; they therefore receive a ‘‘trial ology allows us to identify legal and extra-
penalty’’ at sentencing. By contrast, defend- legal variables that influence monetary out-
ants who plead are rewarded with shorter comes regardless of the characteristics of
confinement sentences. If applied to mone- the county in which a conviction occurred.
tary sanctions, the organizational efficiency HLM also enables us to identify the county
perspective would predict that financial pen- characteristics that are significantly related
alties are assessed primarily to recoup crimi- to variation in monetary sanctions by
nal justice expenditures; counties that spend accounting for error structure at the convic-
a relatively large percentage of their budgets tion (Level 1) and county (Level 2) levels.
on criminal justice would therefore impose This method helps correct downward biased
comparatively large monetary sanctions. estimates of standard errors due to nesting
(Raudenbush and Bryk 2002) and allows us
to model cross-level interactions that are
METHODS, DATA, AND integral to our hypotheses.7
Diagnostics helped shape the specified
ESTIMATION models. Partial plots suggested that one
This study presents a multilevel analysis of county, King County, served as a potential
conviction-level data gathered from Wash- outlier on a number of dimensions. We
ington State Superior Courts and county- keep this county in the model for statistical
level data drawn from the U.S. Census and substantive reasons. Statistical tests indi-
Bureau and other government sources. The cate that King County, although an outlier,
sample includes 3,366 Superior Court con- does not have considerable leverage, suggest-
victions sentenced in Washington State dur- ing little overall influence or bias to our
ing the first two months of 2004. We supple- results. Substantively, King County is the
ment case-level data with criminal history largest metropolitan area in Washington
data obtained through the Justice Information State and has considerable political and eco-
System (JIS) and maintained by the nomic influence; excluding King County
Harris et al. 11

from an analysis of Washington’s justice sys- to determine individuals’ length of incarcera-


tem would be misleading. Because our model tion from a determinate sentencing grid.9
contains varying group (county) sizes (rang- SRA seriousness level is a numerical mea-
ing from 2 to 536 cases), we conducted a sen- sure of the seriousness of the offense charge,
sitivity analysis by omitting each county in ranging from a low of 1 to a high of 16.10
turn, comparing point estimate direction Offender score is also derived from a stat-
and significance for each variable afterward. ute-based formula that reflects the number
Results indicate that no single county unduly and seriousness of defendants’ previous juve-
influences the results.8 We also conducted nile and criminal convictions. Offender
tests for heteroskedasticity and multicolli- scores in our sample range from 0 to 9. We
nearity that guided our model selection and dropped all cases in which defendants did
prompted variable transformations. not have a legally determined offender
score.11 We tested confinement length, but
Variables and Measurement due to multicollinearity, we do not include
it in the final analysis.12 Type of offense is
Dependent variable: fines and fees.
incorporated as three dummy variables: vio-
Legal Financial Obligations (LFOs) include lent offense, drug offense, and other
fines and fees (a single category in court offense.13 Trial (i.e., adjudication method)
records), restitution, and other monetary is also a dummy variable indicating whether
charges associated with arrest, court proceed- individuals pled guilty or opted to have their
ings, and incarceration. Our analysis focuses case taken to trial.14 We also include non-
on fines and fees imposed by judges at the legal factors, including age, gender, and
time of sentencing as the outcome of interest. race/ethnicity of the defendant associated
We exclude restitution from the analysis with each conviction. Age is a continuous,
because it is driven by case-specific factors logged variable in the analysis. Because
such as property replacement and victims’ court records indicate that 14 individuals in
medical costs (Ellsworth and Weisheit the sample were 105 years of age, we drop-
1997; Olson and Ramker 2001; Ruback, ped these cases. After these adjustments,
Shaffer, and Logue 2004), a source of varia- the sample contains 3,256 cases without
tion for which we cannot control. In Wash- missing data on the variables analyzed.
ington State, the minimum LFO is $500; Using the U.S. Census as our guide, we
any amount above $500 is determined at use the term race to refer to officially desig-
the discretion of the presiding judge. In our nated racial categories (i.e., American Indian
sample, the maximum amount assessed was or Alaska Native, Asian, Black or African
$11,960. The variable fines and fees is American, Native Hawaiian or other Pacific
heavily skewed to the right; fines and fees Islander, and White), and the term ethnicity
is logged in the regression analysis, further to refer to subgroups within racial categories
normalizing its distribution and readily lend- (e.g., Hispanic, Latino/not Hispanic, or
ing itself to interpretation. Latino).15 We construct race as a set of
Case- and conviction-level data. We dummy variables including white, black or
include several case-level variables in this African American, Asian, Native American,
analysis. Legal case-level factors include and other. We use Hispanic Surname Analy-
the Sentencing Reform Act (SRA) score, sis to estimate the proportion of white, black,
offender score, type of offense, adjudication and other defendants who are Latino. Using
method, and length of confinement. SRA the U.S. Census Spanish Surname database,
seriousness level and offender score were this program assigns a numeric value
established by the Washington State Sentenc- between 0 and 1 to all surnames in the data-
ing Reform Act of 1981, and judges use them base. These numeric values are provided by
12 American Sociological Review XX(X)

the U.S. Census Department and represent a unit increase in X. In this analysis, we pres-
the probability that a given surname corre- ent three different sets of models. The first
sponds to persons who identified themselves model is a random intercept model that
as Hispanic/Latino in the 1990 U.S. Census includes all racial and all offense categories;
(Perkins 1993; Word and Perkins 1996). contextual factors included are percent
The list used to identify defendants of His- minority, percent who voted Republican,
panic origin in the Washington State Sen- and percent of the budget spent on law and
tencing Guidelines Commission (WSSGC) justice. The formal description of the model
data includes only Spanish surnames that is written as the following:
the Census Bureau classifies as ‘‘Heavily
Hispanic.’’16 Set1: Logfine = aj[i] 1 b1(SRA)
1 b2(Offender Score) 1 b3(lnAge)
County-level variables. Previous research
1 b4(Male) 1 b5(Latino) 1 b6(Black)
identifies contextual factors that may shape
sentencing outcomes. We analyzed a variety 1 b7(Asian) 1 b8(Native American)
1 b9(Other Ethnicity) 1 b10(Drug Offense)
of contextual factors to test other theoretical
1 b11(Other Offense) 1 b12(Trial) aj ~ N(aj
frameworks and to refine our own. Key
1 g1(% Minority) 1 g2(% Vote Republican)
demographic factors include percent of
1 g3(% Law & Justice), s2)
a county population that is minority (i.e.,
neither white nor Asian17), percent black,
where Logfine is fees and fines in logged dol-
and percent Latino. In addition, we include
lar amounts, SRA is offense seriousness level,
county violent crime rate, property crime
rate, drug arrest rate, and percent of the Offender Score is the score based on the
number and seriousness of past offenses,
county budget spent on law and justice.
LnAge is logged age, Male is a dummy equal
Because research shows that political con-
to 1 for male, Latino is a dummy equal to 1
text influences incarceration rates (Beckett
for Latino offenders, Black is a dummy equal
1997; Jacobs and Carmichael 2001), we
to 1 for black or African American offenders,
also include political orientation measured
Asian is a dummy equal to 1 for Asian Amer-
by percent of a county that voted Republi-
ican offenders, Native American is a dummy
can in the 2000 presidential election. (See
Table A1 in the Appendix sources for each equal to 1 for Native American offenders,
Other Ethnicity is a dummy equal to 1 for
county-level independent variable). We
all other racial/ethnic identified offenders,
tested logged population size, poverty rate,
Drug Offense is a dummy equal to 1 for
percent change in the Latino population,
drug offenses, Other Offense is a dummy
and presence/absence of a public defender
equal to 1 for nondrug offenses, Trial is
system in the county. However, because
a dummy equal to 1 for cases adjudicated
none of these factors significantly improved
via a judicial or jury trial, % Minority is
the model, we do not present the results
here. We used AIC (Akaike 1974), BIC, non-white percentage of the population in
the county, % Vote Republican is percentage
and other goodness-of-fit measures to guide
of people in the county voting Republican in
our final model selection.
the 2000 presidential election, and % Law &
Estimation. Using the statistical software Justice is percent of the county budget spent
Stata 10, we estimated multilevel, mixed on law and justice.
effects models using restricted maximum The second and third sets of models
likelihood estimation (REML) to produce include cross-level interactions. We construct
unbiased estimates. Because the dependent interactions as the product of the two varia-
variable is logged, regression coefficients bles specified. Each interaction is tested
are interpreted as a percent change in Y for separately (i.e., there is never more than
Harris et al. 13

one interaction in a model). We ran these to 16, the mean SRA offense seriousness
models with unstructured covariances to score is 2.9 and the mean offender score is
accommodate the additional random effects 2.5. The average age of persons sentenced
and to test our hypotheses regarding fine is 32 years. The majority of the sample
and fee assessment for blacks and violent is male (81 percent) and white (68
offenders and Latinos and drug offenders, percent). The average sentence length was
respectively. To fully test our theoretical 13.5 months and the maximum was 814
framework, variables in the model remained months.
uncentered.18 In the second set of models, Table 2 shows descriptive statistics for the
we include percent black and violent crime outcome and key independent variables by
rate. In the third set, we include percent racial and ethnic categories. A larger share
Latino and drug arrest rate. The formal of black offenders were convicted of violent
description of these models, including an offenses (15 percent) than were Latinos (13
example interaction term, can be written as percent) or whites (8 percent); black defend-
the following: ants also had higher average SRA scores
(3.3) than Latinos (3.2) or whites (2.8). Black
Set2: Logfine = aj[i] 1 b1(SRA) offenders had the highest mean offender score
1 b2(Offender Score) 1 b3(LnAge) (3.2) compared with Latinos (1.6) and whites
1 b4(Male) 1 b5(Black) (2.6). Black offenders in the sample tended to
1 b6(Violent Offense) 1 b7(Trial) aj ~ N(aj be older (33.7 years) than whites (32.6) and
1 g1(% Black) 1 g2(% Vote Republican) Latinos (29.4). Black offenders had the lon-
1 g3(% Violent Crime Rate) gest average confinement sentence length
1 g4(% Black * Black), s2) (19.4 months) compared with whites (13.1
months) and Latinos (11.8 months). Nonethe-
Set3: Logfine = aj[i] 1 b1(SRA) less, Latinos had the highest average assess-
1 b2(Offender Score) 1 b3(LnAge) ment of fines and fees ($1,666), followed by
1 b4(Male) 1 b5(Latino) whites ($1,458), and then blacks ($978).
1 b6(Drug Offense) 1 b7(Trial) aj ~ N(aj These descriptive data provide preliminary
1 g1(Latino) 1 g2(% Vote Republican) evidence that offense seriousness is not the
1 g3(Drug Arrest Rate) main driver of monetary sanctioning.
1 g4(% Latino * Drug Offense), s2) Before describing the results of our
regression analyses, we reiterate our
hypotheses:
RESULTS
Hypothesis 1: Drug offenders and violent
We begin by presenting descriptive statis- offenders will receive more severe financial
tics. Table 1 shows the means, medians, penalties than will other (mainly property)
standard deviations, and minimum and offenders.
maximum values for all variables. In Wash- Hypothesis 2: Latino and black defendants will
ington State, felony conviction results in receive more severe financial penalties than
will their white counterparts.
a mandatory $500 minimum charge. Fines
Hypothesis 3: Judges in jurisdictions with
and fees beyond $500 are assessed at the larger black or Latino populations will
discretion of the court. In our sample of impose more severe monetary penalties
3,256 convictions, 10 percent were assessed than will judges in jurisdictions with com-
the minimum; the mean amount assessed paratively small black or Latino populations.
was $1,398, the median was $1,110, Hypothesis 4: The effect of the size of the
and the maximum fee and fine amount black or Latino population will be greater
assessed was $11,960. On a scale from 1 for black and Latino defendants sentenced
14 American Sociological Review XX(X)

Table 1. Description of Variables and Summary Statistics

Mean Median SD Min. Max. Obs.

Dependent Variable: Fines and Fees $1,398 $1,110 $984 $500 $11,960 3,256
Level 1: Case Covariates
SRA Score 2.91 2 2.38 1 15 3,256
Offender Score 2.50 2 2.73 0 9 3,256
Age 32.36 31 9.84 17 72 3,256
Male .81 .40 0 1 3,256
Black .13 .33 0 1 3,256
White .68 .46 0 1 3,256
Latino .10 .30 0 1 3,256
Asian .02 .15 0 1 3,256
Native American .02 .15 0 1 3,256
Other .04 .19 0 1 3,256
Trial .03 .16 0 1 3,256
Drug Offense .34 .47 0 1 3,256
Violent Offense .09 .29 0 1 3,256
Other Offense .57 .49 0 1 3,256
Superior Court Priors 1.74 1 2.82 0 40 3,256
Other Court Priors 6.19 3 7.91 0 75 3,255
Months Sentenced 13.55 5 30.79 0 814 3,256
Level 2: County Covariates
Population 2004 159,155 57,238 315,354.2 2,313 1,777,746 39
Percent Minoritya 16.32 11.30 12.63 3.6 51.6 39
Percent Latino 10.89 5.80 12.44 2.1 50.9 39
Percent Black 1.27 0.70 1.45 0 7.1 39
Percent Poverty 13.18 13.00 2.76 8.3 18.8 39
Political Orientationb 54.44 54.86 10.46 34.39 73.89 39
Violent Crime Ratec 2.39 2.43 1.09 .46 5.41 39
Property Crime Ratec 39.62 36.79 15.38 5.18 67.24 39
Drug Arrest Ratec 3.28 3.19 1.82 0 7.2 39
Percent Budget on Law and Justiced 24.69 24.70 5.91 10.73 37.33 39
Percent Change in Latino Populatione 99.98 98.33 58.35 250.7 212.40 39
a
This measure includes the percentage of non-white and non-Asian inhabitants in 2005.
b
Percent of the county who voted Republican in the 2000 presidential election.
c
Rate = number of incidents per 1,000 people in 2004.
d
Percent of the county’s budget spent on law and justice in 2003.
e
Percent change in Latino population from 1990 to 2000.

in counties with relatively large black or severe monetary penalties than will their
Latino populations. counterparts in counties with smaller black
Hypothesis 5: Black defendants charged with or Latino populations.
violent crimes, and Latino defendants
charged with drug offenses, will receive
more severe monetary penalties than will Case- and County-Level Factors
black and Latino defendants charged with
less stereotype-congruent offenses.
Several case-level factors influence the
Hypothesis 6: All defendants convicted of vio- imposition of monetary sanctions. As
lent offenses in counties with larger black Hypothesis 1 predicts, offense type signifi-
populations, and all defendants convicted cantly affects the assessment of monetary
of drug offenses in counties with larger sanctions (see Table 3). Drug offenders are
Latino populations, will receive more assessed substantially (34 percent) greater
Harris et al. 15

Table 2. Descriptive Statistics, Comparing Racial and Ethnic Groups

Blacks Latinos Whites All

Dependent Variable
Fines and Fees $978 $1,666 $1,458 $1,399
(699) (1,008) (1,030) (984)
Key Covariates
Age 33.70 29.35 32.63 32.36
(10.33) (8.88) (9.76) (9.84)
Males .86 .89 .79 .81
(.35) (.32) (.41) (.40)
SRA Score 3.34 3.20 2.78 2.91
(2.48) (2.48) (2.33) (2.38)
Offender Score 3.16 1.60 2.55 2.50
(2.86) (2.07) (2.77) (2.73)
Drug Offense .30 .39 .34 .34
(.46) (.49) (.47) (.47)
Violent Offense .15 .13 .08 .09
(.36) (.33) (.27) (.29)
Other Offense .55 .49 .58 .57
(.49) (.50) (.49) (.49)
Superior Court Priors 1.94 1.16 1.78 1.74
(2.61) (2.41) (2.81) (2.82)
Trial .019 .018 .027 .026
(.14) (.14) (.16) (.16)
Months Sentenced 19.47 11.82 13.15 13.55
(39.24) (24.89) (31.04) (30.80)
N 417 327 2,231 3,256

Note: Data shown are means with standard errors in parentheses.

fines and fees than are property offenders and At the county level, Hypothesis 3 high-
violent offenders (31 percent). Contrary to lights the effect of county demographics on
our expectations, however, violent offenders monetary sanctions. This initial test does
do not receive significantly greater fines not support Hypothesis 3, as the share of
and fees than nonviolent offenders after con- a county population that is comprised of peo-
trolling for offense seriousness. The results ple of color (i.e., black, Latino, or Native
provide partial support for Hypothesis 2. American) does not significantly predict the
Latinos are assessed 6.7 percent greater fees dollar value of monetary sanctions assessed
and fines than similarly situated white by judges. We do find evidence, however,
defendants, although blacks do not receive that more conservative counties impose
harsher assessments than whites. In short, greater monetary sanctions. The share of
these results suggest that Latinos and drug the budget devoted to the criminal justice
offenders receive significantly more severe system does not significantly predict LFO
monetary penalties than do non-Latino and assessment. Our model explains roughly 22
nondrug offenders. Consistent with prior percent of the variation at each level.
findings on confinement sentencing out-
comes, individuals convicted at trial are Cross-Level Interactions
assessed 30 percent greater fines and fees,
on average, than defendants who plead Hypotheses 4, 5, and 6 are more complex and
guilty, and men are assessed greater mone- predict that case-level characteristics will
tary sanctions than women.19 interact with contextual factors to enhance
16 American Sociological Review XX(X)

Table 3. HLM Results of Fines and Fees non-blacks and Latinos with non-Latinos.
Assessed Table 4 presents results of four multilevel
mixed effects models in which race is mea-
Fines and Fees (Logged)
sured as black or non-black, and offense
Case Level
SRA score .011***
type is measured as violent or nonviolent.
(.003) County-level factors include percent of the
Offender Score .003 population that is black, percent voting
(.002) Republican, and the violent crime rate.
Ln Age .005 Results from Model 1 indicate that SRA
(.019)
Male .036*
score, adjudication method (trial versus
(.015) plea), and offense type are important predic-
Black 2.019 tors of fine and fee assessment. Contrary to
(.019) our expectations, violent offenders are
Latino .067*** assessed roughly 11 percent lower fees and
(.021)
Asian 2.039
fines than their nonviolent counterparts, and
(.040) black defendants do not receive significantly
Native American 2.051 greater sanctions than non-blacks (the coeffi-
(.039) cient is negative but not significant). Without
Other 2.011 interactions, our model explains roughly 10
(.036)
Trial .302***
percent of the variation at the case level
(.038) and 16 percent at the county level.
Drug Offense .314*** Results shown in Models 2 through 4 in
(.024) Table 4 add important caveats to these find-
Other Offense 2.026 ings. These results provide additional evi-
(.023)
Intercept 6.59
dence that there is no direct penalty for being
(.404) black, and that the size of the black popula-
County Level tion alone does not influence the imposition
Percent Minority 2.003 of monetary sanctions. On the other hand,
(.005) findings regarding all of the three predicted
Percent Vote Republican .013*
(.006)
interactions are positive, and two of these
Percent Law and Justice 2.008 are significant. Consistent with Hypothesis
(.010) 4, assessment of monetary sanctions depends
Level-1 Variance .117 on the size of the black population in the
Level-1 Explained Variation .217 county in which a black defendant is con-
Level-2 Variance .108
Level-2 Explained Variation .226
victed. Put differently, these findings indicate
Level-1 N 3,256 that the effect of race depends on the size of
Level-2 N 39 the black population in a given county. A
black defendant convicted in Garfield
Note: Data shown are hierarchical linear model
(HLM) regression coefficients; standard errors
County, which has no black residents, would
are in parentheses. Reference categories: race = be assessed 11.8 percent less than a non-
white; offense = violent. black defendant. By contrast, in Pierce
*p \ .05; ** p \ .01; *** p \ .001 (two-tailed County, where 7.1 percent of the population
tests).
is black, a black defendant could expect to
pay 3.1 percent more in fines and fees than
the effect of racialized scripts and stereotypes a non-black defendant.20
on sentencing outcomes. Because we think it The interaction between being black
is possible that race and ethnicity matter dif- and being convicted of a violent crime is pos-
ferently, the models compare blacks with itive but not significant. Consistent with
Harris et al. 17

Table 4. Blacks and Violence: HLM Results of Fines and Fees Assessed

Fines and Fees (logged) Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4

Case Level
SRA Score .017*** .017*** .017*** .017***
(.003) (.003) (.003) (.003)
Offender Score 2.004 2.005 2.004 2.005
(.002) (.002) (.002) (.002)
Ln Age .063** .063** .063** .065**
(.022) (.022) (.022) (.022)
Male .014 .014 .014 .015
(.017) (.017) (.017) (.017)
Black 2.014 2.118* 2.022 2.013
(.020) (.048) (.022) (.020)
Trial .327*** .326*** .327*** .328***
(.042) (.042) (.042) (.042)
Violent Offense 2.109*** 2.106*** 2.120*** 2.200***
(.025) (.025) (.027) (.039)
Intercept 6.54 6.55 6.54 6.54
(.397) (.397) (.397) (.397)
County Level
Percent Black 2.079 2.081 2.078 2.080
(.054) (.054) (.054) (.054)
Percent Vote Republican .007 .007 .007 .007
(.006) (.006) (.006) (.006)
Violent Crime Rate .052 .053 .052 .051
(.068) (.068) (.068) (.068)
Interactions
Percent Black x Black .021*
(.009)
Violent Offense x Black .057
(.056)
Violent Offense x Percent Black .027**
(.009)
Level-1 Variance .131 .131 .131 .131
Level-1 Explained Variation .102 .102 .102 .101
Level-2 Variance .127 .127 .127 .127
Level-2 Explained Variation .163 .163 .163 .160
Level-1 N 3,256 3,256 3,256 3,256
Level-2 N 39 39 39 39

Note: Data shown are hierarchical linear model (HLM) regression coefficients; standard errors are in
parentheses. Reference categories: race = non-black; offense = nonviolent.
*p \ .05; ** p \ .01; *** p \ .001 (two-tailed tests).

Hypothesis 6, however, monetary sanctions share of the population is black, a violent


for violent offenders depend on the size of offender (regardless of his race or ethnicity)
the black population: counties with larger can expect to pay .83 percent less than a non-
black communities monetarily penalize vio- violent offender (see Table 4, Model 4).
lent offenders more severely. For example, Adjudication by trial continues to positively
a violent offender in Garfield County can and significantly increase the severity of
expect to receive a fine and fee assessment monetary penalties in all of the models.
20 percent lower than a nonviolent offender. In short, the findings presented in Table 4
By contrast, in Pierce County, where a larger indicate that blacks sentenced in counties
18 American Sociological Review XX(X)

Table 5. Latinos and Drugs: HLM Results of Fines and Fees Assessed

Fines and Fees (logged) Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4

Case Level
SRA Score .012*** .012*** .012*** .012***
(.002) (.002) (.002) (.002)
Offender Score .003 .003 .003 .003
(.002) (.002) (.002) (.002)
Ln Age .004 .004 .006 .013
(.019) (.019) (.019) (0.19)
Male .036* .036* .037* .033*
(.015) (.015) (.015) (.015)
Latino .072*** .060* .019 .068***
(.021) (.031) (.025) (.020)
Trial .303*** .303*** .306*** .306***
(.038) (.038) (.038) (.038)
Drug Offense .337*** .337*** .321*** .245***
(.013) (.013) (.013) (.017)
Intercept 6.15 6.15 6.15 6.15
(.321) (.321) (.321) (.324)
County Level
Percent Latino 2.005 2.005 2.005 -.008
(.005) (.005) (.005) (.005)
Percent Vote Republican .013* .013* .013* .013*
(.006) (.006) (.006) (.006)
Drug Arrest Rate .067* .067* .067* .067*
(.031) (.031) (.031) (.031)
Interactions
Percent Latino x Latino .001
(.001)
Drug Offense x Latino .142***
(.404)
Drug Offense x Percent Latino .009***
(.001)

Level-1 Variance .108 .108 .107 .106


Level-1 Explained Variation .259 .259 .258 .256
Level-2 Variance .105 .105 .106 .108
Level-2 Explained Variation .304 .305 .300 .288
Level-1 N 3,256 3,256 3,256 3,256
Level-2 N 39 39 39 39

Note: Data shown are hierarchical linear model (HLM) regression coefficients; standard errors are in
parentheses. Reference categories: race = non-Latino; offense = nondrug.
*p \ .05; ** p \ .01; *** p \ .001 (two-tailed tests).

with larger black populations, and all defend- models that include the proportion of Latinos
ants convicted of violent charges in counties in a county; ethnicity is measured as Latino
with larger black populations, receive signifi- or non-Latino, and offense type as drug
cantly greater monetary penalties. For black offense or nondrug offense. Because stigma
defendants, we find support for Hypotheses 4 related to drugs is relevant to our hypotheses,
and 6, but only weak support for Hypothesis 5. we include the county drug arrest rate rather
A similarly complex dynamic plays out than violent crime rate in these models.
for Latinos. Table 5 presents results from Results of Model 1 support Hypotheses 1
Harris et al. 19

and 2: drug offenders can expect, on average, share of Latinos would receive a penalty
to be assessed 34 percent higher fines and 70.3 percent higher than someone convicted
fees than nondrug offenders, and Hispanic of a nondrug offense in that county. The
defendants are assessed 7 percent more than presence of a relatively large Latino popula-
their non-Latino counterparts. As in the pre- tion appears to heighten concern about drug
vious models, adjudication by trial is signifi- offenders, regardless of individual defend-
cantly and positively associated with mone- ants’ race or ethnicity.
tary sanctions. Without interactions, the In summary, we find strong but not uni-
variables included in the model explain 26 versal support for our hypotheses as they per-
percent of the variation at the individual level tain to Latino defendants. Consistent with
and 30 percent at the county level. Hypotheses 1 and 2, drug offenders and
Given the consistency of the findings pre- Latino defendants receive significantly
sented thus far regarding the significance of greater fees and fines than do nondrug and
ethnicity and drugs, it is not surprising that non-Latino offenders. Contrary to Hypothe-
the intersection of these variables is signifi- ses 3 and 4, we do not find that the size of
cantly associated with more severe financial the Latino population alone matters, or that
sentences. On the one hand, we find only Latino defendants in jurisdictions with larger
weak evidence that the penalty for being Latino populations receive higher monetary
Latino varies according to the demographic sanctions. However, consistent with Hypoth-
composition of the county in which one is eses 5 and 6, Latinos convicted of drug
convicted (Hypothesis 4): the interaction offenses received greater monetary sanctions
between being Latino and percent Latino in than non-Latinos convicted of drug offenses,
a county is positive but not statistically sig- and drug offenders sentenced in jurisdictions
nificant. However, consistent with Hypothe- with large Latino populations received
sis 5, if a defendant is convicted of a drug greater LFOs than those in counties with
offense, being Latino will increase fine and smaller Latino populations.
fee assessment by 14 percent. In total,
a Latino defendant convicted of a drug crime
can expect to pay 46.3 percent more than DISCUSSION AND
a non-Latino, nondrug offender. And, as
Hypothesis 6 predicts, the impact of the
CONCLUSIONS
stigma associated with drugs depends on Our findings, summarized in Table 6, are
the size of the Latino community. That is, largely supportive of our socio-cultural the-
all defendants convicted of a drug offense ory of punishment. This theory suggests
(regardless of their race or ethnicity) in that decisions about sanctions are shaped by
a county with a large Latino population emotions that are especially likely to be trig-
receive significantly greater fines and fees gered by certain defendants accused of cer-
than do defendants convicted of drug crimes tain crimes in certain contexts. Because prior
in counties with small Latino populations. To studies indicate that blackness, brownness,
make this concrete: in Washington counties, violence, and drugs are associated with par-
the Latino population ranges from 2.1 to ticularly negative emotions and moral judg-
50.9 percent. At the two extremes, a person ments, we hypothesized that these qualities
convicted of a drug crime in a county whose would be significantly associated with the
population is 2.1 percent Latino would severity of monetary sanctions. Consistent
receive a monetary penalty 26.4 percent with our conception of race and ethnicity as
higher than if the person was convicted of complex and shifting cultural categories
a nondrug related felony; a comparable per- rather than just individual attributes, we fur-
son convicted in the county with the largest ther hypothesized that these associations
20 American Sociological Review XX(X)

Table 6. Hypotheses by Expected and Observed Impact and Statistical Significance

Expected Impact Observed Impact Statistically Significant


on Monetary on Monetary Confirmation of
Sanctions Sanctions Hypothesis

Hypothesis 1: Offense Type


Violent offenders will receive Positive Negative No
greater LFOs
Drug offenders will receive Positive Positive* Yes
greater LFOs
Hypothesis 2: Race/Ethnicity of
Defendant
Black defendants will receive Positive Negative No
greater LFOs
Latino defendants will receive Positive Positive* Yes
greater LFOs
Hypothesis 3: Racial/Ethnic
Composition of County
Counties with larger black Positive Negative No
populations will impose
greater LFOs
Counties with larger Latino Positive Negative No
populations will impose
greater LFOs
Hypothesis 4: Interaction of
County Demographics and
Defendant Race/Ethnicity
Black defendants in counties Positive Positive* Yes
with larger black
populations will receive
greater LFOs
Latino defendants in counties Positive Negative No
with larger Latino
populations will receive
greater LFOs
Hypothesis 5: Interaction of
Defendant Race/Ethnicity and
Offense Type
Black violent offenders will Positive Positive No
receive greater LFOs
Latino drug offenders will Positive Positive* Yes
receive greater LFOs
Hypothesis 6: Interaction of Size
of Black/Latino Population
and Offense Type
Violent offenders in counties Positive Positive* Yes
with large black populations
will receive greater LFOs
Drug offenders in counties Positive Positive* Yes
with large Latino
populations will receive
greater LFOs

Note: Asterisk signifies a statistically significant correlation.


Harris et al. 21

would be interactive and triggered by key Zatz 1998; Schlesinger 2005; Steffensmeier
contextual variables. Because the meanings and Demuth 2000, 2001). Steffensmeier and
and effects of race and ethnicity vary across Demuth (2000:708) suggest that in the con-
space and time, and because the Latino pop- text of heated debate about immigration,
ulation is expanding rapidly, we suggested Latinos may be viewed as more ‘‘culturally
that ethnicity may be more salient than race dissimilar and threatening’’ than either
in Washington State. blacks or whites (see also Hagan and Palloni
Overall, our findings provide support for 1999; Martinez 1996; Zatz 1985). Our find-
this perspective. Specifically, we find that ings are consistent with this argument.
ethnicity (being Hispanic) and drug charges Two factors may help to explain why nei-
(associated with being Hispanic) directly ther black nor violent offenders receive
increase penal severity. We also find that greater monetary sanctions than do non-black
Latinos convicted of drug charges and drug and nonviolent offenders. First, the absence of
offenders convicted in counties with larger a direct black effect likely reflects the small
Latino populations receive significantly and stable size of the black population in
greater LFOs than do other defendants. Washington State, which can be contrasted
Thus, ethnicity appears to matter in direct with the comparatively large and growing
and interactive ways. Although there is no size of the Latino and immigrant popula-
direct financial penalty for being black or tions.23 Second, the fact that violent offenders
being convicted of the type of offense (i.e., do not receive greater monetary sanctions than
violent) most strongly associated with black- nonviolent offenders may reflect a tendency to
ness, black defendants sentenced in counties see incarceration as the appropriate response
with comparatively large black populations to individuals who engender fear, while finan-
also receive more severe monetary penalties. cial penalties may seem more appropriate for
Moreover, monetary sanctions for violent defendants seen as disgusting or morally cor-
offenders depend on the size of the black rupt (i.e., drug offenders).
population. Race appears to matter, but in In summary, we find strong evidence that
more complex and interactive ways than eth- ethnicity and its interaction with offense type
nicity in Washington State. Several control significantly impact the imposition of mone-
variables also significantly influence the tary sanctions in Washington State. Latino
assessment of fees and fines: counties with defendants whose charges are stereotype
more politically conservative voters impose congruent are penalized most harshly. We
harsher financial penalties21 (see also Jacobs also find robust evidence of racial and ethnic
and Carmichael 2001), and male defendants courtesy stigma: all defendants, regardless of
receive greater monetary sanctions than do their race or ethnicity, are punished more
females. Finally, as in studies of confinement severely when they are convicted of racially
sentencing outcomes, adjudication method or ethnically coded crimes in counties with
significantly predicts penal outcomes: comparatively large black or Latino popula-
defendants who exercise their right to a trial tions. By contrast, we find no evidence that
receive greater monetary penalties than do counties that spend a comparatively large
individuals who plead guilty.22 share of their budgets on law and justice
These findings are consistent with an impose greater monetary penalties, as the
emerging body of evidence that suggests organizational efficiency model predicts.
ethnicity has become a comparatively salient Moreover, only one of the two legal factors
factor in decisions to punish. Indeed, several allowable in a formally rational sentencing
recent studies indicate that Latinos now system (i.e., SRA score) significantly
receive longer prison sentences than either predicts the imposition of monetary sanc-
whites or blacks (Demuth 2003; Mann and tions, and many socio-legal factors retain
22 American Sociological Review XX(X)

significance after taking these two legal fac- their impact on institutional outcomes such
tors into account. It thus appears that judicial as sanctions. Prior studies suggest that partic-
decisions about monetary sanctions are ular racial and ethnic groups are culturally
shaped by many of the socio-cultural pro- associated with certain offense types, and
cesses we foreground—ethnic dynamics in that activation of these scripts by stereo-
particular—rather than by an organizational type-congruent defendants triggers enhanced
imperative to recoup criminal justice costs. punishment (Jones and Kaplan 2003; Schle-
Like other sentencing studies, our analysis singer 2005; Steen et al. 2005). Similarly,
is limited by the absence of data regarding we find that Latinos convicted of stereo-
unobserved individual-level characteristics. type-congruent offenses (i.e., drug crimes)
For example, our models do not include receive significantly greater financial penal-
information about defendants’ legal repre- ties than do Latinos convicted of other crimes
sentation or pretrial release status (e.g., and non-Latinos convicted of drug crimes.
bail), education, employment status, or Our results further indicate, however, that
income level. In addition, although results racialized crime scripts affect not only
of the HLM regression analysis identify the defendants whose ethnicity is consistent
factors that significantly influence punish- with the stereotype in question, but all
ment severity, they do not reveal how each defendants convicted of racially or ethnically
of the significant explanatory factors influen- stigmatized behavior. That is, counties with
ces these outcomes. We theorize that offense comparatively large Latino populations
type, race, ethnicity, and demographic con- penalize all drug offenders significantly
text interact to influence penal outcomes by more severely than do counties with small
triggering the negative emotions and moral Latino populations. Similarly, counties with
judgments associated with heightened puni- relatively large black populations penalize
tiveness, but we do not have direct measures all violent offenders (of all racial and ethnic
of these processes. backgrounds) significantly more severely
As a result, some of our findings are than do counties with small black popula-
potentially subject to multiple interpreta- tions. It appears that even white people who
tions. In particular, the finding that Latinos commit racially or ethnically stigmatized
are assessed comparatively greater monetary offenses in jurisdictions with comparatively
sanctions could be construed as evidence that large black or Latino populations will experi-
judges perceive Latino defendants as better ence the courtesy stigma (Goffman 1963)
able or more likely to pay than white or black that flows from racialization, and they will
defendants. Although we cannot definitively be penalized more severely as a result.
disprove this interpretation, we think it is Race and ethnicity, then, are not just individ-
incorrect, for two reasons. First, as noted pre- ual attributes, but cultural categories that
viously, judges are not required by statute to shape the distribution of stigma and the insti-
assess or even consider defendants’ ability to tutional consequences that flow from it.
pay when imposing fees or fines, and our The theoretical framework we develop
interviews with court actors and observations here, and especially our findings regarding
of courtroom proceedings indicate that they courtesy stigma, may be useful for under-
do not. Second, we think it is unlikely that standing decision-making processes in other
judges believe Latinos are better able or institutional settings in which clients are
more likely to pay than whites, given com- labeled, processed, and punished. Indeed,
paratively high poverty rates among Latino studies indicate that race and ethnicity affect
residents of Washington State.24 decision-making processes in a number of
Our findings have important implications institutions that are central to the stratifi-
for theories of racial and ethnic stigma and cation system, including schools, welfare
Harris et al. 23

offices, and penal institutions (Bridges and and surveillance has spread to non-criminal
Steen 1998; Morris 2005; Schram et al. justice institutions, as the ‘‘technologies, dis-
2009). For example, comparatively high rates courses, and metaphors of crime and criminal
of suspension among black students are in justice’’ have permeated politics, policies,
part a function of race-inflected assessments and institutions that are seemingly unrelated
of students’ behavior and character (Gregory to crime-fighting (Simon 2007:4). Schools,
1995; Morris 2005). Similarly, beliefs about for example, are increasingly subject to
blacks and Latinos inform welfare agents’ drug sweeps, K-9 units, metal detectors,
sanctioning decisions (Schram et al. 2009), zero-tolerance rules, and detention and
as well as probation officers’ assessments expulsion (Lyons and Drew 2006; Simon
of juveniles’ blameworthiness and amenabil- 2007). Similarly, welfare agencies are
ity (Bridges and Steen 1998). These institu- increasingly oriented toward surveillance
tional processes have important consequen- and sanctioning, as ‘‘disciplinary approaches
ces for individuals’ life chances and quality to poverty management are ascendant in the
of life, but they remain poorly understood. United States today’’ (Schram et al.
The possibility that courtesy stigma plays 2009:398; see also Soss et al. 2008). In this
an important role in these processes has not context, identifying and understanding the
been explored; our findings suggest that it complex ways that race, ethnicity, and other
should be. sources of stigma interact to influence insti-
Recent developments underscore the need tutional decision-making processes and the
for such research. Even as mass incarceration allocation of sanctions is of utmost
fuels inequality, the logic of penal control importance.
24 American Sociological Review XX(X)

APPENDIX
Table A1. County-Level Variables, Description, and Data Sources

Variable Description Source

Population 2004 Population (2004) U.S. Census Bureau, State and County Quick
Facts, http://quickfacts.census.gov/qfd/
states/53/53001.html
Percent Minority Percent non-white and U.S. Census Bureau, State and County Quick
non-Asian (2005) Facts, http://quickfacts.census.gov/qfd/
states/53/53001.html
Percent Latino Percent Hispanic/Latino U.S. Census Bureau, State and County Quick
(2005) Facts, http://quickfacts.census.gov/qfd/
states/53/53001.html
Percent Black Percent black (2005) U.S. Census Bureau, State and County Quick
Facts, http://quickfacts.census.gov/qfd/
states/53/53001.html
Percent Poverty Percent people living below U.S. Census Bureau, State and County Quick
poverty (2004) Facts, http://quickfacts.census.gov/qfd/
states/53/53001.html
Political Percent people who voted David Lublin and D. Stephen Voss. 2001.
Orientation Republican in 2000 ‘‘Federal Elections Project.’’ American
presidential election University, Washington, DC and the
University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY,
http://spa.american.edu/ccps/
pages.php?ID=12
Violent Crime Rate Violent crimes per 1,000 Office of Financial Management, Washington
people (2004) State, http://www.ofm.wa.gov/sac/
cjdatabook/default.asp
Property Crime Property crimes per 1,000 Office of Financial Management, Washington
Rate people (2004) State, http://www.ofm.wa.gov/sac/
cjdatabook/default.asp
Drug Arrest Rate Drug arrests per 1,000 people Office of Financial Management, Washington
(2004) State, http://www.ofm.wa.gov/sac/
cjdatabook/default.asp
Percent Budget Percent of county expenditures Local Government Financial Reporting
Spent on Law and allocated to law System, Washington State Auditor, http://
Justice and justice (2003) www.sao.wa.gov/applications/lgfrs/
Percent Change in Percent change in Latino U.S. Census Bureau, State and County Quick
Latino Population population from 1990 to 2000 Facts, http://quickfacts.census.gov/qfd/
states/53/53001.html

Acknowledgments of the authors contributed significantly to the research and


development of this article.
Thanks to Matt Baretto at the University of Washington
for providing the Hispanic Surname Analysis database,
and to Jennifer Creighton at the Washington State Admin- Funding
istrative Office of the Courts and Kari Steelhammer at the This study was funded in part by the Washington State
Washington State Department of Corrections for provid- Minority & Justice Commission and the Department of
ing data. We appreciate suggestions from Jerry Herting Sociology at the University of Washington.
and Bryan Sykes for the development of this article. Pre-
vious versions of this article were presented at the Amer- Notes
ican Society of Criminology and the Racial Democracy,
Crime and Justice Network at The Ohio State University. 1. In Washington State, for example, some jails assess
The authors are listed in reverse alphabetical order; each booking fees and charge inmates up to $100 a day
Harris et al. 25

for the cost of their detention, even prior to adjudi- No other variable was sensitive to the omission of
cation. Sentencing judges are also allowed to assess any one county, increasing our confidence in the
a fee for indigent defense counsel, and they may narrative we present.
not waive this fee even if the defendant is not con- 9. RCW 9.94(A).
victed or if his conviction is reversed upon appeal 10. RCW 9.94(A).515.
(Anderson 2009). 11. Unranked offenses are crimes that are rarely
2. In Pennsylvania, an effort is underway to structure charged or were recently created by the Washington
the imposition of economic sanctions based on an State legislature (n = 95). These offenses do not
offender’s ability to pay (see Act 2007-37 [SB have a seriousness level and do not require sentence
116, PN1323]). calculations based on the sentencing grid. Courts
3. See the Revised Code of Washington 7.68.035 and are required to impose a sentence ranging from
43.43.74. 0 to 365 days of confinement for persons convicted
4. U.S. Sentencing Commission National Association of unranked offenses. A sentence of more than
of State Sentencing Commissions website (http:// a year may be imposed, but it is considered an
nasc2010.alacourt.gov/NASC/Membershi- exceptional sentence and requires a written justifi-
p%20List.pdf, visited January 12, 2011). Informa- cation by the judge (RCW 9.94A.505(2)(b); RCW
tion on this website may not be current (Frase 9.94A.535). In some cases, no SRA score was
2005). recorded, but the offense type was classified by
5. Some scholars interpret these findings as evidence the SRA. In such cases, we imputed the SRA score
that decisions to punish are guided by judges’ focal associated with that charge in other cases (n = 88).
concerns. According to this argument, court actors 12. The vast majority (98.7 percent) of defendants in
must make decisions quickly, yet they possess min- our sample received at least some confinement
imal amounts of information and time. As a result, time. We tested confinement length, measured in
their adjudication and sentencing decisions are months, in our early models. When included, we
guided by assessments of defendants’ blameworthi- added 1 to all values (44 cases were sentenced to
ness and dangerousness (Albonetti 1987, 1991, zero months) and logged the variable to normalize
1997; Emerson 1969; Johnson 2006; Kurlychek its distribution. However, because the sentencing
and Johnson 2004; Savelsberg 1992; Steffensmeier, guidelines that determine confinement length are
Ulmer, and Kramer 1998; Ulmer and Bradley governed by SRA score and offender score, con-
2006). We agree that judicial decisions are shaped finement length is highly correlated with both (.5
by judges’ focal concerns and that these concerns with SRA and .7 with offender score.) When
may be influenced by local environments, but the substituted with SRA or offender score, months
conceptual notion of focal concerns does not shed had a small but positive association with fine and
light on why certain extra-legal variables, such as fee assessment. To avoid problems associated with
race, are central to perceptions of blameworthiness. multicollinearity, we dropped months from the final
6. Between 1980 and 2007, the proportion of the analyses.
Washington State population that identified as His- 13. Correlation between violent offense and SRA is
panic rose from 2.9 to 9.4 percent. By contrast, the .459. To our surprise, certain drug offenses have
black population grew much more slowly, from 2.6 higher SRA scores than certain violent crimes. For
percent in 1980 to 3.6 percent in 2007 (U.S. Census example, manufacturing methamphetamine (10)
n.d.). and the delivery or possession with intent to distrib-
7. Alternatively, we could use a fixed effects model to ute methamphetamine (8) have higher SRA scores
account for the specific effects of the 39 counties. than do violent offenses such as child molestation
However, this would preclude analysis of the in the second degree (7), rape in the third degree
cross-level interactions that are central to our (5), and kidnapping in the second degree (5). Vari-
hypotheses. ation in SRA is thus not directly correlated to our
8. We found that when removing just under half of the offense categories (i.e., violent, drug, and other).
counties, each one at a time, political orientation 14. We tested models with number of superior and
frequently shifted from significant to nonsignificant other court convictions (with and without SRA).
or marginally significant, although the direction and Results show that neither of these measures has
relative size of the coefficient remained unchanged. a significant impact on the outcome or other inde-
The shift does not appear to stem from the number pendent variables. We therefore do not include prior
of observations in these counties, as it occurred convictions in the models presented here.
when removing very small (n = 7) and large (n = 15. Treating race and ethnicity as analytic categories is,
536) counties. This variable’s wavering significance of course, problematic. Race and ethnicity are social
does not surprise us, as political orientation is not constructs that were created to manipulate, control,
a consistently significant predictor in our models. and conquer populations (Almaguer 1994; Omi and
26 American Sociological Review XX(X)

Winant 1986). On the other hand, the consequences financial penalties than those who plead guilty is
of racial and ethnic ideologies become tangible debatable. In studies of confinement sentencing, evi-
when reified through governments, institutions, dence of a trial penalty is often seen as indicative of
and economic structures (Marx 1998; Takaki the court’s effort to enhance organizational effi-
1990). Thus, beliefs about race and ethnicity have ciency by avoiding lengthy and costly trials (Dixon
very real consequences over time, although they 1995; Engen and Steen 2000). This interpretation
are also fluid and subject to change (Omi and makes sense in the context of confinement sentences:
Winant 1986). We treat race and ethnicity as social reduced sentences are offered as an incentive to
constructs and identities that carry real social plead guilty; defendants who agree to a plea are
consequences. rewarded with less prison time, while those who go
16. It is possible that applying this methodology led to to trial are not. In the context of monetary sanctions,
the misidentification of some (mainly white) the theoretical significance of a trial penalty is less
defendants as Hispanic. It is also possible that clear. In Washington State, monetary sanctions are
some Hispanics were unidentified as such, as imposed after adjudication at the sentencing hearing.
many Hispanics have surnames that are not on the For this reason, assessing defendants who go to trial
list generated by the Census Bureau. However, by greater LFOs provides no incentive for defendants to
classifying only individuals with surnames consid- plead guilty. It therefore does not appear that a finan-
ered to be ‘‘heavily Hispanic,’’ we presumably err cial trial penalty in this context is evidence of con-
on the side of undercounting Hispanics. cerns about organizational efficiency.
17. We group Asians and whites together because stud- 23. See note 6.
ies have not found that either group is overrepre- 24. U.S. Census data for 2006 from the American Com-
sented in the criminal justice system or receives munity Survey indicate that 23.9 percent of His-
comparatively severe penalties. We do recognize panics, compared with 9.5 percent of non-Hispanic
that the Asian category comprises multiple ethnic whites, had incomes that fell below the poverty line
groups and that these groups have varying outcomes in the previous 12 months (see U.S. Census Bureau
and experiences. American Community Survey 2006, Tables
18. Our hypotheses drove our decision not to center B17020H and B170201). Formerly incarcerated
variables (see Snijders and Bosker 1999). Cross- white males have slightly greater earnings than for-
level interactions comprised of levels one and two merly incarcerated black or Hispanic males (West-
variables include dichotomous variables (e.g., ern 2006).
black/nonblack) and proportions, such as percent
of the county population that is Latino. Our theoret-
ical framework predicts that it is the relative size of References
the population of color in a particular county that
triggers a perception of threat, not the size of the Adamson, Christopher R. 1983. ‘‘Punishment after Slav-
population of color relative to populations of color ery: Southern State Penal Systems, 1865–1890.’’
in other counties. Thus, the true value of the propor- Social Problems 30:555–69.
tion, rather than deviation from the county average Akaike, H. 1974. ‘‘A New Look at the Statistical Identi-
(or grand mean), allows a more meaningful inter- fication.’’ IEEE Trans. Automat Control 19:716–23.
pretation of our results. Albonetti, Celesta A. 1987. ‘‘Prosecutorial Discretion:
19. Adults with dependent children are more likely to The Effects of Uncertainty.’’ Law and Society
receive leniency in sentencing; other researchers Review 21:291–313.
suggest that this pattern largely accounts for gender Albonetti, Celesta A. 1991. ‘‘An Integration of Theories
differences in sentencing (Daly 1987; Daly and to Explain Judicial Discretion.’’ Social Problems
Tonry 1997; Griffin and Wooldredge 2006; 38:247–66.
Koons-Witt 2002). Albonetti, Celesta A. 1997. ‘‘Sentencing under the Fed-
20. Interaction coefficients can be interpreted as b’= b1 eral Sentencing Guidelines: Effects of Defendant
1 b3X2. Because the dependent variable is logged, Characteristics, Guilty Pleas, and Departures on Sen-
b’ should be interpreted as a percent change in fines tencing Outcomes for Drug Offenses, 1991–1992.’’
and fees, not as a dollar amount. Law and Society Review 31:789–822.
21. For reasons described in note 8, we ran numerous Almaguer, Tomas. 1994. Racial Fault Lines: The Histor-
models with varied constellations of variables. ical Origins of White Supremacy in California. Los
Unlike the other significant variables discussed Angeles: University of California Press.
here, the political orientation result is significant Anderson, Helen. 2009. ‘‘Penalizing Poverty: Making
roughly half of the time. Indigent Criminal Defendants Pay for their Court-
22. The theoretical significance of our finding that Appointed Counsel through Recoupment and Contri-
defendants convicted at trial receive greater bution.’’ 42 U. Mich. J.L. 323.
Harris et al. 27

Beckett, Katherine. 1997. Making Crime Pay: Law and Chiricos, Ted and Sarah Eschholz. 2002. ‘‘The Racial
Order in Contemporary American Politics. New and Ethnic Typification of Crime and the Criminal
York: Oxford University Press. Typification of Race and Ethnicity in Local Televi-
Beckett, Katherine, Alexes Harris, and Heather Evans. sion News.’’ Journal of Research in Crime and
2008. The Assessment and Consequences of Legal Delinquency 39:400–420.
Financial Obligations in Washington State. Olym- Chiricos, Ted, Ranee McEntine, and Marc Gertz. 2001.
pia, WA (http://www.courts.wa.gov/committee/pdf/ ‘‘Perceived Racial and Ethnic Composition of
2008LFO_report.pdf). Neighborhood and Perceived Risk of Crime.’’ Social
Beckett, Katherine and Theodore Sasson. 1998. ‘‘The Problems 48:322–40.
Media and the Construction of the Drug Crisis in Clear, Todd R. 2007. Imprisoning Communities: How
America.’’ Pp. 25–44 in The New War on Drugs: Mass Incarceration Makes Disadvantaged Neigh-
Symbolic Politics and Criminal Justice Policy, edi- borhoods Worse. New York: Oxford University
ted by E. Jensen and J. Gerber. Cincinnati, OH: Press.
Anderson Publishers. Clear, Todd R., Dina R. Rose, and Judith Ryder. 2001.
Beckett, Katherine and Bruce Western. 2001. ‘‘Govern- ‘‘Incarceration and the Community: The Problem
ing Social Marginality: Welfare, Incarceration and of Removing and Returning Offenders.’’ Crime
the Transformation of State Policy.’’ Punishment & and Delinquency 47:335–51.
Society 3:43–59. Comfort, Megan. 2007. ‘‘Punishment beyond the Legal
Blackmon, Douglas A. 2008. Slavery by Another Name: Offender.’’ Annual Review of Law in the Social Sci-
The Re-Enslavement of Black Americans from the ences 3:271–96.
Civil War to World War II. New York: Doubleday. Correll, Sherry, Stephen Benard, and In Paik. 2007.
Blalock, Hubert M. 1967. Toward a Theory of Minority ‘‘Getting a Job: Is there a Motherhood Penalty?’’
Group Relations. New York: Wiley. American Journal of Sociology 112:1297–1338.
Blumer, Herbert. 1958. ‘‘Race Prejudice as a Sense of Daly, Kathleen. 1987. ‘‘Structure and Practice of Familial-
Group Position.’’ Pacific Sociological Review 1:3–7. Based Justice in a Criminal Court.’’ Law and Society
Bobo, Lawrence D. and Devon Johnson. 2004. ‘‘A Taste Review 21:267–90.
for Punishment: Black and White Americans’ Views Daly, Kathleen and Michael Tonry. 1997. ‘‘Gender,
on the Death Penalty and the War on Drugs.’’ Du Race, and Sentencing.’’ Pp. 201–252 in Crime and
Bois Review 1:151–80. Justice: A Review of the Research, edited by M.
Bontrager, Stephanie, William Bales, and Ted Chiricos. Tonry. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
2005. ‘‘Race, Ethnicity, Threat and the Labeling of Delgado, Richard and Jean Stefancic. 1998. ‘‘Racial
Convicted Felons.’’ Criminology 43:589–622. Depiction in American Law and Culture.’’
Braman, Donald. 2002. ‘‘The Impact of Incarceration on Pp. 209–214 in The Latino/a Condition: A Critical
Families.’’ Pp. 117–35 in Invisible Punishment: The Reader, edited by R. Delgado and J. Stefancic.
Collateral Consequences of Mass Imprisonment, edi- New York: New York University Press.
ted by M. Mauer and M. Chesney-Lind. New York: Demuth, Stephen. 2003. ‘‘Racial and Ethnic Differences
The New Press. in Pretrial Release Decisions and Outcomes: A Com-
Bridges, George S. and Robert D. Crutchfield. 1988. parison of Hispanic, Black, and White Felony
‘‘Law, Social Standing and Racial Disparities in Arrestees.’’ Criminology 41:873–908.
Imprisonment.’’ Social Forces 66:699–724. Devine, Patricia G. and Andrew J. Elliot. 1995. ‘‘Are
Bridges, George S., Robert D. Crutchfield, and Edith E. Racial Stereotypes Really Fading? The Princeton
Simpson. 1987. ‘‘Crime, Social Structure and Crim- Trilogy Revisited.’’ Personality and Social Psychol-
inal Punishment: White and Nonwhite Rates of ogy Bulletin 11:1139–50.
Imprisonment.’’ Social Problems 34:345–61. Dixon, Jo. 1995. ‘‘The Organizational Context of Crim-
Bridges, George and Sara Steen. 1998. ‘‘Racial Dispar- inal Sentencing.’’ American Journal of Sociology
ities in Official Assessments of Juvenile Offenders: 100:1157–98.
Attributional Stereotypes as Mediating Mecha- Durkheim, Emile. 1984. The Division of Labor in Soci-
nisms.’’ American Sociological Review 63:554–70. ety. New York: The Free Press.
Britt, Chester L. 2000. ‘‘Social Context and Racial Dis- Eberhardt, Jennifer L. and Phillip A. Goff. 2005. ‘‘See-
parities in Punishment Decisions.’’ Justice Quarterly ing Race.’’ Pp. 219–31 in Social Psychology of Prej-
17:707–32. udice: Historical and Contemporary Issues, edited
Bureau of Justice Assistance. 1996. How to Use Struc- by C. S. Crandall and M. Schaller. Lawrence, KS:
tured Fines (Day Fines) as an Intermediate Sanction. Lewinian Press.
Washington, DC: Vera Institute of Justice. Eberhardt, Jennifer L., Phillip A. Goff, Valerie J. Purdie,
Chavez, Leo R. 2008. The Latino Threat: Constructing and Paul G. Davies. 2004. ‘‘Seeing Black: Race,
Immigrants, Citizens, and the Nation. Palo Alto: Crime, and Visual Processing.’’ Journal of Personal-
Stanford University Press. ity and Social Psychology 87:876–963.
28 American Sociological Review XX(X)

Edsall, Thomas B. and Mary D. Edsall. 1991. Chain Attitudes towards Punitive and Preventive Crime
Reaction: The Impact of Race, Rights, and Taxes Policies.’’ Law and Human Behavior 30:435–54.
on American Politics. New York: W. W. Norton & Gregory, James. 1995. ‘‘The Crime of Punishment:
Company. Racial and Gender Disparities in the Use of Corporal
Eitle, David and John Taylor. 2008. ‘‘Are Hispanics the Punishment in U.S. Public Schools.’’ Journal of
New ‘Threat’? Minority Group Threat and Fear of Negro Education 64:454–62.
Crime in Miami-Dade County.’’ Social Science Griffin, Timothy and John Wooldredge. 2006. ‘‘Sex-
Research 37:1102–15. Based Disparities in Felony Dispositions Before ver-
Ellsworth, T. and R. A. Weisheit. 1997. ‘‘Supervision sus After Sentencing Reform in Ohio.’’ Criminology
and Treatment of Offenders on Probation: Under- 44:893–923.
standing Rural and Urban Differences.’’ Prison Hagan, John and Alberto Palloni. 1999. ‘‘Sociological
Journal 77:209. Criminology and the Mythology of Hispanic Immi-
Emerson, Robert. 1969. Judging Delinquents: Context gration and Crime.’’ Social Problems 46:617–32.
and Process in Juvenile Court. Chicago: Aldine. Haney, Craig. 2006. Reforming Punishment: Psycholog-
Engen, Rodney and Randy Gainey. 2000. ‘‘Modeling the ical Limits to the Pains of Imprisonment. Washing-
Effects of Legally Relevant and Extralegal Factors ton, DC: American Psychological Association
under Sentencing Guidelines: The Rules Have Books.
Changed.’’ Criminology 38:1207–30. Harris, Alexes. 2007. ‘‘Diverting and Abdicating Judi-
Engen, Rodney, Randy Gainey, Robert Crutchfield, and cial Discretion: Cultural, Political and Procedural
Joseph Weis. 2003. ‘‘Discretion and Disparity under Dynamics in California Juvenile Justice.’’ Law &
Sentencing Guidelines: The Role of Departures and Society Review 41:387–428.
Structured Sentencing Alternatives.’’ Criminology Harris, Alexes. 2008. ‘‘The Social Construction of
41:99–130. ‘Sophisticated’ Adolescents: How Judges Integrate
Engen, Rodney and Sara Steen. 2000. ‘‘The Power to Juvenile and Criminal Justice Decision-Making
Punish: Discretion and Sentencing Reform in the Models.’’ Journal of Contemporary Ethnography
War on Drugs.’’ American Journal of Sociology 37:469–506.
105:1357–95. Harris, Alexes, Heather Evans, and Katherine Beckett.
Feldman, Stanley and Leonie Huddy. 2005. ‘‘Racial 2010. ‘‘Drawing Blood from Stones: Monetary Sanc-
Resentment and White Opposition to Race- tions, Punishment and Inequality in the Contempo-
Conscious Programs: Principles or Prejudice.’’ rary United States.’’ American Journal of Sociology
American Journal of Political Science 49:168–83. 115:1753–99.
Foster, Holly and John Hagan. 2007. ‘‘Incarceration and Harris, Lasana T. and Susan T. Fiske. 2006. ‘‘Dehuman-
Intergenerational Social Exclusion.’’ Social Prob- izing the Lowest of the Low: Neuro-Imaging
lems 54:399–433. Responses to Extreme Outgroups.’’ Psychological
Frase, Richard. 2005. ‘‘State Sentencing Guidelines: Science 1:847–53.
Diversity, Consensus, and Unresolved Policy Hay, Douglas. 1975. ‘‘Property, Authority and Criminal
Issues.’’ Columbia Law Review 105:1190–92. Law.’’ Pp. 17–63 in Albion’s Fatal Tree: Crime and
Frohmann, Lisa. 1991. ‘‘Discrediting Victims’ Allega- Society in Eighteenth-Century England, edited by D.
tions of Sexual Assault: Prosecutorial Accounts of Hay, P. Linebaugh, J. G. Rule, E. P. Thompson, and
Case Rejection.’’ Social Problems 38:213–26. C. Winslow. New York: Pantheon Books.
Frohmann, Lisa. 1997. ‘‘Convictability and Discordant Hillsman, Sally and Judith A. Greene. 1992. ‘‘The Use
Locales: Reproducing Race, Class, and Gender of Fines as an Intermediate Sanction.’’ Pp. 123–41
Ideologies in Prosecutorial Decision-Making.’’ Law in Smart Sentencing: The Emergence of Intermediate
and Society Review 31:531–56. Sanctions, edited by J. M. Byrne, A. J. Lurigio, and
Garfinkel, Harold. 1956. ‘‘Conditions of Successful J. Petersilia. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Degradation Ceremonies.’’ American Journal of Hurwitz, Jon and Mark Pefley. 1997. ‘‘Public Percep-
Sociology 6:420–24. tions of Race and Crime: The Role of Racial Stereo-
Garland, David. 1990. Punishment and Modern Society. types.’’ American Journal of Political Science
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 41:375–401.
Goffman, Erving. 1956. ‘‘Embarrassment and Social Jacobs, David and Jason Carmichael. 2001. ‘‘The Poli-
Organization.’’ American Journal of Sociology tics of Punishment across Time and Space: A Pooled
62:264–71. Time-Series Analysis of Imprisonment Rates.’’
Goffman, Erving. 1963. Stigma: Notes on the Manage- Social Forces 80:61–91.
ment of Spoiled Identity. New York: Simon and Johnson, Brian. 2005. ‘‘Contextual Disparities in Guide-
Schuster. lines Departures: Courtroom Social Contexts, Guide-
Green, Eva G. T., Christian Staerkle, and David O. lines Compliance, and Extralegal Disparities in
Sears. 2006. ‘‘Symbolic Racism and Whites’ Criminal Sentencing.’’ Criminology 43:761–96.
Harris et al. 29

Johnson, Brian. 2006. ‘‘The Multilevel Context of Crim- Massoglia, Michael and Jason Schnittker. 2009. ‘‘No
inal Sentencing: Integrating Judge- and County- Real Release: The Health Effects of Incarceration.’’
Level Influences.’’ Criminology 44:259–98. Contexts 8:38–42.
Johnson, Brian, Jeffery Ulmer, and John Kramer. 2008. McLanahan, Sara. 2009. ‘‘Fragile Families and the
‘‘The Social Context of Guidelines Circumvention: Reproduction of Poverty.’’ Annals of the American
The Case of Federal District Courts.’’ Criminology Academy of Political and Social Sciences 621:
46:737–83. 111–31.
Johnson, Rucker C. and Steven Raphael. 2006. ‘‘The McLean, Rachel L. and Michael D. Thompson. 2007.
Effects of Male Incarceration Rates on AIDS Infec- Repaying Debts. New York: Council of State Gov-
tion Rates among African American Women and ernments Justice Center.
Men.’’ National Poverty Center Working Paper Mead, George Herbert. 1918. ‘‘The Psychology of Puni-
Series #06-22, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor. tive Justice.’’ American Journal of Sociology
Jones, Christopher S. and Martin F. Kaplan. 2003. ‘‘The 23:577–602.
Effects of Racially Stereotypical Crimes on Juror Merry, Sally Engle. 2000. Colonizing Hawai’i: The Cul-
Decision-Making and Information-Processing Strate- tural Power of Law. Princeton: Princeton University
gies.’’ Basic and Applied Social Psychology 25: Press.
1–13. Miethe, Terrance D. 1987. ‘‘Charging and Plea Bargain-
King, Ryan. 2007. ‘‘Group Threat and Social Control: ing under Determinate Sentencing: An Investigation
Race, Perceptions of Minorities and the Desire to of the Hydraulic Displacement of Discretion.’’ Jour-
Punish.’’ Social Forces 85:1254–80. nal of Criminal Law and Criminology 78:155–76.
Koons-Witt, Barbara. 2002. ‘‘The Effect of Gender on Miethe, Terance D. and Hong Lu. 2005. Punishment: A
the Decision to Incarcerate Before and After the Comparative Historical Perspective. New York:
Introduction of Sentencing Guidelines.’’ Criminol- Cambridge University Press.
ogy 40:297–328. Miller, William Ian. 1997. The Anatomy of Disgust.
Kramer, John and Darrell Steffensmeier. 1993. ‘‘Race Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
and Imprisonment Decisions.’’ Sociological Quar- Moore, Charles and Terrance Miethe. 1986. ‘‘Regulated
terly 34:357–76. and Unregulated Sentencing Decisions: An Analysis
Kurlychek, Megan and Brian D. Johnson. 2004. ‘‘The of First-Year Practices under Minnesota’s Felony
Juvenile Penalty: A Comparison of Sentencing Out- Sentencing Guidelines.’’ Law and Society Review
comes for Transferred Juveniles and Young Adult 20:253–78.
Offenders.’’ Criminology 42:485–517. Morris, Edward. 2005. ‘‘‘Tuck in That Shirt!’ Race,
Levingston, Kirsten D. 2008. ‘‘Making the Bad Guy Class, Gender, and Discipline in an Urban School.’’
Pay: Growing Use of Cost Shifting as an Economic Sociological Perspectives 48:25–48.
Sanction.’’ Prison Legal News 19:4. Mosher, Clay. 2001. ‘‘Predicting Drug Arrest Rates:
Liska, Allen E., Joseph J. Lawrence, and Andrew Conflict and Social Disorganization Perspectives.’’
Sanchirico. 1982. ‘‘Fear of Crime as a Social Crime & Delinquency 47:84–104.
Fact.’’ Social Forces 60:760–70. Mullaney, Fahy G. 1988. Economic Sanctions in Com-
Steven Lukes, and Andrew Scull, eds. 1983. Durkheim munity Corrections. Washington, DC: U.S. Depart-
and the Law. New York: St. Martin’s Press. ment of Justice, National Institute of Corrections.
Lyons, William and Julie Drew. 2006. Punishing Myers, Martha A. and Suzette Talarico. 1986. ‘‘The
Schools: Fear and Citizenship in American Educa- Social Contexts of Racial Discrimination in Sentenc-
tion. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. ing.’’ Social Problems 33:236–51.
Mann, Coramae and Marjorie Zatz. 1998. Images of Nagel, Ilene H. and Stephen J. Schulhofer. 1992. ‘‘A
Color: Images of Crime. Los Angeles: Roxbury. Tale of Three Cities: An Empirical Study of Charg-
Manza, Jeff and Christopher Uggen. 2006. Locked Out: ing and Bargaining Practices under the Federal Sen-
Felon Disenfranchisement and American Democ- tencing Guidelines.’’ Southern California Law
racy. New York: Oxford University Press. Review 66:501–66.
Martinez, Ramiro, Jr. 1996. ‘‘Latinos and Lethal Vio- Olson, David E. and Gerard F. Ramker. 2001. ‘‘Crime
lence: The Impact of Poverty and Inequality.’’ Social Does Not Pay, but Criminals May: Factors Influenc-
Problems 43:131–46. ing the Imposition and Collection of Probation
Marx, Anthony W. 1998. Making Race and Nation: A Fees.’’ Justice System Journal 22:29–46.
Comparison of the United States, South Africa, and O’Malley, Pat. 2009. ‘‘Theorizing Fines.’’ Punishment
Brazil. New York: Cambridge University Press. and Society 11:67–83.
Massoglia, Michael. 2008. ‘‘Incarceration, Health and Omi, Michael and Howard Winant. 1986. Racial Forma-
Racial Disparities in Health.’’ Law & Society Review tion in the United States: From the 1960s to the
42:275–306. 1990s. New York: Routledge.
30 American Sociological Review XX(X)

Oshinsky, David M. 1996. Worse than Slavery: Parch- Schlesinger, Traci. 2005. ‘‘Racial and Ethnic Disparity
man Farm and the Ordeal of Jim Crow Justice. in Pretrial Criminal Processing.’’ Justice Quarterly
New York: Simon and Schuster. 22:170–92.
Pager, Devah. 2003. ‘‘The Mark of a Criminal Record.’’ Schram, Sanford F., Joe Soss, Richard C. Fording, and
American Journal of Sociology 108:937–75. Linda Houser. 2009. ‘‘Deciding to Discipline:
Pager, Devah. 2005. ‘‘Walking the Talk: What Employ- Race, Choice, and Punishment at the Frontlines of
ers Say Versus What They Do.’’ American Sociolog- Welfare Reform.’’ American Sociological Review
ical Review 70:355–80. 74:398–442.
Pager, Devah. 2007. Marked: Race, Crime and Finding Sears, David O. 1988. ‘‘Symbolic Racism.’’ Pp.53–84 in
Work in an Era of Mass Incarceration. Chicago: Eliminating Racism: Profiles in Controversy, edited
University of Chicago Press. by P. A. Katz and D. A. Taylor. New York: Plenum
Pager, Devah and Diana Karafin. 2009. ‘‘Bayesian Press.
Bigot? Statistical Discrimination, Stereotypes, and Simon, Jonathan. 2007. Governing through Crime: How
Employer Decision-Making.’’ Annals of the Ameri- the War on Crime Transformed American Democ-
can Academy of Political and Social Sciences racy and Created a Culture of Fear. New York:
621:70–93. Oxford University Press.
Perkins, Colby R. 1993. ‘‘Evaluating the Passel-Word Snijders, Tom and Roel Bosker. 1999. Multilevel Analy-
Spanish Surname List: 1990 Decennial Census Post sis: An Introduction to Basic and Advanced Multi-
Enumeration Survey Results.’’ Population Division, level Modeling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Working Paper No. 4. U.S. Bureau of the Census. Soss, Joe, Richard C. Fording, and Sanford F. Schram.
Perkinson, Robert. 2008. Texas Tough: The Rise of 2008. ‘‘The Color of Devolution: The Politics of
a Prison Empire. New York: Metropolitan Books/ Local Punishment in an Era of Neo-Liberal Welfare
Henry Holt. Reform.’’ American Journal of Political Science
Quillian, Lincoln. 2008. ‘‘Does Unconscious Racism 52:536–53.
Exist.’’ Social Psychology Quarterly 71:6–11. Spohn, Cassia and David Holleran. 2000. ‘‘The Impris-
Quillian, Lincoln and Devah Pager. 2001. ‘‘Black onment Penalty Paid by Young, Unemployed Black
Neighbors, Higher Crime? The Role of Racial Ster- and Hispanic Male Offenders.’’ Criminology
eotypes in Evaluations of Neighborhood Crime.’’ 38:281–306.
American Journal of Sociology 107:717–67. Steen, Sara, Rodney Engen, and Randy Gainey. 2005.
Raudenbush, Stephen and Anthony Bryk. 2002. Hierar- ‘‘Images of Danger and Culpability: Racial Stereo-
chical Linear Models. Newbury Park, CA: Sage typic, Case Processing, and Criminal Sentencing.’’
Publications. Criminology 4:435–68.
Reeves, Mimmie and Richard Campbell. 1994. Cracked Steffensmeier, Darrell and Stephen Demuth. 2000. ‘‘Eth-
Coverage: Television News, the Anti-Cocaine nicity and Sentencing Outcomes in U.S. Federal
Crusade and the Reagan Legacy. New York: Courts: Who is Punished More Harshly?’’ American
Grisham. Sociological Review 65:705–729.
Reinarman, Craig and Harry G. Levine. 1997. Crack in Steffensmeier, Darrell and Stephen Demuth. 2001. ‘‘Eth-
America: Demon Drugs and Social Justice. Berke- nicity and Judges’ Sentencing Decisions: Hispanic-
ley: University of California Press. Black-White Comparisons.’’ Criminology 39:145–78.
Rosenthal, Alan and Marsha Weissman. 2007. Sentenc- Steffensmeier, Darrell, Jeffery Ulmer, and John Kramer.
ing for Dollars: The Financial Consequences of 1998. ‘‘The Interaction of Race, Gender, and Age in
a Criminal Conviction. Syracuse, NY: Center for Criminal Sentencing: The Punishment Cost of Being
Community Alternatives, Justice Strategies. Young, Black, and Male.’’ Criminology 36:763–98.
Ruback, R. Barry, Jennifer N. Shaffer, and Melissa A. Sykes, Bryan L. and Alex Piquero. 2009. ‘‘Structuring
Logue. 2004. ‘‘The Imposition and Effects of Resti- and Recreating Inequality: Health Testing Policies,
tution in Four Pennsylvania Counties: Effects of Size Race, and the Criminal Justice System.’’ Annals of
of County and Specialized Collection Units.’’ Crime the American Academy of Political and Social Sci-
& Delinquency 50:168–88. ence 623:214–27.
Sampson, Robert J. and Stephen W. Raudenbush. 2004. Takaki, Ronald. 1990. Iron Cages: Race and Culture in
‘‘Seeing Disorder: Neighborhood Stigma and the 19th Century America. New York: Oxford Univer-
Social Construction of ‘Broken Windows.’’’ Social sity Press.
Psychology Quarterly 67:319–42. Tonry, Michael. 1996. Sentencing Matters. New York:
Savelsberg, Joachim. 1992. ‘‘Law That Does Not Fit Oxford University Press.
Society: Sentencing Guidelines as a Neoclassical Tonry, Michael. 1998. ‘‘Intermediate Sanctions.’’ In The
Reaction to the Dilemmas of Substantivized Law.’’ Handbook of Crime and Punishment, edited by M.
American Journal of Sociology 97:1346–81. Tonry. New York: Oxford University Press.
Harris et al. 31

Tonry, Michael and Mary Lynch. 1996. ‘‘Intermediate Crime, edited by G. L. Fox. London: Oxford Univer-
Sanctions.’’ Crime and Justice 20:99–144. sity Press.
Travis, Jeremy. 2005. But they All Come Back: Facing Western, Bruce and Becky Pettit. 2005. ‘‘Black-White
the Challenges of Prisoner Reentry. New York: Wage Inequality, Employment Rates, and Incar-
Urban Institute Press. ceration.’’ American Journal of Sociology 111:
Uggen, Christopher, Jeff Manza, and Melissa Thompson. 553–78.
2006. ‘‘Citizenship, Democracy and the Civic Reinte- Wooldredge, John and Amy Thistlethwaite. 2004. ‘‘Bile-
gration of Criminal Offenders.’’ Annals of the vel Disparities in Court Dispositions for Intimate
American Association of Political Science Studies Assault.’’ Criminology 42:417–56.
605:281–310. Word, David L. and R. Colby Perkins Jr. 1996. ‘‘Build-
Ulmer, Jeffery and Mindy Bradley. 2006. ‘‘Variation in ing a Spanish Surname List for the 1990s (A New
Trial Penalties among Serious Violent Offenses.’’ Approach to an Old Problem).’’ Technical Working
Criminology 44:631–70. Paper No.13. Washington, DC: Population Division,
Ulmer, Jeffery and John Kramer. 1996. ‘‘Court Commu- U.S. Census Bureau.
nities under Sentencing Guidelines: Dilemmas of Zatz, Marjorie S. 1985. ‘‘Los Cholos: Legal Processing
Formal Rationality and Sentencing Disparity.’’ of Chicano Gang Members.’’ Social Problems
Criminology 34:383–407. 33:13–30.
U.S. Census Bureau. American Community Survey,
2006. Tables B17020H and B170201 (http://
www.census.gov/acs/www/).
U.S. Drug Enforcement Administration. 2009. Drug Alexes Harris is an Assistant Professor in the Depart-
Trends Washington (http://www.usnodrugs.com/ ment of Sociology at the University of Washington.
drugtrends.htm?state=Washington). Her work concentrates in the area of social stratification
U.S. Department of Justice, Bureau of Justice Statistics. and inequality, with specific attention to factors that
N.d. Survey of Inmates in State and Federal Correc- influence institutional decision-making and the conse-
tional Facilities. Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-university quences of juvenile and criminal justice involvement.
Consortium for Political and Social Research [pro- She has published articles in Law and Society Review,
ducer and distributor], 2007-02-28. ICPSR Nos. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, Race and Soci-
6068; 2598; 4572. DOI: (http://dx.doi.org/10.3886/ ety, and American Journal of Sociology.
ICPSR06068); (http://dx.doi.org/10.3886/ICPSR0
2598); (http://dx.doi.org/10.3886/ICPSR04572). Heather Evans is a graduate student in the Department
Warr, Mark. 2000. ‘‘Fear of Crime in the United States: of Sociology and a Fellow in the Comparative Law and
Avenues for Research and Policy.’’ Pp. 451–89 in Society Studies program at the University of Washing-
Criminal Justice 2000, Vol. 4, Measurement and ton. Her research employs mixed methodologies, includ-
Analysis of Crime and Justice, edited by D. Duffee. ing ethnographic fieldwork and statistical analysis.
Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice, Broadly, she is interested in the ways in which institu-
National Institute of Justice. tions reproduce social inequality, how new social spaces
Weber, Max. 1968. Economy and Society. Berkeley: are created, and perceptions of citizenship among mar-
University of California Press. ginalized people.
Western, Bruce. 2006. Punishment and Inequality in
America. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. Katherine Beckett is a Professor in the Department of
Western, Bruce and Katherine Beckett. 1999. ‘‘How Sociology and Law, Societies & Justice Program at the
Unregulated is the U.S. Labor Market? Penal System University of Washington. She has published numerous
as Labor Market Institution, 1980–1995.’’ American articles and book chapters on the politics of crime and
Journal of Sociology 104:1030–60. punishment, and is the author of three books. The
Western, Bruce and Sara McLanahan. 2000. ‘‘Fathers in most recent of these, co-authored with Steve Herbert,
Fragile Families: The Impact of Incarceration on is titled Banished: The New Social Control in Urban
Family Formation.’’ Pp. 309–324 in Families and America (Oxford University Press, 2010).

Вам также может понравиться