Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 27

Motivation in action: A process model of L2 motivation1

Zoltán Dörnyei and István Ottó

As part of a long-term project aimed at conducted on what motivation is—the em-


designing classroom interventions to phasis in both theoretical and empirical
motivate language learners, we have work on motivation has traditionally been
searched for a motivation model that placed on identifying various influential
could serve as a theoretical basis for the motives and validating motivational theo-
methodological applications. We have ries. Consequently, as Good and Brophy
found that none of the existing models (1994) summarise, “motivation [in the
we considered were entirely adequate classroom] did not receive much scholarly
for our purpose for three reasons: (1) attention until recently, so that teachers were
they did not provide a sufficiently forced to rely on unsystematic ‘bag-of-
comprehensive and detailed summary of tricks’ approaches or on advice coming from
all the relevant motivational influences questionable theorizing” (p. 212). It must be
on classroom behaviour; (2) they tended noted that there have been a number of
to focus on how and why people choose exceptions to this generalisation (e.g.
certain courses of action, while ignoring Brophy, 1987, 1998; Burden, 1995; Good &
or playing down the importance of Brophy, 1994; Jones & Jones, 1995,
motivational sources of executing goal- McCombs, 1994; McCombs & Pope, 1994;
directed behaviour; and (3) they did not Pintrich & Schunk, 1996; Raffini, 1993,
do justice to the fact that motivation is 1996; and in the L2 field: Alison, 1993;
not static but dynamically evolving and Brown, 1994; Cranmer, 1996; Dörnyei,
changing in time, making it necessary 1994; Dörnyei & Csizér, 1998; Oxford &
for motivation constructs to contain a Shearin, 1994; Williams & Burden, 1997)
featured temporal axis. Consequently, and these studies on various aspects of
partly inspired by Heckhausen and motivating learners have constituted an
Kuhl’s ‘Action Control Theory’, we important starting point in our project.
have developed a new ‘Process Model In order to generate a systematic col-
of L2 Motivation’, which is intended lection of motivational strategies, we need a
both to account for the dynamics of solid motivational theory to serve as an
motivational change in time and to underlying organisational structure. Al-
synthesise many of the most important though, as is well known, there is no short-
motivational conceptualisations to date. age of competing motivational theories in
In this paper we describe the main social and motivational psychology, none of
components of this model, also listing a the models we have considered were entirely
number of its limitations which need to appropriate for our purpose for three main
be resolved in future research. reasons:

(1) They did not provide a sufficiently


Introduction comprehensive and detailed summary of
all the relevant motivational influences
on learner behaviour in the classroom.
The motivation model presented in this (2) Motivational theories typically focus on
paper has grown out of a research project how and why people choose certain
whose aim was to design motivational courses of action, rather than on the
strategies for the purpose of classroom motivational sources of executing goal-
intervention in second language (L2) edu- directed behaviour, whereas, as we will
cation. The rationale for the project lay in argue below, in educational contexts
the fact that the amount of psychological (and from the point of view of motiva-
research devoted to analysing how to moti- tional classroom interventions in par-
vate language learners has been rather ticular) the motivational influences on
meagre relative to the amount of research action implementation are more im-

Working Papers in Applied Linguistics, Thames Valley University, London, Vol. 4, pp. 43-69
© 1998 Zoltán Dörnyei & István Ottó
44 Working Papers in Applied Linguistics

portant than the directive function of sued by choice. Accordingly, key variables
motivation. in goal theories concern various goal prop-
(3) We felt that most motivational theories erties. The underlying principle of a third
did not do justice to the fact that moti- main direction in current motivation re-
vation is not a static state but rather a search, self-determination theory, and the
dynamically evolving and changing accompanying intrinsic vs. extrinsic moti-
entity, associated with an ongoing pro- vational paradigm, is that the desire to be
cess in time; thus, we intended to draw self-initiating and self-regulating is a pre-
up a motivation construct that had a requisite for any human behaviour to be
featured temporal axis. intrinsically rewarding, and therefore the
essence of motivated action is a sense of
autonomy. Finally, the key tenet in social
Comprehensive versus reductionist psychology is the assumption that it is
paradigms attitudes that exert a directive influence on
people’s behaviour since one’s attitude
towards a target influences the overall
The fact that motivation theories in general pattern of the person’s responses to the
tend not to offer very detailed and compre- target. It must also be noted that although
hensive taxonomies of the relevant motiva- these broad approaches dominate current
tional components is not at all surprising, thinking, in motivation research, there have
because the main objective of mainstream in the past been a number of other factors as
motivation research has been exactly the well that were at the time seen as central to
opposite. Since the study of motivation the understanding of human behaviour (e.g.
concerns the basic question of why humans inner forces such as instincts, volition, and
behave as they do, it follows that this issue psychical energy; stimulus and
is immensely complex and the number of reinforcement contingencies; basic human
potential determinants of human behaviour needs).
is extensive. A great deal of effort in moti- From the point of view of designing
vation research has, therefore, focused on motivational classroom interventions we
drawing up reductionist paradigms by trying need a particularly detailed and somewhat
to identify a relatively small number of key eclectic model that would list all the main
variables to explain a significant proportion motives that are likely to have an impact on
of the variance in people’s behaviour. learning achievement. Although some key
In order to reduce the number of rele- variables do indeed appear to stand out in
vant motivational components, various terms of their pervasive effect on learning
theories have selected certain motivational behaviour in general, the number of moti-
variables as principal components and then vational influences that are fundamental (in
proposed that these subsumed or mediated the sense that their absence can cancel or
the other interrelated factors. Expectancy- significantly weaken any other factors
value theories assume that motivation to whereas their active presence can boost
perform various tasks is the product of two action behaviour) is far more extensive than
key factors: the individual’s expectancy of each individual theory would suggest.
success in a given task and the value the Weiner’s (1984) conclusion summarises well
individual attaches to success in that task. our stance: “Any theory based on a single
Within this framework, we can find a variety concept, whether that concept is
of subtheories that attempt to explain the reinforcement, self-worth, optimal motiva-
cognitive processes that shape the indi- tion, or something else, will be insufficient
vidual’s expectancy: attribution theory to deal with the complexity of classroom
places the emphasis on how one processes activities” (p. 18).
past achievement experiences (successes or
failures); self-efficacy theory refers to ‘Choice’ versus ‘executive’ motivation
people’s judgement of their capabilities to
carry out certain specific tasks; and self- Another reason why existing motivation
worth theory claims that the highest human models were inadequate for designing mo-
priority is the need for self-acceptance and tivational classroom interventions is related
to maintain a positive face. to the target of our research: the study of a
Following somewhat different princi- foreign language. Schumann (1998) refers to
ples, goal theories propose that human L2 studies as “sustained deep learning” and
action is spurred by purpose, and for action argues that all such sustained learning
to take place, goals have to be set and pur- processes of skill/knowledge acquisition
Zoltán Dörnyei & István Ottó 45

(i.e. also applying to other areas such as the tion. In school environments, the key moti-
study of mathematics, bridge, celestial vational issues involve maintaining assigned
navigation, etc.) show different motivational goals, elaborating on subgoals, and
characteristics from short-term activities and exercising control over other thoughts and
simpler learning tasks. This is because in behaviours that are often more desirable
sustained learning contexts a major moti- than concentrating on academic work.
vational function is to maintain the moti- Therefore, in order to explain a significant
vational impetus for a considerable period proportion of the variability in learner
(often several years). In contrasting the persistence in classroom contexts, we need
motivational basis of “skill acquisition” with to focus on ‘executive motivation’, that is,
that of simpler activities that do not require consider motivational influences that operate
task learning because the goal is executed during task engagement, facilitating or
quickly, Kanfer (1996) presents a rather impeding goal-directed behaviour.
similar argument:

When goals can be accomplished with- The temporal organisation of motivation


out task learning, the influence of mo-
tivation on behaviour is often largely a
matter of choice. For example, the de- The third main concern of ours has been that
cision about which of two job offers to very few of the existing motivation theories
accept depends primarily on the individ- contain a temporal dimension, that is, they
ual’s evaluation of the costs and do not portray motivational processes as
benefits associated with each offer. they happen in time. Although some
Once a decision is made, however, the motivation theories have included certain
actions involved in implementing the time elements, these have typically focused
goal of accepting the job are straight- on broad issues such as past attributions or
forward. … However, this is not the future goals, rather than detailing sequences
case in skill acquisition. During skill or patterns of motivational events and
training, goal accomplishment proceeds components (cf. Karniol & Ross, 1996;
slowly, as the individual develops an Raynor & Roeder, 1987). We would assert,
understanding of the task and however, that in view of the fact that the
proficiency in skills relevant to per- mastery of most subject matters, particularly
formance. … Continued task practice a second language, usually takes several
(i.e. persistence) is necessary to yield months or years, the temporal axis of a
improvements in task performance. But motivational theory relevant to such
for practice to have a positive effect on sustained activities should be featured.
performance, additional motivational Indeed, even within the duration of a single
mechanisms are required to sustain course, most learners experience a regular
attention and effort over time and in the fluctuation of their enthusiasm/commitment,
face of difficulties and failures. (p. 405) often on a day-to-day basis. One basic
assumption underlying this paper is the
In other words, complex learning contexts belief that motivation is not so much a
reduce the role of the motivational influ- relatively constant state but rather a more
ences associated with the initial decision to dynamic entity that changes in time, with the
pursue the goal, and highlight the impor- level of effort invested in the pursuit of a
tance of motivational influences that affect particular goal oscillating between regular
action during goal implementation. Heck- ups and downs.
hausen (1991, p. 170) refers to this duality We must also note that the initial moti-
of motivational aspects as ‘choice motiva- vation to pursue an action does not simply
tion’ and ‘executive motivation’, and points arise from one moment to the other. Rather,
out that motivation research has traditionally motivation can be associated with a dynamic
restricted its focus to the first aspect, while mental process whereby the individual
including few motivational paradigms undergoes a number of stages such as initial
touching upon executive aspects. planning and goal-setting, intention-
Educational settings differ from many formation, the generation of concrete
achievement situations in that most of the subtasks to be taken, prioritising between
decisions and goals are not really the multiple tasks, the enactment of intentions,
learners’ own products but are imposed on and the evaluation of the outcomes. In other
them by the system, thus limiting the im- words, motivation from this perspective
portance of the ‘choice’ aspect of motiva- refers to a complex of decision-making,
46 Working Papers in Applied Linguistics

action-implementation and action- sequence until the goal is reached. This


controlling processes as well as the accom- second, implementional stage (‘executive
panying energy sources fuelling the action. motivation’, also termed the ‘volitional’
Although motivation theories in the past stage), involves motivational maintenance
have typically suggested a more static con- and control during the enactment of the
struct, one valuable exception has been a intention. The key issues to be examined
line of research associated with the work of here are the phenomena of action initiation,
German psychologists Julius Kuhl and Heinz perseverance, and overcoming internal
Heckhausen and their associates (e.g. obstacles to action.
Gollwitzer, 1990; Heckhausen 1991; Heck- Building on the above principles,
hausen & Kuhl, 1985; Kuhl, 1985, 1987, Heckhausen and Kuhl (1985) developed a
1992; Kuhl & Beckmann, 1994). Their more detailed ‘Theory of Action Control’,
model of motivational processes, often which was further elaborated on by Kuhl and
referred to as ‘Action Control Theory’, his associates (e.g. Kuhl, 1985, 1987, 1992;
emphasises the distinction of separate, and the studies in Kuhl & Beckmann, 1994).
temporally ordered action phases, intro- The theory attempts to explain the common
ducing a “temporal perspective that begins observation that people’s actual behaviour
with the awakening of a person’s wishes does not always correlate with the priorities
prior to goal setting and continues through set by their expectancy and value beliefs,
the evaluative thoughts entertained after and that even when the expectancies and
goal striving has ended” (Gollwitzer, 1990, values remain constant, the accompanying
p. 55). This approach has been very influ- motivational tendencies show a marked
ential on our thinking and, therefore, before waxing and waning. Furthermore, there is
presenting our Process Model, we begin with also the phenomenon that people sometimes
a brief summary of the main tenets of persist in pursuing an activity in spite of
Heckhausen and Kuhl’s theory. more attractive alternative goals.
The key component of Kuhl’s (1987)
action control model is ‘intention’, which is
Heckhausen and Kuhl’s theory of defined as an “activated plan to which the
volition actor has committed herself or himself”
(Kuhl, 1987, p. 282). In order for action to
take place, two memory systems need to be
The starting point in Heckhausen’s theory activated at the same time: motivation
(for an English summary, see Heckhausen, memory (which is content-independent, that
1991) is that research on motivation should is, when it is activated, it serves as a
be divided into two main camps, the study of continuous source of activation supporting
(a) how intentions are formed and (b) how any structure that is currently dominant in
they are implemented. As he argues, “Why other memory systems) and action memory
one wants to do something and that one (which contains behavioural programmes for
wants to do it is one thing, but its actual the performance of the particular act). An
implementation and successful completion is activated plan with support from the
another” (p. 163). He compares the motivation memory system becomes what
boundary between the intention-formation Kuhl (1987, p. 284) calls a “dynamic plan”,
process of the ‘predecisional phase’ and the which means that the executional process
implementation process of the has been instigated. From this point on the
‘postdecisional phase’ to a metaphorical motivation system carries out a new, chiefly
‘Rubicon’, which separates two distinct maintenance role, that is, it keeps sustaining
processes with different functional charac- (i.e. energising) the pursuit of the intention
teristics. The first, decision-making stage and also protecting it against the detrimental
(‘choice motivation’) has been the main effects of competing plans. Once the plan
focus of most mainstream psychological has been completed, the motivation system
theories of motivation in the past, with the is turned off. If the execution of the plan is
analyses centring around complex planning unsuccessful, an attempt is made to abandon
and goal-setting processes during which the plan.
initial wishes and desires are articulated and An important part of action control
evaluated in terms of their desirability and theory is the set of processes in charge of
chance of fulfilment. According to action maintenance, that is, the active use of
Heckhausen’s conceptualisation, a positive action control strategies to protect dynamic
evaluation results in an intention to act, (i.e. ongoing) behavioural intentions. Kuhl
which then guides the particular action (1987) describes six such self-regulatory
Zoltán Dörnyei & István Ottó 47

strategies: some people are more inclined toward one


orientation than towards the other.
(1) selective attention, that is, intentionally
ignoring attractive alternatives or
irrelevant aspects; The proposed Process Model of L2
(2) encoding control, that is, selectively Motivation
encoding only those features of a
stimulus that are related to the current
intention; Figure 1 presents the schematic represen-
(3) emotion control, that is, the active tation of our proposed Process Model of L2
inhibition of emotional states that may Motivation. As can be seen, the model
undermine the enacting and protection contains two dimensions: Action Sequence
of the intention, as well as the conscious and Motivational Influences. The first
generation of emotions that are dimension represents the behavioural proc-
conducive to the implementation of the ess whereby initial wishes, hopes, and de-
intention; sires are first transformed into goals, then
(4) motivation control, which is an active into intentions, leading eventually to action
process of changing the hierarchy of and, hopefully, to the accomplishment of the
tendency strengths when a more pow- goals, after which the process is submitted to
erful alternative arises, for example, by final evaluation. The second dimension of
focusing on what would happen if the the model, Motivational Influences, include
original intention failed and by keeping all the energy sources and motivational
in mind favourable expectancies or forces that underlie and fuel the behavioural
positive incentives; process. These will be detailed when
(4) environment control, that is, manipu- discussing the specific subphases of the
lating the environment in a way that the action sequence they affect.
resulting environmental (or social)
pressure or control makes the aban-
doning of the intention more difficult
Action Sequence
(e.g. by making a social commitment or
asking people not to allow one to do
something), or by creating safeguards
Following Heckhausen and Kuhl’s Action
against undesirable environmental
Control Theory, the action sequence process
temptations (e.g. by removing objects
has been divided into three main phases:
that invite unwanted activities);
preactional phase, actional phase, and
(5) parsimony of information processing,
postactional phase.
which essentially refers to a “let’s not
think about it any more but get down to
doing it” strategy, particularly if further
processing may reveal information that Preactional phase
undermines the motivational power of
the current intention. The first, preactional phase, is made up of
three subphases, goal setting, intention
Another important facet of Kuhl’s formation, and the initiation of intention
(1987) theory is the distinction between enactment. Goal setting is described as
action and state orientations. In the first, the having three antecedents, wishes/hopes,
individual’s focus is on a fully-developed desires and opportunities. This last compo-
and realistic action plan; in the state orienta- nent is included because on occasions the
tion mode, however, “attention focuses on starting point of the motivated behavioural
the present state (status quo), a past state process is not the individual’s fantasy land
(especially: a failure) or a future state (es-
pecially: unrealistic goals)” (p. 289). State
orientation (which is similar in many ways
to ‘learned helplessness’) is therefore seen
as a counterproductive disposition. Although
state orientation can be induced by
uncontrollable failure experiences or unre-
alistic instructions, Kuhl assumes that the
two orientations are, to some extent, estab-
lished individual difference factors; that is,
48 Working Papers in Applied Linguistics

Figure 1. Schematic representation of the Process Model of L2 Motivation

MOTIVATIONAL INFLUENCES ACTION SEQUENCE

P Wishes & Hopes

R Motivational
Influences on Desires Opportunities
Goal Setting
E Goal Setting
(Table 1)
A
C
Goal
T (Assigned Task)
Motivational
I Influences on Commitment
Action Plan Intention
O Intention (Compliance)
Formation
Formation
N (Table 2)
A
Intention
L
Motivational
Influences on
P the Initiation of Means & Initiation of Start
Intention Resources Intention Condition
H
Enactment Enactment
A (Table 3)
S
E Action-Launching

I N S T I G A T I O N FORCE Crossing the “Rubicon” of Action

A Subtask
Generation &
C
Implementation
T
I Appraisal Action Control
Executive Action
O
N Motivational
A
L Influences Modify or
Continue Action
Actional
(Table 4)
P Outcome
H Modify Goal
A - +
S
E Terminated Action Achieved Goal

Forming Elaborating
POST- Motivational Causal Postactional Standards &
AC- Influences on Attributions Evaluation Strategies
TION- Postactional
AL Evaluation Dismissing
PHASE (Table 5) Intention &
Further
Planning
Zoltán Dörnyei & István Ottó 49

but rather an emerging opportunity. As can goals or pastimes, along with the re-
be seen in the figure, the above components wards that might have attended them. In
of goal setting are not shaded grey, indi- short, these cases involve placing
cating that at this stage the process has not contingencies on oneself. (p. 37)
as yet reached a state of concrete reality.
The first component to do so is the outcome It needs to be noted here that school situa-
of the goal setting process, the actual goal. tions typically dictate that students meet
It is at this point that the motivated be- established goals as a performance re-
havioural process begins in earnest. quirement for many academic tasks; these
A ‘goal’ is a rather ill-defined or over goals are often set by teachers, sometimes
defined term in motivation theories. The school districts, or by parents (Corno,
assumed role of goals in various constructs 1993). Thus, instead of a voluntarily se-
ranges from being secondary (e.g. the pre- lected goal we often find assigned tasks set
dominating social psychological theory of externally for the students and, therefore,
language learning motivation by Gardner, commitment making can be seen more as a
1985, did not include goals—or as he termed process of reaching compliance. We will
them, orientations—in the core motivation return to this issue at the end of the paper.
concept), to being the single most important Adding commitment to a goal is a cru-
determinants, or the motivational foci, of cial step in the motivational process but it is
action. In Locke and Latham's (1994) goal- not sufficient in itself to energise action if
setting theory, for example, a goal, is seen the goal is not translated into concrete steps
as the ‘engine’ to fire the action and provide the individual needs to take. Thus, a final
the direction in which to act. We take an necessary step in generating a fully
intermediary position and see goals as the operational intention is to develop a man-
first concrete mental representations of a ageable action plan which contains the
desired endstate; goals, in our theory, do not necessary technical details regarding the
directly determine action but are an planned action, namely the action schemata
indispensable step in the motivated (i.e. concrete behavioural guidelines such as
behavioural sequence. subtasks to implement, and a number of
Similarly to Action Control Theory, an relevant attainment strategies to follow) and
‘intention’ in our model is qualitatively the time frame or start condition (i.e.
different from a ‘goal’ in that it already temporal specifications regulating the actual
involves commitment. This is an important timing of the onset of action, e.g. a concrete
distinction and it has been made in order to time - “I’ll get down to it tomorrow” - or a
account for the huge difference which exists condition - “I’ll do it when I have finished
between, on the one hand, the multiple this”). Although a plan of action does not
ideas, wishes, hopes, desires, and long-term have to be completed before initiating an
plans the individual may harbour at a given act—it may be (and usually is) finalised
point of time and, on the other hand, the far while acting—there must be at least a
fewer concrete intentions the individual will general action plan before one is able to act
make actual resolutions to carry out. The at all.
significance of the ‘commitment’ component An operationalised intention is the
was also recognised by goal theoreticians. immediate antecedent of action, but it is
Locke and Latham (1990), for example, state important to realise that action does not
that “Believing that a goal is desirable and follow automatically from it. The right
reachable does not automatically force an opportunity for starting the action may never
individual to act. The individual must materialise, or the means and resources may
choose to put his or her judgement in not be made available, leaving the intention
action” (p. 127); accordingly, they postulate unfulfilled. Thus, our model suggests that
that ‘goal commitment’ is an important goal there are two necessary conditions for
property. Commitment making is a highly issuing an “action-launching impulse”
responsible personal decision and it entails a (Heckhausen & Kuhl, 1985, p. 137): the
significant qualitative change in one’s goal- availability of the necessary means and
related attitudes. As Baumeister (1996) ar- resources and the start condition. The exact
gues, start condition has been specified by the
action plan and, as mentioned above, it can
commitments may involve staking in- be a specific time or a condition. In
terpersonal prestige and even material addition, one usually has several parallel
resources on that goal. Commitments intentions in mind of which only one or two
may also entail forgoing other possible can be implemented at a time. In order to
50 Working Papers in Applied Linguistics

coordinate these, the action plan assigns L2 as a subject matter, language learning in
priority tags to the intentions, determining general, learning in the classroom in general,
their order of enactment, and, therefore, the learning in the particular institution in
start condition may also mean that the turn general, learning in general, and
of a certain intention has come. achievement behaviour in general. The
important point is that a person’s appraisal
of one level can easily be transferred to a
Actional phase broader or narrower level; for example,
negative attitudes evoked by failure in doing
a particular task can easily be generalised to
The onset of action is a major step in the the whole language course or to the whole of
motivational process, resulting in significant language learning (“I’m just not good at
qualitative changes. Following Heckhausen, languages…”), and, conversely, established
we believe that action engagement can be attitudes about the whole school can
compared to crossing a metaphorical profoundly affect one’s specific L2 learning
‘Rubicon’: the individual has committed disposition (“I dislike everything that’s
him/herself to action and now the emphasis going on in this building”).
shifts to factors concerning the implemen- The third main process, action control,
tation of action. In other words, “choice denotes those processes which “protect a
motivation” is replaced by “executive moti- current intention from being replaced should
vation” (Heckhausen, 1991, p. 170). As one of the competing tendencies increase in
Dibbelt and Kuhl (1994) state, “The theory strength before the intended action is
of action control explicitly states that the completed.” (Kuhl, 1994, p. 102). In
actual enactment of an action can be based academic situations this can be charac-
on sources of motivation that differ from terised, using Corno’s (1993) words, “as a
those upon which the original decision was dynamic system of psychological control
based.” (p. 179). processes that protect concentration and
During the actional phase three basic directed effort in the face of personal and/or
processes come into effect: subtask genera- environmental distractions, and so aid
tion and implementation, a complex ongoing learning and performance” (Corno, 1993,
appraisal process, and the application of a p.16). Although the term ‘action control’
variety of action control mechanisms. The may sound novel, similar processes have
first of these refers to learning behaviours been the subject of an increasing amount of
proper. Action initiation starts with research in educational psychology for the
implementing the subtasks that were speci- past decade under the umbrella term of ‘self
fied by the action plan; however, as men- regulatory processes’. For the purpose of our
tioned earlier, action plans are rarely com- model we will distinguish between three
plete (particularly not with sustained ac- types of self-regulatory strategy: action
tivities such as the pursuit of L2 learning) maintenance, language learning, and goal
and during the course of action, one con- setting strategies. Active use of such
tinuously generates (or is assigned) sub- strategies may ‘save’ the action when
tasks/subgoals. In fact, the quality of subtask ongoing monitoring reveals that progress is
generation and the accompanying setting of slowing, halting, or backsliding.
subgoals is one of the principal indicators of Action control/self-regulatory strategies
effective learning. are particularly important from an educa-
The second important ongoing process tional point of view for at least two basic
is appraisal. One continuously evaluates the reasons. First, as Wong and Csikszentmi-
multitude of stimuli coming from the halyi (1991) have found, studying and
environment and the progress one has made schoolwork in general are considered among
towards the action outcome, comparing adolescents’ to be the least rewarding
actual events with predicted ones or with activities. When in class or doing homework
ones that an alternative action sequence students report “low intrinsic motivation and
would offer. This complex process is further negative experience. They generally feel
complicated if we consider the multi-level sad, passive, constrained, bored, detached,
nature of the stimuli one receives. The basic and lonely” (p. 544). Schneider,
unit of language learning behaviour is the Csikszentmihalyi & Knauth (1995) report a
participation in language tasks. These tasks strong negative relationship between being
are embedded in a number of physical and in an academic class and feeling motivated,
psychological contexts of various breadths which they explain by the fact that students
such as the language class, the course, the tend to find most academic classroom
Zoltán Dörnyei & István Ottó 51

activities unenjoyable and uninteresting. All process (starting from goal-setting) and its
this creates fertile ground for distractions effect on his/her self-esteem (Heckhausen,
that need to be controlled for the sake of 1991). During this phase, the actor compares
learning effectiveness. initial expectancies and plans of action to
Second, as argued in the introduction, how they turned out in reality and forms
in school environments most tasks are causal attributions about the extent the
imposed on the students without involving intended goal has been reached. This critical
them in designing their own learning retrospection contributes significantly to
schedules or choosing which activities to accumulated experience, and allows the
engage in. In school, there is often little learner to elaborate his/her internal
preactional activity by students. Therefore, standards and the repertoire of action-
the cumulative instigation force arising from specific strategies. It is through such
the preactional phase (i.e. ‘choice evaluation that an individual can develop a
motivation’) is often relatively weak and stable identity as a successful learner
needs active scaffolding during the actional (Boekaerts, 1988).
phase, which is exactly what action control The formation of adequate standards to
processes are there for. compare actual and potential performance,
On the basis of the interplay of the and the extension of the repertoire of per-
appraisal and control/maintenance proc- sonalised action-control strategies already
esses, the ongoing action will lead to some serve to prepare the ground for the future,
kind of actional outcome: the optimal sce- but before further action can be taken, the
nario is that the actor achieves his/her goal, initial intention has to be dismissed to give
whereas the other extreme is terminating the way to new wishes, goals, and intentions. An
action completely. However, arriving at a accomplished intention may clear the way
dead end during the actional phase does not for a subsequent intention leading to a more
necessarily lead to action abandonment. If distant superordinate goal—in this case the
the motivational foundation of the initial postactional motivation process evolves into
wish or desire was sufficiently powerful, the a preintentional phase and the cycle begins
individual may mentally step back to the anew.
preactional phase, revise the concrete goal
to be pursued and form a new intention (e.g.
by lowering the level of aspiration). Motivational influences on the different
Alternatively, by maintaining the original action phases of the model
intention, the individual may fine-tune or
modify the strategies and subtasks applied in
the pursuit of the goal during the actional The action sequence dimension described
phase. Finally, in case of a temporary above outlines the sequential pattern of the
interruption, action can be continued at a motivational process but it is incomplete
later time. without a second, complimentary dimension
of motivation which is made up of the
various motivational influences that fuel the
Postactional phase actional sequence. These energy sources can
be enhancing or inhibiting, depending on
whether they contribute to the successful
The postactional stage begins after either the implementation of the goal or dampen the
goal has been attained or the action has been actor’s endeavour. As such, motivational
terminated; alternatively, it can also take influences encompass all the various motives
place when action is interrupted for a longer discussed in the motivation literature,
period (e.g. a holiday). The main processes including cognitive, affective, and situ-
during this phase entail evaluating the ational factors or conditions. In our model,
accomplished action outcome and we have made a point of including every
contemplating possible inferences to be major motivational factor from previous
drawn for future actions. Postactional studies in the L2 field and from mainstream
evaluation is different from the ongoing psychology that has been found to influence
appraisal process in that here the individual the commitment to learning.
is not engaged in actual action any longer As indicated in Figure 1, motivational
(that is, he/she is no longer in an influences form five clusters, according to
implementation-oriented mind set), which the five specific phases of the motivated
allows him/her to adopt a broader perspec- action sequence they affect (i.e. goal setting,
tive on the whole motivated behavioural intention formation, initiation of intention
52 Working Papers in Applied Linguistics

enactment, action, and postactional reenrolling in the course (since once action
evaluation). The specific lists of the relevant engagement has been terminated, preactional
motives are included in Tables 1-5; Figure 1 forces become activated again). The reason
describes the interrelationship between these why such cycles do not go on ad infinitum is
motive sets. The motivational influences that after the termination (or completion) of
associated with goal-setting are linked with action a third set of motivational influences,
an arrow to the determinants of intention associated with the postactional phase, come
formation, which are in turn linked to those into force, and the explanations one arrives
of the initiation of intention enactment. This at during this phase about the previous
indicates that in the preactional phase the sequence (e.g. “I simply don’t have the
relevant motivational influences are assumed time/energy/aptitude for L2 learning”)
to have a cumulative effect: the forces active significantly affect subsequent action
in the first stage continue to exert their tendencies.
influence in the second and the third phases,
and the factors first appearing in the second
(intention formation) phase also fuel the Goal setting
third (intention enactment) phase.
Thus, the preactional motivational sys-
tem works like a series of interlinked filters: At any given time people harbour a great
Only the wishes that receive sufficient variety of wishes, hopes, desires, ‘what-
support from the first set of motivational would-happen-ifs’, ‘if-onlys’, etc. These
influences qualify for becoming goals; these coexist peacefully alongside each other on
goals are then submitted to a second the plane of unreality; some of them will
motivational phase, intention formation, never get beyond this stage and remain as
where new energy sources are added to the ‘daydreams’, whereas others will be acted
resultant motivational force, and if this out and fulfilled in the long run. How do we
exceeds the necessary threshold for stepping select from the multitude of our wishes and
further, the goal becomes a fully-fledged desires and how do we process the selected
intention; finally, an action launching wish/desire? The understanding of human
impulse will be issued if the sum of the motivation starts at this basic level of
influences that have fuelled the intention so transforming ‘fantasies’ into reality-oriented
far and the new factors that come into force goals.
in the third, action initiation phase reaches a
certain level of strength. The overall
resultant motivational force associated with
the preactional phase is labelled in the figure Table 1. Motivational influences on goal
as the instigation force, which determines setting
the intensity of action initiation.
Moving further ‘down’ Figure 1, how-
ever, the motivational influences associated • Subjective values and norms
with the actional phase are not directly Incentive value of goal-related action,

related to the motives affecting the earlier outcomes, and consequences
stages of the process. This is in line with (instrumentality)
Heckhausen and Kuhl’s ‘Action Control
Theory’, which emphasises that ‘executive • Perceived potency of potential goal
motives’ are largely different from the • Environmental stimuli; action
motives making up ‘choice motivation’. possibilities; family expectations
Indeed, very few motivational forces have a • Language/language-learning-related
global effect on every stage of the actor’s attitudes (integrativeness)
behaviour, which explains why even a strong
motivational disposition can be cancelled
out by newly emerging forces. Only by
assuming such a division of motives related
to the preactional and the actional phases In our model we have distinguished five
can we explain, for example, the frequent main motivational factors underlying the
phenomenon of someone deciding to enrol in goal-setting process (Table 1). First and
a language course (motivated by ‘choice foremost are the individual’s subjective
motivation’), then soon dropping out values and norms that have developed
(because the ‘executive motives’ fail to during the past, as a reaction to past expe-
sustain the instigation force), and then again riences. This “sense-of-self” dimension
(Maehr, 1984, p. 126) refers to the more or
Zoltán Dörnyei & István Ottó 53

less organised collections or internalised tives that a person perceives to be available


perceptions, beliefs, and feelings related to and appropriate in a given situation, in view
who one is in the social world. Individuals of the sociocultural norms and pressures
differ greatly in the content and the hierar- (including family expectations), as well as
chies of their value dispositions (cf. the other external factors such as geographic
saying, “There is no accounting for taste”). location and socioeconomic status that exists
These internal preferences interplay with the for the individual. Broadly speaking, one
specific incentive values associated with the will act in terms of what is perceived as
anticipated goals. It is important to realise possible. In educational contexts the role of
that there are three different levels of goal- the environment is particularly strong. As
specific values (Heckhausen & Kuhl, 1985): Anderman & Maehr (1994, p. 296) argue,
The first-order level refers to the action (i.e.
the intrinsic value of the ongoing activity While individuals may bring entering
itself), the second-order level to the outcome biases to bear in any given situation,
of an action (the inherently valuable characteristics of the situation are also
characteristics with reference to one’s basic crucial in determining what goals will
personal values and needs), and the third- be adopted. Thus, recent studies suggest
order level to the consequences that might that the psychological environment of
arise from an achieved outcome. the classroom may have a strong
Heckhausen and Kuhl’s conceptualisation is influence on the goals that students
very similar to a comprehensive model of adopt…. The adoption of goals is
task values developed by Eccles and related to specific instructional prac-
Wigfield (1995), defining such values in tices (e.g. grouping, recognition,
terms of four components: “intrinsic value” evaluation, the nature of the task) and
(or interest), “attainment value” (or students’ perception of goal stresses...
importance), “extrinsic utility value”, and Other research suggests that the school
“cost”. In our model the last component, as a whole can influence the goals that
cost, is assumed to come into force only in students adopt. Research on school
the second, intention formation stage. culture and climate suggests that
In the L2 field, the perceived incentive schools emphasise different goals.
value of the outcome and the consequence
has typically been captured by Gardner’s Finally, since our model concerns moti-
(1985) concept of ‘instrumental motivation’ vation to learn a L2, attitudes toward the L2
or ‘instrumentality’. Because the study of an and L2 learning (captured by Gardner’s,
L2 has usually been conceived of as merely 1985, concept of ‘integrativeness’) also play
instrumental to reaching the desired endstate a crucial role in making any L2-related
(namely, L2 proficiency), the first aspect, motivational decisions (e.g. language choice,
the intrinsic interest in pursuing language decision to start L2 learning or to visit the
studies, has been largely underplayed (cf., L2 community for learning’s sake).
however, Noels, Clément & Pelletier, in
press).
The above mentioned value preferences Intention formation
already screen out many ‘unsuitable’ wishes
and desires, and they also help to determine
the general “potency” (Heckhausen & Kuhl, Arriving at a goal, means that the individual
1985, p. 135) of the goals. This refers to a has formulated an ‘I want to’ type of internal
subjective feeling about the general statement. However, the fact is that not
probability of attaining the respective goal. every goal will be realised. Simply having
Potency appraisals are based on the the incentive to strive for a goal does not
opportunities and affordances that one guarantee that the person will actually
perceives one will have in the future— undertake the effort that is required. There
wishes and desires that are seen as totally are a great number of factors that determine
unrealistic remain ignored. whether the goal will be further processed
The external environment also exerts a into an intention, and therefore intention
considerable influence on our choice of formation involves a process of deliberation,
potential goals. Some of our wishes may be weighing the feasibility and desirability of
seen as supported by the environment while the available options, and visualising the
others may be completely out of place. possible incentive-laden consequences of
Maehr (1984) uses the term ‘action possi- one’s potential actions (Table 2)
bilities’ to refer to the behavioural alterna-
54 Working Papers in Applied Linguistics

tries. One thing needs to be noted here


Table 2. Motivational influences on in- regarding goal difficulty: The above would
tention formatiom suggest that the easier the goals are the
higher the individual’s motivation, whereas
this is not the case. Locke and Kristof
• Expectancy of success/perceived (1996) report on meta-analyses of over 400
coping potential studies which show unambiguously that
goals that are perceived as difficult and
• self-efficacy/self-confidence
challenging (but still attainable) lead to
• perceived goal difficulty higher performance than goals that are easy.
• amount of expected support A second important motivational factor
• L2 anxiety is the perceived relevance of the goal and
• perceived L2 competence the accompanying cost-benefit calculations
• L2 contact the individual makes. Relevance can be both
• causal attributions personal and setting-related. The personal
• Relevance (personal and setting- aspect is associated with the individual’s
related); cost-benefit calculations current life concerns: Only a limited number
• Need for achievement and fear of of goals can be pursued at a time so the
failure individual needs to choose the ones
currently most pressing. Setting-related
• Degree of self-determination (type of
relevance is similar in nature but concerns
regulation)
more specific situational aspects. It implies
• Goal properties that a goal may or may not fit into the
• goal specificity current concerns that dominate at the time or
• goal proximity social setting. For instance, action
• goal harmony/conflict tendencies directed at relaxing activities are
• level of aspiration more appropriate, and therefore more
• Availability of task opportunities and powerful, for leisure or holiday periods than
options in vocational settings (Heckhausen & Kuhl,
1985). Costs involve the negative value
• Learner beliefs about L2 learning; component of a goal. So far it has been the
knowledge of learning strategies; positive aspects of the initial wishes that
domain-specific knowledge were considered, whereas now the individual
• Urgency; external demands; unique needs to compare those with the inevitable
opportunity negative valence of the planned action,
which includes factors such as expended
effort and time, other actions that the
planned action would exclude, and various
emotional costs (e.g. anxiety, fear of
failure).
A selected goal, by definition, must
Achievement motivation theories have
have already passed the test of potency, that
traditionally entailed the relatively stable
is, it must have been regarded as broadly
and enduring personality constructs of need
attainable. In the intention formation phase
for achievement and fear of failure (e.g.
the expectancy of success is more
Atkinson & Raynor, 1974). Individuals with
specifically assessed. Based on a number of
a high need for achievement are interested in
interacting factors such as self-efficacy/self-
excellence for its own sake, tend to initiate
confidence, perceived goal difficulty, the
achievement activities, work with heightened
amount of expected support, L2 anxiety,
intensity at these tasks, and persist in the
perceived competence, the quality and
face of failure. Fear of failure is the opposite
quantity of previous L2 contact, and causal
of need for achievement in that here the
attributions about past experiences
main drive to do well comes from avoiding a
(successes and failures), the individual
negative outcome rather than approaching a
makes an evaluation of his/her coping
positive one. These two tendencies are
potential in the planned action. The greater
considered to affect a person's achievement
the perceived likelihood of goal attainment,
behaviour in every facet of life, including
the higher the degree of the individual's
language learning.
positive motivation. Conversely, it is un-
The issue of self-determination, or
likely that effort will be invested in a task if
learner autonomy, and the related paradigm
the individual is convinced that he/she
of intrinsic versus extrinsic motivation have
cannot succeed no matter how hard he/she
Zoltán Dörnyei & István Ottó 55

been in the focus of motivational psychology ultimate level of L2 proficiency the learner
for over two decades, and it has become an intends to reach. Not everybody sets out to
integral part of several L2 motivational attain a near-native level of L2 competence:
approaches in the 1990’s (for a review, see some learners, for example, only aim to
Dörnyei, 1998). Without going into details acquire a working knowledge of the L2,
here, it has been generally accepted that which obviously effects their long-term
motivation to learn and learner autonomy go achievement strivings (cf. Dörnyei, 1990).
hand in hand, that is, “enhanced motivation As was said earlier, the development of
is conditional on learners taking an action plan is an imperative to forming a
responsibility for their own learning [...] and fully operational intention. This is why the
perceiving that their learning successes and availability of task opportunities and
failures are to be attributed to their own options is an important, though not indis-
efforts and strategies rather than to factors pensable, motivational condition. It is easy
outside their control” (Dickinson, 1995, p. to see that one may be more inclined to
173-74). decide on a certain course of action where
A further set of influential factors con- ready-made options are given than on an
cern various properties of the selected goal, activity for which creating the necessary
such as goal specificity, proximity, goal conditions already requires considerable
harmony/conflict, and the level of aspira- effort. For example, an advert drawing
tion. Goal specificity refers to how clear and attention to an attractive language course
elaborate goal specifications are. Locke and may be more influential in initiating lan-
Kristof (1996) provide evidence that goals guage learning than a situation in which the
that are specific rather than vague enhance learner needs to find out from scratch what
performance. A second important channels of learning, if any, are available.
characteristic of goals is their proximity. In As Heckhausen (1991) argues, “The decision
terms of time scale, goals range in time from [of initiating action] is frequently
those that are nearly immediate to those that predetermined by anticipated opportunities
are several months or years away. As that seem favorable for the realization of
Karniol and Ross (1996) summarise, a particular intentions” (p. 11); indeed, it is
“positive time preference” (p. 603) can be this recognition that underlies the provision
observed, with the motivational pull of goals of vocational information to learners before
with immediate outcomes being stronger they commit themselves to a certain career
than that of goals in a temporal distance path.
because it is easier to judge progress toward Another, equally important, determinant
the former. Furthermore, the power of distal of the quality of the action plan one
goals, even if they are selected for action, develops is the learner’s beliefs about L2
may spontaneously decrease more rapidly learning, knowledge of learning strategies,
during goal pursuit than that of proximal and sufficient domain-specific knowledge.
goals, and it is also easy to postpone These factors form influential predisposi-
pursuing a distal goal in the present in the tions in the learners about the learning
belief that there is ample time to mount the process, stemming from the learners’ fami-
effort later. lies, peer groups, and prior learning expe-
A further issue is that an individual may riences. For example, If someone thinks of
often wish to achieve a number of different the study of a language only as tedious and
goals at the same time, for example, acquire hard work characterised by endless memo-
knowledge, meet people, and have a good risation of bilingual word lists, this will
time. With such multiple goals the extent of obviously reduce his/her initial enthusiasm,
goal harmony/conflict is an important factor. whereas an informed, ‘made to measure’
If the various goals one entertains can action plan (e.g. a computer devotee de-
coexist harmoniously, this will increase goal ciding to learn through specially designed
commitment, whereas if striving for a goal computer games) might give the necessary
goes at the expense of a potential other, incentive to engage in the learning process.
efforts towards this goal may eventually Finally, in certain cases commitment
weaken as one thinks about alternatives does not happen even if many of the above
(Green, 1995). A final goal property that is mentioned motivational influences are in
of great importance with complex learning place—at such times what we need is a final
targets such as the mastery of L2 proficiency ‘push’, such as some sort of urgency,
is the level of aspiration. In our case this powerful external demands, or a unique
variable is not so much related to concepts opportunity. In Heckhausen and Kuhl’s
like goal level or goal difficulty as to the (1985) words,
56 Working Papers in Applied Linguistics

enact an intention and thus to bridge the gap


Commitment, however, does not appear between choice and action, Kuhl introduces
to be a necessary result of the belief the constructs of action versus state
that attainment of a goal is desirable. orientation, which we have already de-
Even a high product of value and scribed when summarising his theory. State
expectancy may not be sufficient to oriented people tend to be hindered by
produce a commitment. A unique op- “intrusive thoughts about bugs, slips in
portunity or increased urgency—in the strategy, and failure” (Boekaerts, 1994, p.
face of an approaching deadline—may 434); they often procrastinate and tend to
represent an additional requirement for ruminate on acting rather than getting down
a commitment for future action (i.e. for to it. Those with action orientation, on the
generating an intention). (p. 136) other hand, are more disposed to act their
intentions out. Thus, action and state
orientations differ from achievement ten-
The initiation of intention enactment: dencies such as need for achievement and
Crossing the ‘Rubicon’ of action fear of failure in that they concern the
effectiveness or impairment of the control of
action implementation rather than the
It is not always the case that intentions are formation of intentions.
implemented immediately after their for- A second variable affecting the enact-
mation; quite frequently there is some delay ment of an intention is the person’s per-
before action takes place, and, as argued ceived behavioural control. In Ajzen’s
earlier, in certain cases even fully (1988) theory of planned behaviour this
operationalised intentions never reach the component is one of the key variables (along
actional phase. This indicates that there is a with intention) predicting behavioural
separate processing phase between intention performance. It refers to the perceived ease
formation and action: the initiation of or difficulty of performing the behaviour;
intention enactment. This is not to be con- simply stated, one must believe that he/she
fused with intention formation, which con- has sufficient control over the outcome to
cerned the actual decision whether to do a exert effort towards achieving it.
certain thing; here the main question is There are also some negative forces
finding the right point in time for actualising working against intention enactment. These
the intention to act, particularly with respect may be caused by various distracting influ-
to seeking and utilising suitable ences and obstacles, which obviously stand
opportunities and the preparation of appro- in the way of action implementation, par-
priate steps for implementation. Table 3 ticularly if there are powerful competing
presents the main motivational influences action tendencies available. In situations in
that affect this action initiation phase. which the efficient implementation of an
intention is rather difficult or requires too
much time to seize the best opportunity,
Table 3. Motivational influences on the even relatively strong intentions may easily
initiation of intention enactment be downgraded in terms of priority and the
execution of a competing action alternative
may occupy attentional capacity.
Finally, should one be inclined to aban-
• Action vs. state orientation don the enactment of an intention due to
• Perceived behavioural control some of the above difficulties, there is one
• Distracting influences and obstacles; more powerful factor that comes into force,
number and strength of competing action potentially making the person think again:
tendencies the perceived consequences for not acting.
Even if everything seems to work against
• Perceived consequences for not acting
pursuing an intention, the feeling that “I
simply cannot not do it!”, that is, shifting the
perspective from what it takes to reach the
As Kuhl (1994) argues, people often do, target to what will happen if it is not
or perhaps more importantly fail to do, reached, may provide the necessary impetus
things without any available ‘rational’ to instigate action.
explanation to derive from the information
that underlay the selection of the intention.
In order to explain such irrational failure to
Zoltán Dörnyei & István Ottó 57

Actional processes
Table 4. Executive motivational influ-
Once an initial wish has ‘obtained’ sufficient ences
motivational support to pass all the hurdles,
the individual is ready to embark on a course
of action. The intensity of the ‘action- • Selective sensitivity to aspects of the
launching impulse’ will depend on the environment
cumulative or resultant force of all the • Quality of internal model of reference
motivational influences active in the pre- • action schemata
actional phase (i.e. ‘instigation force’). As • performance standards
stated earlier, crossing the metaphorical
• Quality of learning experience
‘Rubicon’ of action opens a whole new
chapter in the motivation scene; indeed, only • novelty
few of the motivational influences • pleasantness
associated with the actional phase in Table 4 • goal/need significance
appeared earlier in relation to preactional • coping potential
events. Not surprisingly, the biggest group • self and social image
of factors concerns the appraisal system and • Perceived contingent relationship
the outcome of the appraisal process. The between action and outcome; perceived
rest of the components concern the progress
effectiveness of the action control processes,
• success
the impact of external influences such the
• “flow”
teacher’s role, and factors inherent to the
action itself. • Sense of self-determination/autonomy
The functioning of the appraisal system • Teacher’s and parents’ motivational
is greatly affected by the individual’s se- influence
lective sensitivity to aspects of the envi- • autonomy supporting vs. controlling
ronment. As Boekaerts (1987) points out, • affiliative motive
learners give different weights to specific direct socialisation of motivation

segments of the task-situation complex.
Based on the learners’ idiosyncratic features • modelling
and past experiences, they may encode • task presentation
certain aspects of the learning environment • feedback
in a strikingly different manner. For • Performance appraisal, reward structure,
example, what one person may find stressful, classroom goal structure (competitive.
another may find challenging. For a review Individualistic, cooperative)
of the various psychological factors that • Influence of learner group (goal-
might underlie individual differences in the orientedness, cohesiveness, norm and
learners’ perception and interpretation of the role system, peer role modelling),
environment, the reader is referred to a classroom climate, and school
recent summary by Ehrman and Dörnyei environment
(1998). A second, partly related factor
• Task conflict; competing action
affecting the appraisal system is the quality
tendencies; other distracting influences;
of the internal model of reference
availability of action alternatives
(Boekaerts, 1988). In order to be able to
interpret the learning context, the task • Costs involved and natural tendency to
demands, and one’s own competence to meet lose sight of goal and get bored/tired of
these demands, learners draw on an the activity
internally generated model made up of • Knowledge of and skills in using self-
“declarative, procedural and episodic regulatory strategies
information … activated from long-term • language learning strategies
memory upon confrontation with a learning • goal setting strategies
task” (Boekaerts, 1988, p. 275). This inter- • action maintenance strategies
nal model acts as a frame of reference and
functions as a performance standard in • Perceived consequences of action
defining what success and failure entails in a abandonment
particular situation. It also provides a sound
basis for selecting, constructing, and
monitoring strategies and subtasks.
58 Working Papers in Applied Linguistics

The next group of factors affecting the their action is conducive to reaching that
results of the appraisal process are the actual outcome they experience a feeling of
stimuli generated by the environment, that success, which then provides further
is, the perceived quality of the learning motivation. In Boekaerts’s (1988) words,
experience. According to Schumann’s
(1998) neurobiological model of stimulus When a learner perceives a contingent
appraisal, the brain evaluates the stimuli it path between his potential actions and
receives along five dimensions: novelty the learning outcome, his confidence
(degree of unexpectedness/ familiarity), will be high and his performance will
pleasantness (attractiveness), goal /need not be impeded by debilitating anxiety.
significance (whether the stimulus is When the opposite relation holds,
instrumental in satisfying needs or achieving mental withdrawal from the threatening
goals), coping potential (whether the demands may result as well as the
individual expects to be able to cope with perception of discomfort and tension.
the event), and self and social image (p. 275)
(whether the event is compatible with social
norms and the individual's self-concept). A particularly powerful state of optimal
The five dimensions capture well the various experience is the concept of ‘flow’ intro-
situation-specific appraisals proposed in the duced by Csikszentmihalyi (1990). It repre-
L2 literature (e.g. Crookes & Schmidt’s, sents a state of total involvement during
1991, system made up of interest, relevance, some creative activity that is characterised
expectancy, and satisfaction, also adopted by an equilibrium between the amount of
by Dörnyei, 1994), as well as covering challenge in activities and the individual’s
several of the most important current issues capabilities.
in the educational psychological literature A further powerful factor regarding
(e.g. the concern about self-esteem/self- learning experiences that was already
worth, self-efficacy, intrinsic interest, well- mentioned with respect to the intention
being). For example, based on her extensive formation stage is the learner’s sense of self-
classroom research, Boekaerts (1994) determination/autonomy. The issue of the
identified three types of appraisals to type of regulation seems to be one of the
explain much of the variance in learning most pervasive ones during the motivated
intention: (1) task attraction, (2) perceived behavioural process; this underlies Deci and
personal relevance, and (3) perceived self- Ryan’s (1985) claim that the need for
competence. Schumann’s model covers all autonomy, that is, the desire to be self-
the three components. In a more detailed initiating and self-regulating of one's ac-
summary of the main aspects of the task- tions, is an innate human need, and is a
situation complex from the pupils’ point of prerequisite for any behaviour to be intrin-
view, Boekaerts (1988) also listed sically rewarding (indeed, Csikszentmi-
familiarity judgement, success expectancy halyi’s, 1990, ‘flow’ also presupposes a
judgement, reward value judgement, primarily intrinsically regulated behavioural
perceived teacher utility judgement, and sequence).
peer success expectancy in addition to the Besides the learner, there are certain
above factors. Only the last two components other key figures affecting the motivational
are not directly covered by Schumann’s quality of the learning process, namely the
proposed appraisal dimensions, but they can teacher and the parents. Their role as mo-
be seen as being subsumed by the other tivational socialisers has been described in
components. However, in order to emphasise detail by a number of works in the literature
the social nature and aspects of classroom (e.g. Colletta, Clément & Edwards, 1983;
learning, we have also separated peer and Dörnyei, 1994; Gardner, 1985, Gottfried,
teacher appraisals from the more general Fleming & Gottfried, 1994). Teachers are
appraisal of the course and the curriculum the officially designated leaders within the
(see below). classroom; as such they are the most visible
Because learning is a goal-oriented figures, who embody group conscience and
activity, the perceived contingent relation- serve as a reference and a standard. They are
ship between action and outcome and the often the focus of attention and, as Jesuíno
perceived progress the learner has made on (1996) summarises, they function as an
this contingent path deserves explicit “emotional amplifier of the group whose
treatment. Students constantly evaluate how appeals and example are critical for
well they are doing in terms of approaching mobilising the group” (p. 115) In short, “To
the desired outcome, and if they feel that lead is to motivate, that is, ‘directing’ and
Zoltán Dörnyei & István Ottó 59

‘energizing’ ” (p. 114). One of the main recently looked into group-specific cognitive
impacts teachers and parents exert is related constructs (like group efficacy). Recent
to self-determination, as several studies have studies suggest that the psychological
found that these authority figures’ environment of the school as a whole (e.g.
motivational practices can be described school-wide stress on accomplishment,
along a continuum between autonomy- power, recognition, affiliation; school-level
supporting versus controlling (e.g. Gottfried authority and management structures,
et al. 1994; Noels, Clément & Pelletier, in grouping and evaluation practices) may also
press). Another important motive related to have a strong influence on students’
these superordinate figures is the ‘affiliative motivation (Maehr & Midgley, 1991). For
motive’, which refers to students’ need to do example, Anderman and Maehr (1994)
well in school in order to please the teacher report on a study which demonstrated that
or their parents. Finally, teachers can also school effects such as the above seem to
exert a direct motivational influence by increase with grade level: whereas in the 4th
actively socialising the learners’ motivation grade these explained 7% of the variance in
through appropriate modelling, task pres- motivation, the figure grew to 21% when
entations, and their feedback. students reached the 10th grade.
One particularly featured aspect of how It probably requires little justification
teachers structure classroom life is the type that task conflict, competing action ten-
of performance appraisal, reward structure, dencies, other distracting influences, and
and the more general classroom goal the availability of action alternatives have a
structure they introduce. It is well docu- weakening effect on the resultant moti-
mented in the literature that these have far vational force associated with the particular
reaching and often unintended consequences course of action. In such cases, unless ef-
on how learners approach the learning tasks fective action control strategies are activated
(e.g. Ames, 1992; Maehr, 1984; Pintrich & (see below), the behavioural process may be
Schunk, 1996). Harter (1992), for example, interrupted and in some cases terminated.
found that the combination of comparative Further negative influences are provided by
grading practices, standardised test scores, a the costs involved in pursuing the activity (a
focus on the correct solutions, and the factor already mentioned at the intention
salience of social comparison, serve to formation phase) and one’s natural tendency
decrease children’s interest in and to lose sight of goal and get bored/tired of
enjoyment of the learning process and the activity; these factors have been part of
moderate their preference for challenge. what Atkinson and Birch (1974) termed
Cooperation in the classroom, on the other ‘consummatory force’ in their ‘Dynamic
hand, has been shown to augment motivation Action Model’.
to learn (e.g. Dörnyei, 1997; Sharan & An important source of scaffolding and
Shaulov, 1990; Slavin, 1996). enhancing motivation is the knowledge of
Parents and teachers are not the only and skills in using self-regulatory strategies.
external sources of situation-specific moti- Winne (1995) argues convincingly that all
vation. An increasing body of research has learners inherently self-regulate, but there
highlighted the influence of the learner are individual differences regarding their
group, the classroom climate, and the school knowledge base about self-regulatory
environment. Learners do not exist in learning and their knowledge about when to
isolation but function within organisational engage that knowledge and their skills. We
structures through socially mediated effort. already stated in the introduction that in
Therefore, various aspects of the dynamics sustained learning of skills and knowledge,
of the learner group (e.g. goal-orientedness, self-regulatory processes take on special
cohesiveness, the emerging classroom role significance as key motivational influences
and norm system, peer role modelling; for a on learning and performance (Kanfer, 1996).
review, see Ehrman & Dörnyei, 1998) have a As mentioned earlier, we distinguish three
profound influence on the individual types of such strategies: learning, goal
members’ motivation as they try to conform setting, and action maintenance strategies.
to social standards set by the class group and By using learning strategies, a learner
the school. Our belief is that the significance already demonstrates motivation, since they
of these factors has not been sufficiently involve processes whereby the learner vol-
highlighted in the motivational literature untarily activates cognitions/behaviours/
relative to their importance, although, as affects (depending how one defines learning
Dörnyei (1998) summarises, several studies strategies) in order to increase the
in social and educational psychology have effectiveness of his/her own learning (in-
60 Working Papers in Applied Linguistics

deed, Corno, 1993, p. 17, for example, “Set contingencies for performance that can
refers to them as “mindful effort invest- be carried out mentally (e.g. self-reward;
ments”). The fact that learning strategies self-imposed penance”, “Escalate goals by
enhance achievement generates positive prioritising and imagining their value”, and
affect in the learners about how and what “Visualise doing the work successfully”(p.
they study, thereby reinforcing their moti- 16). Emotion control strategies include
vated disposition. “Generate useful diversions”, “Visualise the
Goal-setting strategies are more di- work successfully and feeling good about
rectly related to motivation. Goals are not that (change the way you respond
only outcomes to shoot for but also stan- emotionally to the task”, “Recall your
dards by which to evaluate one's perform- strengths and your available resources”, and
ance. Thus, goal setting refers to estab- “Consider any negative feelings about the
lishing quantitative and qualitative standards experience and ways to make it more
of performance that can help guide and reassuring” (p. 16).
regulate action better than distal, vague, or Baumeister (1996) emphasises the as-
‘do-your-best’ kind of goals. In the case of pect of action maintenance strategies that
long-lasting, continuous activities such as provides people with powerful motivational
language learning, where there is only a forces to enable them to regulate the cog-
rather distal goal of task completion (i.e. nitive and emotional impact of ego threats.
mastering the L2), the setting of proximal By consciously ignoring face-threatening
subgoals (i.e. short-term objectives, such as stimuli, by adopting ‘defensive preoccupa-
taking tests, passing exams, satisfying tion’ (i.e. focusing on an alternative stimulus
learning contracts) may therefore have a that can absorb attention), by summoning
powerful motivating function in that they positive feelings/happy memories to defuse
mark progress and provide immediate the threat, or by constructing their narratives
incentive and feedback. Winne (1995) points of events so as to place themselves in a more
out that although it may appear relatively positive light, people may self-regulate
simple to train students to set more and more cognitive processes and thus protect their
precise objectives, their ‘stylistic self-esteem from threatening implications.
dispositions’ to set such objectives for Garcia and Pintrich (1994) highlight one
themselves may constitute an important particular strategy that serves to maintain
individual difference variable. self-worth, ‘self-affirmation’: If an
Finally, action maintenance strategies individual experiences a negative evaluation
are specifically directed at maintaining of the self in a valued domain, a self-
motivation and protecting the currently affirmative process is initiated, and the
active intention. This reactive, protection individual will “seek to affirm a positive
function is of particular significance be- global evaluation of the self by activating
cause, as Atkinson and Birch (1974) em- positive conceptions of the self (those in
phasised, there are many action tendencies other, equally valued domains)” (p. 137).
awaiting implementation at a given point of Just like in the preactional phase, the
time and even during the course of a last motivational factor to be listed here is
seemingly smoothly running activity the the perceived consequences of action
opportunity to pursue other attractive ac- abandonment. It is sometimes only when
tivities can suddenly surface. Action main- everything else fails and one is about to quit,
tenance strategies are also useful with distal that one thinks over what action
goals to help individuals to maintain their abandonment would really entail, and the
priorities in the face of temptation and perceived possible negative consequences
adversity. may activate enough energy to keep going.
During the last decade quite an array of
action maintenance strategies have been
documented in the literature. We have Postactional evaluation
already described Kuhl’s (1987) system of
six major types of self-regulatory strategies.
Adapting this conceptualisation to In our model we distinguished four major
educational contexts, Corno (1993) distin- motivational influences active in the post-
guishes two large classes of “volitional actional phase: attributional factors, self-
control strategies” (the term she uses for concept beliefs, the quality and quantity of
action maintenance and goal-setting strate- evaluational/attributional cues and feed-
gies): motivation control and emotion con- back, and action versus state orientation
trol strategies. Examples of the former are (Table 5).
Zoltán Dörnyei & István Ottó 61

Table 5. Motivational influences on post- multiple experiences with those events.


actional evaluation For example, some people, usually labelled
as ‘internals’, tend to perceive a direct link
between their behaviour and reinforcement,
• Attributional factors: attributional whereas ‘externals’ tend not to see such a
style and biases, prior knowledge about contingency (and thus are likely to make
“scripted” events external kind of attributions). Perhaps the
Self-concept beliefs: self- best-known stylistic disposition is ‘learned

confidence/self-efficacy; self- helplessness’, referring to an acquired
competence; self worth; prior resigned, pessimistic, and helpless state that,
performance history once established, is very difficult to reverse.
Second, in certain situations people may
• Evaluational/attributional cues, also have attributional biases, that is, in-
feedback correct schemas and inference rules that are
• Action vs. state orientation used to make attributions (Pintrich &
Schunk, 1996). Examples include the self-
serving bias (the tendency to take responsi-
bility for success but deny it for failure); the
The key tenet of attribution theory is
self-centred bias (the tendency to take more
that the perceived causal attributions of past
than due responsibility for any outcome); or
successes and failures (i.e. inferences about
basic attribution errors such as the tendency
why outcomes occur) have behavioural
to attribute something to dispositional or
consequences on future achievement
personal factors while ignoring relevant (or
strivings. As Graham (1994) summarises, the
even crucial) situational factors, and vice
most common attributions in school
versa.
environments are those to ability (including
Third, as Weiner (1984) points out,
both aptitude and acquired skills), effort,
attributional search is not indiscriminately
task difficulty, luck, mood, family back-
displayed in all situations, for this would
ground, and help or hindrance from others.
place great cognitive strain on the individ-
Among these, ability and effort are the most
ual. Rather, causal searches are more
dominant perceived causes in the Western
prominent in the case of unexpected out-
culture. In Weiner’s (1992) model, causal
comes that do not conform with the ‘scripts’
attributions can be categorised along three
of what are seen as normal events and
dimensions: stability (the cause is constant
situations (e.g. failure when success was
or varying over time), locus (the cause is
anticipated or unfulfilled desires); it seems
internal or external to the person), and
reasonable to assume, then, that differences
control (whether or not the cause is subject
in the prior knowledge about scripted events
to volitional control). It has been shown in
will also cause individual differences in the
several studies that the type of attribution
attribution process.
one makes directly affects the person’s
Self-concept beliefs, including one’s
future behavioural outcome expectancies.
established level of self-confidence/self-
Failure that is ascribed to stable and
efficacy, self-competence, and self-worth in
uncontrollable factors such as low ability
different domains, also influence the result
hinders future achievement behaviour more
of postactional evaluation. Learners with
than failure that is ascribed to unstable and
relatively high self-perceptions handle
controllable factors such as effort. Evidence
occasional failures much better than learners
for the importance of attribution to effort in
with low self-worth beliefs in that they tend
secondary school pupils’ motivation
to heighten and sustain effort in the face of
complex has been provided by Boekaerts
failure, while mobilising new strategies to
(1988), who found that seeing effort as an
tackle the task. Confident learners are also
important causal factor in gaining progress
less likely to engage in debilitating self-
(i.e. realising that effort pays off)
analysis rather than maintaining a task-
significantly contributed to the students’
focus. It needs little justification that the
willingness to devote processing capacity to
individual’s prior performance history plays
learning tasks in her sample.
an important role in shaping these self-
There are considerable individual dif-
beliefs.
ferences in forming attributions. First,
So far we have concentrated on the
people have different attributional styles,
learner-internal factors affecting postac-
that is, a habitual way of explaining events
tional evaluation, but the process is also a
along one rather than the other attribution
function of external evaluational/ attribu-
dimensions, which develops as a result of
62 Working Papers in Applied Linguistics

tional cues. Of all the attributional cues in 1994, p. 95).


classroom contexts, the most featured one
from the learners’ point of view is the
feedback from the teacher, including subtle Limitations of the model
emotional responses. An often mentioned
distinction of two types of feedback involves
‘informational feedback’, which comments The Process Model presented in this paper is
on competence, and ‘controlling feedback’, not without limitations. The issues that will
which judges performance against external be briefly summarised in the following all
standards (Brophy & Good, 1986). It is concern areas that have not been given
generally maintained that from a sufficient attention in psychological research
motivational point of view the former should and therefore require further investigations
be dominant since social comparison is to resolve them.
considered most detrimental to intrinsic
motivation (Ames, 1992). Graham (1994)
talks about three more subtle feedback types Unconscious/irrational motives. One of
whose negative impact on learner behaviour the most prevailing issues in motivational
has been confirmed by laboratory research: psychology is the question of how conscious
communicated pity instead of anger after or unconscious the motivational
failure; the offering of praise after success determinants of human behaviour are (the
(particularly for easy tasks); and unsolicited issue has, in fact, come up under a number
offers of help (particularly ‘gratuitous help’ of different labels, with non-conscious
such as supplying answers outright). All the motives referred to as unconscious, non-
three feedback practices are often related to cognitive, automatic, instinct/drive-driven,
low self-concept of competence; of these, socially/behaviourally-conditioned, etc.).
the mention of praise might be most This issue as a whole reaches far beyond the
surprising, but interestingly, as Graham scope of this paper but it may be worth
summarises, it is blame and criticism rather summarising our current stance regarding
than praise that often conveys to the learners conscious vs. non-conscious motives.
the teacher’s high expectations. Indeed, Classroom events are varied and complex,
Paris and Turner (1994) also point out that and there is no doubt that there are a number
students may interpret success that comes of unconscious/irrational factors that
without challenge or risk-taking as an underlie (motivate and demotivate) student
indication of the lower expectations held by behaviour; for example, as Weiner (1984)
others for their own level of achievement. summarises, “self-esteem is defended in
One well-known effect of teachers’ feedback unknown ways; expectancies are biased;
behaviour is the ‘Pygmalion effect’, when illogical decisions are reached; information
teacher expectations of their students’ rate is improperly utilized; and there is gross
of progress functions as a self-fulfilling personal delusion” (p. 18). Furthermore,
prophecy, with the learners living up (or although Freud’s emphasis on the uncon-
down) to these expectations regardless of scious, sexual motivation may not be com-
their actual learning potential. pletely relevant to learning activities,
Finally, although one might think that classroom events often have certain sexual
nothing could be simpler than abandoning an undertones. In a review of the con-
action, this is not always the case. As scious/unconscious issue, Sorrentino (1996)
Beckmann (1994: 159-60) argues, “some- supports the importance of nonconscious
times it is just as difficult to stop executing forces by arguing that behaviour can happen
an activity which does not seem to be very without reference to conscious thought,
promising any longer and initiate another although cognitions can inhibit or further
more promising action”. People are known instigate such behaviour and can also
to get stuck in unfruitful behavioural se- strengthen or weaken other competing action
quences, unable to ‘cut their losses’, and this tendencies. As he concludes, research
is why Kuhl has extended the concepts of evidence leaves “little room for those who
action vs. state orientation (described believe that all behaviour must be preceded
earlier) to apply to the “disengagement from by conscious thought” (p. 635).
an intention and the initiation of a new On the other hand, it is also clear that
course of action in situations in which the many aspects of student behaviour are quite
intention has become unattainable or in logical and rational. This is especially true if
which changing conditions require a change we consider learning behaviours and
in the goal hierarchy” (Kuhl & Goschke, sustained learning processes in particular—
Zoltán Dörnyei & István Ottó 63

as Bandura (1991) asserts, “most human behaving at all. Otherwise, the


behaviour is activated and regulated over individual is always doing something. A
extended periods by anticipatory and self- simple change from one activity to
reactive mechanisms” (p. 71; emphasis another poses the fundamental problem
ours). Psychology is currently dominated by for a psychology of motivation. (p. 271)
a primarily cognitive approach, and there is
a covert agreement among most researchers While people tend to pursue a limited num-
that most of the significant thoughts and ber of actions at a time within the behav-
feelings that affect learning achievement are ioural stream (and particularly when sus-
conscious and known by the learner. While tained learning is concerned) various action
we tend to agree with this assumption, there episodes can be simultaneously active. For
are two areas that we feel should be given example, a new action may be initiated while
more attention: the impact of the the success of the previous action is still
individuals’ mood states and the role of being evaluated. This raises the question of
unconscious interpersonal processes. In a whether we can talk about a purely
thought-provoking article, Schwarz and ‘preactional’ phase of the motivational
Bohner (1996) draw attention to the fact that process or whether preactional and actional
depending on what mood people are in, they phases overlap in a complex manner. Bau-
tend to find goals more or less attractive, meister (1996) offered a precise summary of
tend to assess their own resources and the this issue:
situation in a more or less favourable way,
and tend to evaluate their performance as Perhaps, then, the function of mental,
more or less satisfactory. This is, in fact, emotional, and motivational processes
well-known to many classroom practitioners, is not so much to initiate behaviour as
yet little controlled research has been done to steer it—that is, to intervene in on-
to understand the exact nature of such mood- going behavioral processes so as to in-
related biases. With regard to interpersonal terrupt, override, or redirect them…
relations, psychoanalytic theory has Cognitive and motivational processes
provided ample evidence that these are often may guide action in a way that resem-
affected by unconscious ‘scripts’ (such as bles changing the channel on a televi-
transference, projections, defence sion set more closely than it does
mechanisms, etc.; for a review, see Ehrman turning the set on in the first place. (p.
& Dörnyei, 1998). Since we believe that 28)
classroom learning is an intensely interper-
sonal process, unconscious relationship Multiple goals and goal hierarchies.
patterns inevitably influence the class Motivation to learn and learning achieve-
members’ achievement and performance, ment in a school context are the product of a
which warrants further research into this complex set of interacting goals and in-
direction. tentions. For example, as mentioned earlier,
by enrolling in a course, one may want to
Simultaneous action. Our Process acquire knowledge, meet people, and have a
Model appears to suggest that the actional good time at the same time. Such multiple
process occurs in relative isolation, without goals often form hierarchies, including
any interference from other ongoing be- superordinate and subordinate goals,
haviours the actor is engaged in (the only similarly to the need hierarchies by Maslow
indication of this not being the case was (1970) or Murray (1938). In Bandura’s
provided by the inclusion of factors such as (1991) words, “The complementary
goal conflict and competing action tenden- regulation of motivation by hierarchical
cies). This is obviously not true in the strict goals of differential achievability charac-
sense. As Atkinson and Birch (1974) high- terizes most of the strivings of everyday
lighted over two decades ago, life” (p. 101). It is still to be decided how
superordinate and subordinate goals interact
the behavioral life of an individual is a with each other (i.e. override or reinforce
continual stream of thought and action, each other) and how they can simultaneously
characterized by change from one ac- be placed in an action sequence process.
tivity to another, from birth until death.
There are no behavioral vacuums Task-specific motivation. We have
except, of course, when an individual is mentioned earlier that a characteristic
literally unconscious for reasons of feature of school environments is that goals
illness or accident and incapable of and corresponding tasks are not chosen
64 Working Papers in Applied Linguistics

voluntarily by the students but are very often to action. Level of motivation is
assigned to them, and in such cases it makes typically indexed in terms of choice of
more sense to talk about compliance rather courses of action and intensity and
than commitment. This being the case, task- persistence of effort. Attempts to
specific motives may have more significance explain the motivational sources of
in the motivation complex than our model behaviour therefore primarily aim at
suggests. Imposed tasks may be seen not clarifying the determinants and inter-
merely as contributors to the general quality vening mechanisms that govern the
of the learning experience (as our model selection, activation, and sustained di-
suggests) but also as being associated with rection of behaviour toward certain
the general power structure of the classroom goals. (p. 69)
as a social unit (since compliance is
dependent on the perceived power base of Although the two definitions (by Heck-
the authority figure assigning the task). hausen and Bandura) cited above do provide
Although we are sensitive to the significance an appropriate reflection of the complexity
of the social psychological organisation of of motivation, and they also emphasise
the learning environment (cf. Ehrman & certain process-oriented elements, they do
Dörnyei, 1998), because of space limitations not highlight sufficiently the dynamic
we have not gone into details regarding character of motivation in sustained learning
issues such as leadership types, the bases of activities. As outlined in this article, the
social influence, or the manner in which motivational forces that are at work during
leadership is exercised and tasks are the preactional phase accumulate in the
assigned. combined instigation force, the degree of
which determines the intensity of the initial
action commitment. This initial force will be
What is motivation? increased or decreased by additional forces
that come into play during action
engagement, and the postactional evaluation
Having surveyed a great variety of ap- of the actional outcome has a forward
proaches to and aspects of the notion of pointing role as it is contributing to the
‘motivation’, and having set up a construct motivational base of further action. Thus, in
detailing what we see as the main compo- a general sense, motivation can be defined
nents of the motivation complex, it is time to as the dynamically changing cumulative
take stock of what this suggests about the arousal in a person that initiates, directs,
nature and definition of motivation. This is coordinates, amplifies, terminates, and
no easy task if we do not want to restrict the evaluates the cognitive and motor processes
definition to superficial generalisations; in whereby initial wishes and desires are
order to capture the multiple aspects and selected, prioritised, operationalised, and
dimensions represented in Figure 1, we need (successfully or unsuccessfully) acted out.
a relatively complex formulation.
Heckhausen (1991) sees motivation as a
Conclusion
global concept for a variety of proc-
esses and effects whose common core is
the realization that an organism selects The theory presented in this chapter is not
a particular behaviour because of novel in the sense that it offers radically new
expected consequences, and then im- insights. Rather, it is a synthesis that
plements it with some measure of en- attempts to integrate propositions and mod-
ergy, along a particular path. (p. 9) els from several sources into a more com-
prehensive scheme. Our goal was to con-
Separating various levels of motivation, struct a framework which is based on sound
Bandura (1991) provided the following theoretical foundations and which is at the
definition: same time useful for practitioners. We are
aware that the discussion has been highly
Motivation is a general construct linked theoretical and at points speculative. We can
to a system of regulatory mechanisms see two obvious ways to verify the
that are commonly ascribed both propositions made: (a) by formally assessing
directive and activating functions. At the construct validity of the model and (b)
the generic level it encompasses the by testing whether the interventions based
diverse classes of events that move one on this model turn out to be effective in
Zoltán Dörnyei & István Ottó 65

enhancing learner motivation. In our future structures, and student motivation.


research we will proceed along these lines. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84,
Adopting a process model of motivation 267-271.
such as the one described above offers con- Anderman, E. M., & Maehr, M. L. (1994).
siderable potential practical gains. It helps Motivation and schooling. Review of
us to understand the main stages of action Educational Research, 64, 287-309.
initiation and enactment, highlighting the Atkinson, J. W., & Birch, D. (1974). The
forces that can enhance the intensity of the dynamics of achievement-oriented ac-
process. It also describes how various action tivity. In J. W. Atkinson & J. O. Raynor
control mechanisms can consciously be (Eds.), Motivation and achievement (pp.
applied in order to maintain, enhance, and 271-325). Washington, DC: Winston &
protect ongoing action. The model offers a Sons.
unified framework in which the impact of Atkinson, J. W., & Raynor, J. O. (Eds.)
various types of self-regulatory strategies (1974). Motivation and achievement.
(cognitive, metacognitive, affective, and Washington, DC: Winston & Sons.
social) can be interpreted and compared. Bandura, A. (1991) Self-regulation of mo-
Finally, by listing the motivational influ- tivation through anticipatory and self-
ences in a comprehensive manner and by reactive mechanisms. Nebraska Sympo-
specifying which concrete phase of the sium on motivation 1990, 39. 69-164.
motivational process they are related to, the Baumeister, R. F. (1996). Self-regulation
framework can serve as a structures basis for and ego threat: Motivated cognition,
designing motivational strategies to be used self-deception, and destructive goal
in the classroom. setting. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A.
To summarise, the above overview of Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action:
the phases of the motivational process, along Linking cognition and motivation to
with the multiple components energising it, behaviour (pp. 27-47). New York:
attest to the fact that the issue is greatly Guilford Press.
complex. A broad array of mental processes Beckmann, J. (1994). Volitional correlates
and motivational conditions play essential of action versus state orientation. In J.
roles in determining why students behave as Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Volition
they do. Having reviewed the numerous and personality: Action versus state
components, it is difficult to imagine that by orientation (pp. 155-166). Seattle, WA:
focusing on only a few selected factors (as is Hogrefe & Huber.
done in various reductionist paradigms) we Boekaerts, M. (1987). Individual differences
would be able to explain a large enough in the appraisal of learning tasks: An
proportion of variance in motivated learning integrative view of emotion and
behaviour. Therefore, we are in complete cognition. Communication & Cognition,
agreement with Graham’s (1994) concluding 20, 207-224.
words: Boekaerts, M. (1988). Motivated learning:
Bias in appraisals. International Journal
If there is one message I wish to convey of Educational research, 12, 267-280.
with what has been presented in this Boekaerts, M. (1994). Action control: How
chapter, that message is that classroom relevant is it for classroom learning? In
motivational life is complex. No single J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Volition
word or principle such as reinforcement and personality: Action versus state
or intrinsic motivation can possibly orientation (pp. 427-435). Seattle, WA:
capture this complexity. (p. 47) Hogrefe & Huber.
Boekaerts, M. (1995). Self-regulated
learning: Bridging the gap between
metacognitive and metamotivation
References theories. Educational Psychologist, 30,
195-200.
Brophy, J. E. (1987). Synthesis of research
on strategies for motivating students to
Ajzen, I. (1988). Attitudes, personality, and learn. Educational Leadership, 45/2,
behaviour. Chicago: Dorsey Press. 40-48.
Alison, J. (1993). Not bothered? Motivating Brophy, J. (1998). Motivating students to
reluctant language learners in Key learn. Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.
Stage 4. London: CILT. Brophy, J. E., & Good, T. L. (1986).
Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms, goals, Teacher behaviour and student
66 Working Papers in Applied Linguistics

achievement. In M. C. Wittrock (Ed.), commandments for motivating language


rd
Handbook of research on teaching (3 learners: Results of an empirical study.
ed., pp. 328-375). New York: Macmil- Language Teaching Research, 2, 203-
lan. 229.
Brown, H. D. (1994). Teaching by princi- Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (1995). In the
ples. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice mind of the actor: The structure of
Hall. adolescents' achievement task values
Burden, P. R. (1995). Classroom manage- and expectancy-related beliefs. Person-
ment and discipline. New York: Long- ality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
man. 21, 215-225.
Colletta, S. P., Clément, R., & Edwards, H. Ehrman, M. M., & Dörnyei, Z. (1998).
P. (1983). Community and parental in- Interpersonal dynamics in second lan-
fluence: Effects on student motivation guage education: The visible and in-
and French second language profi- visible classroom. Thousand Oaks, CA:
ciency. Québec: International Center for Sage.
Research on Bilingualism. Garcia, T., & Pintrich, P. R. (1994).
Corno, L. (1993). The best-laid plans: Regulating motivation and cognition in
Modern conceptions of volition and the classroom: The role of self-schemas
educational research. Educational Re- and self-regulatory strategies. In D.
searcher, 22, 14-22. Schunk & B. J. Zimmerman (Eds.), Self-
Crookes, G., & Schmidt, R. W. (1991). regulation of learning and per-
Motivation: Reopening the Research formance: Issues and educational ap-
Agenda. Language Learning, 41, 469- plications, (pp. 127-153). Hillsdale, NJ:
512. Lawrence Erlbaum.
Cranmer, D. (1996). Motivating high level Gardner, R. C. (1985). Social psychology
learners. Harlow: Longman. and second language learning: The role
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The of attitudes and motivation. London:
psychology of optimal experience. New Edward Arnold.
York: Harper & Row. Gollwitzer, P. M. (1990). Action phases and
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic mind-sets. In E. T. Higgins & R. M.
motivation and self-determination in Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of
human behaviour. New York: Plenum. motivation and cognition: Foundations
Dibbelt, S. & Kuhl, J. (1994). Volitional of social behaviour (Vol. 2, pp. 53-92).
processes in decision making: Person- New York: Guilford Press.
ality and situational determinants. In J. Good, T. L., & Brophy, J. E. (1994).
Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Volition Looking in classrooms (6th ed.). New
and personality: Action versus state York: HarperCollins.
orientation (pp. 177-194). Seattle, WA: Gottfried, A. E., Fleming, J. S., & Gottfried,
Hogrefe & Huber. A. W. (1994). Role of parental
Dickinson, L. (1995). Autonomy and moti- motivational practices in children’s
vation: A literature review. System, 23, academic intrinsic motivation and
165-174. achievement. Journal of Educational
Dörnyei, Z. (1990). Conceptualizing moti- Psychology, 86, 104-113.
vation in foreign-language learning. Graham, S. (1994). Classroom motivation
Language Learning, 40, 45-78. from an attributional perspective. In H.
Dörnyei, Z. (1994). Motivation and moti- F. O'Neil, Jr. & M. Drillings (Eds.),
vating in the foreign language class- Motivation: Theory and research (pp.
room. Modern Language Journal, 78, 31-48). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erl-
273-284. baum.
Dörnyei, Z. (1997). Psychological processes Green, R. G. (1995). Human Motivation: A
in cooperative language learning: Group social psychological approach. Pacific
dynamics and motivation. Modern Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Language Journal, 81, 482-493. Harter, S. (1992). The relationship between
Dörnyei, Z. (1998). Motivation in second perceived competence, affect, and mo-
and foreign language learning. Lan- tivational orientation within the class-
guage Teaching, 31, 117-135. room: Processes and patterns of change.
Dörnyei, Z. (in preparation). Researching In A. K. Boggiano & T. S. Pittman
and teaching motivation. Harlow: (Eds.), Achievement motivation: A
Addison Wesley Longman. social-developmental perspective (pp.
Dörnyei, Z., & Csizér, K. (1998). Ten 77-114). Cambridge: Cambridge
Zoltán Dörnyei & István Ottó 67

University Press. action control: Mental subsystems,


Heckhausen, H. (1991). Motivation and modes of control, and volitional con-
action. New York: Springer. flict-resolution strategies. In J. Kuhl &
Heckhausen, H., & Kuhl, J. (1985). From J. Beckmann (Eds.), Volition and per-
wishes to action: The dead ends and sonality: Action versus state orientation
short cuts on the long way to action. In (pp. 93-124). Seattle, WA: Hogrefe &
M. Frese & J. Sabini (Eds.), Goal-di- Huber.
rected behaviour: The concept of action Locke, E. A., & Kristof, A. L. (1996). Vo-
in psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence litional choices in the goal achievement
Erlbaum. process. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A.
Jesuíno, J. C. (1996). Leadership: Micro- Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action:
macro links. In E. H. Witte & J. H. Linking cognition and motivation to
Davis (Eds.), Understanding group be- behaviour (pp. 363-384). New York:
haviour: Small group processes and Guilford Press.
interpersonal relations (Vol. 2, pp. 93- Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A
125). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. theory of goal setting and task per-
Jones, V. F., & Jones, L. S. (1995). Com- formance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Pren-
prehensive classroom management: tice Hall.
Creating positive learning environments Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1994). Goal
for all students (4th ed.). Needham setting theory. In H. F. O'Neil, Jr. & M.
Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon. Drillings (Eds.), Motivation: Theory
Kanfer, R. (1996). Self-regulatory and other and research (pp. 13-29). Hillsdale, NJ:
non-ability determinants of skill Lawrence Erlbaum.
acquisition. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Maehr, M. L. (1984). Meaning and moti-
Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: vation: Toward a theory of personal in-
Linking cognition and motivation to vestment. In R. Ames & C. Ames (Eds.),
behaviour (pp. 404-423). New York: Research on motivation in education:
Guilford Press. Student motivation (Vol. 1, pp. 115-
Karniol, R., & Ross, M. (1996). The moti- 144). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
vational impact of temporal focus: Maehr, M. L., & Midgley, C. (1991). En-
Thinking about the future and the past. hancing student motivation: A school-
Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 593- wide approach. Educational Psycholo-
620. gist, 26, 399-427.
Kuhl, J. (1987). Action control: The main- Maslow, A. H. (1970). Motivation and
tenance of motivational states. In F. personality. 2nd ed. New York: Harper
Halish & J. Kuhl (Eds.), Motivation, and Row.
intention, and volition (pp. 279-291). McCombs, B. L. (1994). Strategies for
Berlin: Springer. assessing and enhancing motivation:
Kuhl, J. (1985). Volitional mediators of Keys to promoting self-regulated
cognition-behaviour consistency: Self- learning and performance. In H. F.
regulatory processes and action versus O'Neil, Jr. & M. Drillings (Eds.), Mo-
state orientation. In J. Kuhl & J. tivation: Theory and research (pp. 49-
Beckmann (Eds.), Action control: From 69). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
cognition to behaviour. New York: McCombs, B. L. & Pope, J. E. (1994).
Springer. Motivating hard to reach students.
Kuhl, J. (1992). A theory of self-regulation: Washington, DC: American Psycho-
Action versus state orientation, self- logical Association.
discrimination, and some applications. Murray, H. A. (1938). Explorations in
Applied Psychology: An International personality. Oxford: Oxford University
Review, 41, 97-129. Press.
Kuhl, J. (1994). A theory of action and state Noels, K. A., Clément, R., & Pelletier, L. G.
orientations. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann (in press). Perceptions of teachers'
(Eds.), Volition and personality: Action communicative style and students' in-
versus state orientation (pp. 9-45). trinsic and extrinsic motivation. Modern
Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber. Language Journal.
Kuhl, J., & Beckmann, J. (Eds.) (1994). Oxford, R. L., & Shearin, J. (1994). Lan-
Volition and personality: Action versus guage learning motivation: Expanding
state orientation. Seattle, WA: Hogrefe the theoretical framework. Modern
& Huber. Language Journal, 78, 12-28.
Kuhl, J., & Goschke, T. (1994). A theory of Paris, S. G., & Turner, J. C. (1994). Situated
68 Working Papers in Applied Linguistics

motivation. In P. R. Pintrich, D. R. Ames & C. Ames (Eds.), Research on


Brown & C. E. Weinstein (Eds.), motivation in education: Student
Student motivation, cognition, and motivation (Vol. 1, pp. 15-38). San
learning (pp. 213-237). Hillsdale, NJ: Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Lawrence Erlbaum. Weiner, B. (1992). Human motivation:
Pintrich, P. L., & Schunk, D. H. (1996). Metaphors, theories, and research.
Motivation in education: Theory, re- Nebury Park, CA: Sage.
search, and applications. Englewood Williams, M., & Burden, R. (1997). Psy-
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. chology for language teachers. Cam-
Raffini, J. P. (1993). Winners without los- bridge: Cambridge University Press.
ers: Structures and strategies for in- Winne, P. H. (1995). Inherent details in self-
creasing student motivation to learn. regulated learning. Educational
Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Ba- Psychologist, 30, 173-187.
con. Wong, M. M., & Csikszentmihalyi, M.
Raffini, J. P. (1996). 150 ways to increase (1991). Motivation and academic
intrinsic motivation in the classroom. achievement: The effects of personality
Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Ba- traits and the quality of experience.
con. Journal of Personality, 59, 539-574.
Raynor, J. O., & Roeder, G. P. (1987).
Motivation and future orientation: Task
and time effects for achievement moti-
vation. In F. Halisch & J. Kuhl (Eds.),
Motivation, intention, and volition (pp.
61-71). Berlin: Springer.
Schneider, B., Csikszentmihalyi, M., &
Knauth, S. (1995). Academic challenge,
motivation, and self-esteem: The daily
experience of students in high school. In
M. T. Hallinan (Ed.), Restructuring
schools: Promising practices and
policies (pp. 175-195). New York:
Plenum.
Schumann, J. H. (1998). The neurobiology
of affect in language. Oxford: Black-
well.
Schwarz, N. & Bohner, G. (1996). Feelings
and their motivational implications:
Moods and the action sequence.
Sharan, S., & Shaulov, A. (1990). Coop-
erative learning, motivation to learn,
and academic achievement. In S. Sharan
(Ed.), Cooperative learning: Theory
and research (pp. 173-202). New York:
Praeger.
Slavin, R. E. (1996). Research on coopera-
tive learning and achievement: What we
know, what we need to know. Con-
temporary Educational Psychology, 21,
43-69.
Sorrentino, R. M. (1996). The role of con-
scious thought in a theory of motivation
and cognition: The uncertainty
orientation paradigm. In P. M. Goll-
witzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psy-
chology of action: Linking cognition
and motivation to behaviour (pp. 619-
644). New York: Guilford Press.
Weiner, B. (1984). Principles for a theory of
student motivation and their application
within an attributional framework. In R.
Zoltán Dörnyei & István Ottó 69

Notes

1
An earlier version of this paper was presented
as a keynote address at the Annual Conference
of the Education Section of the British
Psychological Society (September, 1998,
Exeter) by the first author. We are grateful to
Bob Burden, Ben Rampton, Peter Skehan, Sarah
Thurrell, Peter Tomlinson and Marion Williams
for their helpful comments and support.

Вам также может понравиться