Евстифеева
ТЕОРЕТИЧЕСКАЯ ФОНЕТИКА
АНГЛИЙСКОГО ЯЗЫКА
Лекции, семинары, упражнения
1
УДК 811.111’34(075.8)
ББК 81.2Англ-1-923
Е26
Р е ц е н з е н т ы:
докт. филол. наук, профессор, ректор,
Новый гуманитарный институт Монина Т.С.;
канд. филол. наук, доцент кафедры иностранных языков,
Владимирский государственный университет Камайданова Н.А.;
канд. пед. наук, доцент кафедры английского языка,
Московский государственный областной гуманитарный институт Котова Е.Г.
Евстифеева М.В.
Е26 Теоретическая фонетика английского языка. Лекции, семинары,
упражнения : учеб. пособие / М.В. Евстифеева. — М. : ФЛИНТА :
Наука, 2012. — 168 с.
ISBN 978-5-9765-1115-6 (ФЛИНТА)
ISBN 978-5-02-037670-0 (Наука)
В пособии в сжатом виде изложены основные аспекты фонетической
теории и практики английского языка в современной лингвистике;
приведе- на обобщенная информация о компонентах фонетической
системы. Посо- бие содержит лекционную часть и методические
разработки к семинарским занятиям, а также практические упражнения.
Для студентов факультетов иностранных языков педвузов.
УДК 811.111’34(075.8)
ББК 81.2Англ-1-923
1
CONTENTS
Предисловие...................................................................................................................6
От автора........................................................................................................................8
3
3.4. Problem of diphthongs and diphthongoids..........................................45
3.5. Problem of vowel length.....................................................................47
Seminar 3......................................................................................................................49
Lecture 4. MODIFICATIONS AND ALTERNATIONS
OF SPEECH SOUNDS IN THE ENGLISH LANGUAGE........................51
§ 1. Types of sound variations in connected speech.........................................51
§ 2. Modifications of sounds in connected speech............................................52
2.1. Modifications of consonants in connected speech............................52
2.2. Modifications of vowels in connected speech....................................56
2.3. Complex vowel and consonant modifications.....................................57
§ 3. Notion of alternation and its types..............................................................57
§ 4. Problem of phoneme identification. Main phonological schools................60
Seminar 4......................................................................................................................61
4
§ 4. Prosodic components of intonation. Structure
of English intonation patterns.....................................................................91
§ 5. Methods of indicating intonation................................................................95
§ 6. Functions of intonation...............................................................................96
§ 7. Phonological aspect of intonation...............................................................98
§ 8. English rhythm...........................................................................................99
Seminar 7....................................................................................................................100
Bibliography................................................................................................................163
5
Предисловие
7
грамматикой, стилистикой), а также осуществляется взаимосвязь
теоретической и нормативной фонетики. Упражнения составлены с
учетом постепенного нарастания сложности и нацелены на
отработ- ку определенных фонетических явлений, описанных в
соответству- ющих разделах лекционного курса.
Третий раздел содержит дополнительные материалы, включаю-
щие:
— упражнения, направленные на тренировку звуков и
звуковых переходов в системе английского языка,
вызывающих частые трудности у русскоязычных студентов;
— информацию о фонетических модификациях звуков англий-
ского языка в связной речи и их графической репрезента-
ции;
— сведения о способах абстрактной передачи основных инто-
национных контуров английского языка;
— схему последовательного анализа фонетических явлений,
представленных в английских текстах.
В конце книги приведен библиографический список.
Отдельные пункты из данного списка можно рекомендовать
студентам при под- готовке к семинарским занятиям. Количество
рекомендованной ли- тературы зависит от специфики работы и
объема аудиторных часов, отведенных на освоение дисциплины, и
должно определяться самим педагогом.
7
От автора
Дорогие друзья!
Перед вами — учебное пособие по теоретической фонетике ан-
глийского языка, в котором в сжатом виде изложены основные
аспекты фонетической теории и практики. Книга предназначена
для тех, кто хочет не только правильно произносить английские
слова и хорошо интонировать предложения, но также знать и уметь
объяс- нить причины возникновения и существования тех или
иных фоне- тических явлений в изучаемом языке.
Фонетика — одна из важнейших лингвистических наук.
Соглас- но общепринятой иерархии, в лингвистике выделяются
следующие уровни изучения языка: фонетический,
морфологический, лексиче- ский, словосочетания, предложения,
текста. При этом фонетический уровень принимается за базовый,
поскольку он составляет основу любого языка. Образно говоря,
звуки являются своеобразной ‘пи- щей’ языка, из них, как из
кирпичиков, строятся слоги, слова, слово- сочетания и
предложения, реализуя наивысшую человеческую по- требность —
потребность в общении.
Возможно, этот учебник станет Вашим гидом в многообразии
английских звуков, благодаря чему Вы сможете свободно общаться
на изучаемом языке. Понятное и последовательное объяснение
мно- гочисленных фонетических теорий, существующих в
современной лингвистике, поможет Вам овладеть навыками
теоретического ана- лиза языкового материала, которые пригодятся
как при изучении других лингвистических дисциплин, так и в
написании рефератов, курсовых и квалификационных работ.
Знания и умения, полученные Вами, также будут способствовать
профессиональному росту и по- надобятся при обучении уже
Ваших учеников.
7
PARTI
Lectures and
Seminars
9
Lecture 1
INTRODUCTION
§ 1. Phonetics as a science
The word ‘phonetics’ comes from the Greek word fonetika meaning
‘the science of the voice’. Nowadays it means the study of the way
humans make, transmit, and receive speech sounds. Phonetics is an
independent branch of linguistics like lexicology or grammar. These
linguistic sciences study language from three different points of view.
Lexicology deals with the language vocabulary, the origin and
development of words, their mean- ing and word building. Grammar
defines the rules governing the modifica- tion of words and the
combination of words into sentences.
Phonetics is a basic branch of linguistics, which deals with speech
sounds and studies the outer form of the language. Neither linguistic
the- ory nor linguistic practice can exist without phonetics, because
language is a system and its components are inseparably connected.
The connection of phonetics with grammar is exercised through or-
thography and intonation. Thus for example, the system of reading rules
helps to pronounce singular and plural forms of nouns correctly (man —
men, foot — feet). The use of the necessary nuclear tone helps to distin-
guish between different types of sentences. It’s especially important in
colloquial speech where one and the same sentence may be understood
as a statement when pronounced with the falling tone (He came \ home.)
or
a question when pronounced with the rising tone (He came / home?).
The connection of phonetics with lexicology is exercised through pro-
nunciation and word-stress. For instance, some corresponding forms of
verbs and nouns are homographs identical in spelling. They may be
distin- guished with the help of pronunciation (wind [wınd] — to wind
[waınd]), word-stress ('object — to ob'ject), or the combinative use of
word-stress and pronunciation (increase ['ınkrıs] — to increase
[in'kri:z]).
1
The connection of phonetics with stylistics is exercised through
into- national components or graphical expressive means. For example,
repeti-
1
tion of words serves as the basis of rhythm and rhyme; capitalization or
italics highlightning underline special prominence of information:
Look to left and look to right,
Note what traffic is in sight.
Note, too, which light can be seen:
The Red, the Amber, or the Green.
Children, keep from dangerous play
And THINK before you cross today.
Phonetics as a science examines the inventory, structure and func-
tions of speech sounds. On the expression level phoneticians investigate
the sound system of the language: phonemes and their allophones, word
stress, syllabic structure and intonation. On the content level
phoneticians are interested in the analysis and characteristics of phonetic
phenomena and their role in a language. Thus phonetics occupies itself
with the study of the ways of sound organization into a system of units,
variations and functions of these units in all types and styles of spoken
language.
Phonetics is the kind of a science that may have application in
various fields of knowledge besides linguistics. Phonetics is also
connected with non-linguistic sciences which have educational or social
value, like meth- ods of language teaching, logics, history, psychology,
sociology. The study of the structure of sound system is indispensable
from sciences studying different aspects of speech production, like
acoustics, physiology. The con- nection of phonetics with other sciences
is easily observed by the example of its branches. Thus, acoustic
phonetics is related to physics and mathe- matics; articulatory phonetics
— to physiology, anatomy, and anthropolo- gy; historical phonetics —
to general history and archaeology; functional phonetics — to
communication theory and statistics.
Phonetics is also a part of some interdisciplinary subjects like socio-
linguistics, psycholinguistics, mathematical linguistics, etc. Each of
these sciences can have theoretical or practical application in the sphere
of phonetic investigation.
1
Human speech is the result of a highly complicated series of events
that can be divided into 6 stages: psychological, physiological, physical/
1
acoustic, reception, transmission, linguistic interpretation. They are
inter- connected and constitute two parts of the speech act.
I. The first part of the speech act contains the stages made by the
speaker. It includes the following:
1) the psychological stage concerns the formation of the concept in
the brain of a speaker;
2) when the message is formed, it is transmitted along the nervous
system to the speech organs which produce particular speech
sounds within the physiological stage;
3) the movements of the speech apparatus disturb the air and pro-
duce sound waves during the acoustic stage.
II. The second part of the speech act includes the stages made by
the listener, because any communication requires a listener as well as a
speaker:
1) the sound waves are percepted by the listener’s ear within the
reception stage;
2) the spoken message is transmitted through the nervous system to
the listener’s brain during the transmission stage;
3) the information conveyed gets its linguistic interpretation.
The analysis of the process of oral speech production makes it pos-
sible to define four levels of speech production: articulatory, acoustic,
auditory and functional. They are inseparable in the real process of com-
munication. But each of them can be singled out in order to characterize
different aspects of sound phenomena which in their turn are necessary
to define the main branches of pnonetics: articulatory, acoustic, auditory
and functional.
§ 3. Aspects of phonetics
1
tions of the speech organs necessary to pronounce a speech sound.
Speech
1
organs have different functions and thus can be divided into four
groups:
1) The power (respiratory) mechanism supplies the energy in the
form of air pressure and regulates the force of air stream. It in-
cludes the following speech organs: the diaphragm, the lungs,
the bronchi, the windpipe (trachea), the glottis and the supra-
glottal cavities, the larynx, the mouth cavity, the nasal cavity.
2) The vibration mechanism functions as a vibrator when producing
voice. It consists of the vocal cords (voice box), situated in the
larynx.
3) The resonator mechanism consists of the speech organs which
function as principal resonators. These are the pharynx, the
larynx, the mouth and nasal cavities.
4) The obstruction mechanism consists of the tongue (the blade, the
tip, the front, the back/dorsum), the lips, the teeth, the soft palate
with the uvula, the hard palate, the alveolar ridge. These speech
organs form different types of obstructions.
II. The acoustic aspect studies sound waves. It is the way in which
the air vibrates between the speaker’s mouth and the listener’s ear.
There may be different types of vibrations which affect the tone of
the voice. The basic vibrations of the vocal cords over their whole
length produce the fundamental tone of voice. The simultaneous
vibrations of parts of the vocal cords produce partial tones (overtones).
The number of vibrations per second is called frequency. Frequency
of basic vibrations of the vocal cords is called the fundamental
frequency which is very important in phonetic investigation. It
determines the pitch of the voice and forms the acoustic basis of speech
melody.
III. The auditory (sound-perception) aspect is a physiological and
psychological mechanism. It combines the process of hearing with the
process of discriminating sounds. People can perceive the range from 16
to 20,000 Hz with a difference in 3 Hz. The human ear transforms vibra-
tions of the air into nervous commands and transmits them to the brain.
This enables the listener discriminate the quality, pitch, loudness, and
length of sounds and identify the sounds.
IV. The functional (linguistic) aspect is concerned with the
1
linguis- tic function of individual sounds and segments of speech. From
the func-
1
tional point of view all sound phenomena of any language present a
clear- cut system of interdependent units: phonemes, syllables, stress,
and into- nation. These phonetic phenomena have no meaning of their
own. Their linguistic function is to constitute and distinguish larger
meaningful units, such as morphemes, words, phrases, etc.
1
— the acoustic nature of word stress;
— the stress position in disyllabic and polysyllabic words;
— the degrees of word stress.
The fourth component is the intonational structure of utterances with
the following manifestations:
— the prosodic components of intonation;
— the structure of intonation patterns;
— the representation of patterns in intonation groups.
All the components of the phonetic system of the language
constitute its pronunciation.
§ 5. Branches of phonetics
1
into general phonetics and special phonetics.
2
General phonetics studies all the sound-producing possibilities of
the human speech apparatus and the ways they are used for the purpose
of communication.
Special phonetics is based on general phonetics and studies the pho-
netic system of a particular language.
According to the number of languages under study special
phonetics is divided into descriptive and comparative phonetics.
Descriptive phonetics studies the system of pronunciation and pho-
netic units of a single language.
The aim of comparative phonetics is to study the correlation
between phonetic systems of two or more languages.
III. According to the time characteristics of sound phenomena
under study linguists distinguish between historical and contemporary
phonetics.
Historical phonetics traces and establishes the successive changes in
the phonetic system of a given language at different stages of its
development.
The aim of contemporary phonetics is to find and fix the
peculiarities of speech sounds of the language at the present moment of
its existence.
IV. According to the field of application and methods of investiga-
tion phonetics is also divided into theoretical and practical.
Practical phonetics studies the substance, the material form of
phonetic phenomena with the help of different methods of phonetic
analysis.
Theoretical phonetics is mainly concerned with the functions of
pho- netic units in the language and uses methods of phonemic analysis.
All the branches of phonetics are closely connected with one
another and study the language in a set of certain phonetic units
arranged in an orderly way.
2
consist in observing and fixing the movements and positions of one’s
own or other people’s organs of speech in the production of various
speech sounds, as well as in analyzing and comparing one’s own
articula- tory and auditory impressions.
Objective (instrumental) methods of phonetic analysis appeared in
the second half of the XXth century with the development of such
sciences as physiology and physics. They involve the use of various
instrumental tech- niques like palatography, laryngoscopy, X-ray
photography, electromyogra- phy, etc. The use of the data of instrumental
analysis gives a detailed study of different phonetic phenomena and
articulatory processes. It’s quite clear that many instruments, which are
used in analyzing different phonetic phe- nomena, derive from other
sciences. For instance, the articulatory parameters of speech are observed
and fixed with the help of articulograph. The spectra of speech sounds are
investigated by means of sound spectrograph. The pitch component of
intonation is studied with the help of intonograph.
Nowadays practically no area of practical phonetic investigation can
do without the combination of subjective and objective methods when
the results of instrumental analysis supplement those available from
intro- spective analysis.
Seminar 1
2
Lecture 2
MAIN ASPECTS OF FUNCTIONAL PHONETICS
§ 1. Phoneme:
definition and functions
2
E.g., cap [khæph] / [khæp]
— the loss of plosion in the final phoneme [p] doesn’t bring any
change of meaning.
3. The phoneme is also an abstract and generalized unit, which
performs the recognitive function. The phoneme serves to
distinguish and un- derstand the meaning, because the use of the
right allophone in the certain phonetic context helps the listener to
understand the message and thus facilitates normal recognition.
E.g., take it — tape it
— the difference in two phrases is understood by two different pho-
nemes.
This materialistic conception of the phoneme is regarded as the most
suitable for the purpose of language teaching in modern linguistics.
2
— slightly palatalized before front vowels and sonorant [j] (deal,
day, dew);
2
— pronounced without any plosion before another stop (bedtime,
good dog);
— pronounced with nasal plosion before [n], [m] (sudden, admit)
or lateral plosion before [l] (idle);
— post-alveolar followed by [r] (dry, dream);
— dental followed by [θ], [ð] (good thing, lead the way);
— labialized followed by [w] (dweller).
Still all the allophones retain the invariant of phoneme [d] and pos-
sess its three basic articulatory features: they are forelingual lenis stops.
The actual realization of allophones in the speech chain is exercised
through phones. These units are not predicted by phonetic context but
modified by phonostylistic, dialectal and individual variations. That’s
why no speech sounds are absolutely alike.
The relationships between the phoneme as an ideal combination of
articulatory features, the allophone as its variant and the phone as a con-
crete speech sound may be illustrated in the following scheme:
stylistic variation
phoneme allophone dialectal variation phone
individual variation
3
— linking [r] in the expression car owner [karǩunǩ];
— reciprocal influence of sounds [n] and [ð] in the expression in
the yard [ın↔ðǩ ja:d].
The broad transcription is mainly used for practical experience
while the narrow one serves the purposes of research work. In practical
teaching the most important variants of allophones should be mentioned
to teach the students correct pronunciation.
3
phonemes representing types of units completely independent of any
phonetic properties which are higher than the phoneme. This approach
was originated by the Swiss linguist F. de Saussure and advocated by
the Danish linguist L. Hjelmslev and his followers in Copenhagen
Linguistic Circle H.J. Uldall, and K. Togby.
The second and third views are rejected as purely idealistic concep-
tions which do not take into consideration the real human speech.
IV. The physical view regards the phoneme as a family of related
sounds that have phonetic similarity and do not occur in the same pho-
netic context. This conception was proposed by D. Jones and shared by
B. Bloch and G. Trager. The lack of this approach is that it studies the
phoneme from the point of view of its articulatory characteristics only
without any regard to its functional aspects.
V. According to the materialistic view originated by L.V. Shcherba,
the founder of Leningrad phonological school, the phoneme is defined
as a real, independent distinctive unit which has its material
manifestation in the form of allophones. The number of allophones is
much greater than the number of phonemes proper and they are
incapable of differentiating the meaning. This theory was developed by
V.A. Vassilyev, who regard- ed the phoneme as a dialectical unity of
functional, material and abstract aspects, which performs constitutive,
distinctive and recognitive func- tions. This view is widely recognized
in modern phonology, its followers are L.R. Zinder, M.I. Matusevich,
V.A. Vassilyev, M.A. Sokolova and others.
Seminar 2
2
Lecture 3
THE SYSTEM OF ENGLISH PHONEMES
§ 1. Vowel and
consonant phonemes
3
Occlusive-constrictive consonants or affricates [t∫, ʤ] are produced
with a complete obstruction, which is slowly released with friction.
III. The place of articulation is determined by the position of the
active organ of speech. According to it English consonants are divided
into labial, lingual and glottal. The class of labial consonants is further
subdivided into bilabial and labio-dental; the class of lingual — into
fore- lingual, mediolingual and backlingual.
1. Labial consonants are articulated by the lips. This class includes:
— bilabial consonants [p, b, m, w], produced with both lips;
— labio-dental consonants [f, v], articulated with the lower lip
against the upper teeth.
2. Lingual consonants are articulated by the tongue. This class is di-
vided into:
— forelingual consonants [t, d, s, z, ∫, j, θ, ð, t∫, ʤ, n, l, r], produced
with the tip of the tongue;
— mediolingual consonant [j], produced with the front part of the
tongue;
— backlingual (velar) consonants [k, g, ŋ], produced with the back
part of the tongue.
The subclass of forelingual consonants may be grouped into:
— interdental consonants [θ, ð];
— alveolar consonants [t, d, s, z, n, l];
— post-alveolar consonant [r];
— palato-alveolar consonants [t∫, ʤ, ∫, j].
3. Glottal consonant [h] is articulated in the glottis.
It’s necessary to mention, that the number of places of articula-
tion may be different. Thus English consonants are divided into:
— unicentral [p, b, t, d, k, g, f, v, θ, ð, s, z, ∫, j, h, m, n, ŋ, r, l, j],
which have one point of articulation;
— bicentral [t∫, ʤ, w], which have two points of articulation.
IV. The voice characteristic depends on the work of the vocal
cords. According to it English consonants are divided into:
3
— voiced [b, d, g, v, ð, z, ʤ, m, n, ŋ, w, r, l, j];
— voiceless [p, t, k, f, θ, s, ∫, h, t∫].
V. According to the position of the soft palate all consonants are
classified into:
— oral consonants [p, b, t, d, k, g, f, v, θ, ð, s, z, ∫, j, h, t∫, ʤ, w, r, l,
j], produced when the soft palate is raised;
— nasal consonants [m, n, ŋ], produced when the soft palate is low-
ered.
3
M.A. Sokolova suggests another approach and states the degree of
noise to be the first and basic principle of classification. Thus conso-
nants are divided into noise consonants and sonorants because of
great articulatory and acoustic differences between them. The
phono- logical relevance of this factor is proved by contrastive
oppositions: bake [beık] — make [meık], veal [vi:l] — wheel [wi:l].
Each of the classes then undergoes further subdivisions.
consonants
noise
sonorants
consonant
occlusive
occlusiv constrictiv occlusiv constrictive
- latera
e e e
media
3
— bilabial vs. forelingual pan — tan [pæn] — [tæn];
— bilabial vs. backlingual pick — kick [pık] — [kık];
— forelingual vs. mediolingual less — yes [les] — [jes];
— forelingual vs. backlingual day — gay [deı] — [geı];
— forelingual vs. glottal sigh — high [saı] — [haı];
— labio-dental vs. forelingual feet — seat [fi:t] — [si:t], etc.
IV. The voice characteristics in phonological analysis is connected
with the force and energy of articulation rather than with the work of the
vocal cords.
According to it consonants are divided into strong (fortis, voiceless)
and weak (lenis, voiced). The difference is exemplified in distinctive
opposi- tional pairs: cap — cab, not — nod, pick — pig, cap — gap,
pit — bit.
V. The position of the soft palate is not phonologically relevant,
because the presence or absence of nasalization is not used for meaning
differentiation in English. There are no distinctive pairs of consonants
which differ in the position of the soft palate so in phonology this
feature is considered as an indispensable concomitant one.
[a]
3
Sounds [e, o] may be added to them to mark the medium degree of
unlikeness in the acoustic and articulatory characteristics. Thus we get
the most common vowel system with 5 vowels.
[i] [u]
[e] [o]
[a]
1 8
2 7
3 6
4 5
The articulatory changes in this case should be described as follows:
the front part of the tongue raised as close as possible to the palate forms
point of articulation No.1, the gradual lowering of the tongue to the back
lowest posi- tion gives point No.5, the lowest front position of the
tongue gives point No.4, the upper back limit for the tongue position
3
gives point No.8. The tongue positions between these points form points
for No. 2, 3, 6, 7.
4
The system of cardinal vowels is an international standard, but in
spite of great theoretical significance its practical application is limited.
In language teaching this system can be learned only by oral instructions
from a teacher who knows how to pronounce the vowels.
The model pronunciation can be illustrated by the following
examples: 1 [i] — German Biene, Russian пили;
2 [e] — Russian тесть;
3 [ε] — Russian эта;
4 [a] — French la;
5 [α] — English hot;
6 [ɔ] — German
Sonne; 7 [o] — French
Rose;
8 [u] — German gut.
The system of cardinal vowels gets necessary transformations when
applied to a particular language.
The standard of English pronunciation, called Received Pronuncia-
tion or BBC English, contains 20 vowel phonemes [ı, e, æ, a:, ɔ, o:, u,
ʤ, ǩ:, ǩ, i:, u:, eı, aı, ɔı, au, ǩu, ıǩ, εǩ, uǩ].
They may be exemplified by the following words: [ı] ink, [e] net, [æ]
act, [a:] arc, [ɔ] on, [o:] all, [u] put, [] utter, [ǩ:] earn, [ǩ] about, [i:]
neat, [u:] pool, [eı] eight, [aı] my, [au] how, [ɔı] oil, [ıǩ] ear, [εǩ] air, [uǩ]
doer, [ǩu] no.
4
articu- lation: [ı, e, æ, a:, ɔ, o:, u, , ǩ:, ǩ].
4
Diphthongs are complex vowel sounds with unstable articulation in-
cluding an articulatory glide from one position to another: [eı, aı, ɔı,
au, ǩu, ıǩ, εǩ, uǩ]. They consist of two elements: the nucleus with a
strong and distinct articulation which forms the starting point of a
vowel, and the glide which reveals the direction of the sound
change.
Diphthongoids are vowel sounds with a slight change in articulation
when the difference between the starting point and the end of the
sound is not so clear: [i:, u:].
II. The variations in the tongue position also have an effect on the
quality of vowel sounds. They include horizontal and vertical
movements of the tongue.
1. According to horizontal movements of the tongue, vowels are di-
vided into front, front-retracted, central, back, and back-advanced.
Front vowels are produced when the tongue is in the front part of the
mouth and its front is raised to the hard palate: [i:, e, æ], [eı] and the
nucleus of [εǩ].
Front-retracted vowels are pronounced when the tongue is in the
front part of the mouth but slightly retracted: [ı] and the nucleus of
[ıǩ, aı, au].
Central vowels are produced when the tongue is in the central part
of the mouth and its front is raised to the back part of the hard
palate: [ʤ, ǩ:, ǩ] and the nucleus of [ǩu].
Back vowels are pronounced when the tongue is in the back part of
the mouth and its back is raised to the soft palate [ɔ, o:, u:, a:] and
the nucleus of [ɔı].
Back-advanced vowels are produced when the tongue is in the back
part of the mouth but slightly advanced and its centre is raised to the
soft palate: [u] and the nucleus of [uǩ].
2. According to vertical movements of the tongue, vowels are divided
into close, mid, and open.
Close (high) vowels are produced when the front or back of the
tongue is raised high to the palate: [i:, ı, u, u:].
4
Open (low) vowels are pronounced when the front or back of the
tongue is at the lowest position: [æ, a:, ɔ, o:].
Mid (half-open) vowels are produced when the raised part of the
tongue is between the close and open positions [e, , ǩ:, ǩ].
Each of these vertical tongue positions has two variants: broad and
narrow, which include a higher or lower position of articulation
with- in one of the levels. Thus a more precise classification
includes the following groups:
— close narrow vowels [i:, u:];
— close broad vowels [ı, u];
— mid narrow vowels [e, ǩ:];
— mid broad vowels [, ǩ];
— open narrow vowel [o:];
— open broad vowels [æ, a:, ɔ].
The nuclei of diphthongs are always pronounced within broad
variants.
III. According to the position of the lips, vowels may be rounded
or unrounded.
Rounded vowels are produced when the lips are drawn together with
a round opening between them: [ɔ, o:, u, u:].
Unrounded vowels are pronounced when the lips are neutral or
spread: [i:, ı, e, æ, a:, , ǩ:, ǩ].
IV. According to the length the classes of long and short vowels are
distinguished:
long vowels are capable of being continued during a longer period of
time [i:, a:, o:, ǩ:, u:];
short vowels — during a shorter period of time [i, e, ɔ, u, ǩ, ].
The vowel sound [æ] stands apart from this category because it’s
relatively long.
V. The degree of tenseness which distributes vowels into tense and
lax is closely connected with vowel length.
4
Tense vowels are produced when the speech organs are tense, here
belong all English long vowels: [i:, a:, o:, ǩ:, u:].
Lax vowels are produced with less tenseness of the speech organs,
here belong all English short vowels: [i, e, ɔ, u, ǩ, , æ].
VI. English vowels are also classified according to the character of
the end into checked and free (unchecked). This criterion is connected
with the quality of vowel sounds under the influence of word stress.
Checked vowels are pronounced with maximum force of utterance
and have a strong end. They are abruptly interrupted by the
following consonant and therefore occur only in closed syllables.
These are English stressed vowels followed by a strong voiceless
consonant (bet [bet], dock [dɔk], cart [ka:t], tape [teıp], teacher
[‘ti:t∫ǩ]).
Free vowels are pronounced with lessening force of utterance and
have a weak end. Here belong English vowels followed by a weak
voiced consonant or no consonant at all (pull [pul], card [ka:d],
tame [teım], try [traı], illusion [ı’lu:jn]).
4
Russian scholars single out the criterion of the stability of articula-
tion, according to which vowels are subdivided into:
— monophthongs with stable tongue position;
— diphthongs with unstable articulation which implies gradual
glide of the tongue from one position to another;
— diphthongoids, with relatively unstable articulation which
implies a slight glide only.
The classification suggested by Russian linguists is more exact from
the articulatory point of view and thus it is more suitable for teaching
purposes. Yet the phonemic status of diphthongs and diphthongoids
causes much argument.
II. The position of the tongue in the mouth cavity is the criterion
acknowledged as phonologically relevant by all linguists. Still the clas-
sifications suggested by Russian and foreign scientists have
considerable meaningful differences.
According to the horizontal movements of the tongue our phoneti-
cians distinguish five classes of vowels: front, front-retracted, central,
back, back-advanced. Foreign phoneticians distinguish only three
classes: front, central and back.
The classification of English vowels according to the vertical move-
ments of the tongue is also variable. British scholars distinguish three
classes of vowels: high, mid and low. Russian phoneticians make this
classification more detailed and distinguish two subclasses in each class,
all in all constituting six classes: broad and narrow variations of close,
mid and open vertical positions.
The controversy in the treatment of this criterion naturally leads to
different views on the next criterion — the length of vowels.
III. The distribution of vowels according to their length into long
and short from the articulatory point of view is stated by all linguists.
The antagonism of foreign and home linguists lies in the field of
phonology. British and American phoneticians consider vowel length to
be an essential phonemic feature whereas Russian scientists don’t
treat it as phonologically relevant. They underline that physical
duration of a vow- el in connected speech depends on many factors and
doesn’t always serve
as the only distinctive feature.
4
The explanation of such a considerable difference in the approaches
to the second and third criteria is quite simple. In fact, the criteria of
tongue position and vowel length are interconnected from the point of
view of their functional significance. Foreign linguists do not single out
the classes of front-retracted and back-advanced vowels when analyzing
the horizontal movements of the tongue. They also do not distribute vo-
wels into broad and narrow variants when dealing with the vertical
move- ments of the tongue. So the number of vowel classes
distinguished on the basis of the tongue position is fairly smaller which
results in different views on the criterion of vowel length.
This can be clearly illustrated by the example of vowels [i:, ı, u:, u].
According to the approach of Russian linguists, they belong to the
same vowel classes, but differ in subclases:
[i:] front, close narrow vowel — [ı] front-retracted, close broad
vowel;
[u:] back, close narrow vowel — [u] back-advanced, close broad
vowel.
Therefore the distinction of minimal pairs like Pete [pi:t] — pit [pıt],
pool [pu:l] — pull [pul] is made with the help of functional features
based on different positions of the tongue. Thus the length of vowels is
not considered to be relevant.
In foreign linguistics the classification of vowels according to the
tongue position is not so precise. Therefore both [i:] and [ı] are classed
as front vowels, both [u:] and [u] — as back ones. In this case word-
meaning in oppositions like beat [bi:t] — bit [bıt], seat [si:t] — sit [sıt]
can be dif- ferentiated only with the help of vowel length which should
be taken into consideration as a phonologically relevant factor.
IV. The traditional classification of vowels according to the lip
position into spread, neutral and rounded, may be reduced to two posi-
tions: rounded and unrounded.
Still lip rounding is not phonologically relevant because it takes
place only due to physiological reasons. From the phonological point of
view lip rounding is caused by different positions of the tongue. Any
back vowel is pronounced with lip rounding and the degree of rounding
de- pends on the height of the raised part of the tongue.
4
V. The degree of vowel checkness or the character of vowel end
concerns the quality of vowels in stressed syllables under the influence
of the following consonant.
According to it all English long vowels are free as their
pronunciation doesn’t depend on the next consonant phoneme.
The pronunciation of English short vowels is checked when they are
stressed. The degree of checkness is terminated by the following conso-
nant: it is greater before a voiceless consonant and smaller before a
voiced one or a sonorant.
But this characteristic has no phonological value and it is important
only for practical application in language teaching.
VI. The degree of tenseness characterizes the state of the organs of
speech at the moment of vowel production. Special instrumental
analysis shows that long vowels are tense while short ones are lax. This
characte- ristic is also non-phonological and it is used only in teaching
practice.
The criteria of vowel checkness and tenseness are phonologically
non-relevant, because they are realized only in connection with other
phonetic phenomena, namely the syllabic structure and the word stress.
Thus in the word pity ['pıtı] the sounds [ı] in the first and second
syllables have different degrees of checkness and tenseness because of
the diffe- rences in their placement and accentuation.
5
posite monophonemic entities? Modern linguistics uses a complex ap-
proach to the solution of this problem.
According to the rules of articulatory indivisibility N.S. Trubetskoy
states that diphthongs are unisyllabic, because:
— their parts can’t belong to different syllables;
— they present one phoneme with gliding articulation;
— their length doesn’t exceed the length of a single phoneme.
According to the criterion of morphological indivisibility added by
L.R. Zinder English diphthongs can‘t be separated, because they belong
to one morpheme: buy [baı] — buyer ['baı-ǩ]. Thus English diphthongs
differ from Russian biphonemic combinations like [ай, ой]: чай [чай]
— чаю [ча-йу], стой [стой] — стою [сто-йу].
Taking this information into consideration, phoneticians grant
mono- phonemic status to the English diphthongs on the basis of
articulatory, morphonological and syllabic indivisibility combined with
the criterion of duration:
— English diphthongs are pronounced within a single articulatory
effort;
— neither morpheme nor syllable boundary can separate the
nucleus and the glide (saying ['seı-ıŋ], crying ['kraı-ıŋ], enjoying
[ın- 'djOı-ıŋ], slower ['slǩu-ǩ], ploughing ['plau-ıŋ], clearer
['klıǩ-rǩ], airing ['εǩ-rıŋ], poorer ['puǩ-rǩ]);
— the duration of diphthongs coincides with the one of long
monoph- thongs in the same phonetic context (site [saıt] — seat
[si:t], coat [kǩut] — caught [ko:t]).
With the help of commutation test V.A. Vassilyev shows that any
diphthong can form oppositions with practically all vowels and thus de-
fines the monophonemic status of diphthongs (bite — bit [baıt — bıt];
bite — but [baıt — bt]; bite — bought [baıt — bo:t]; etc.).
The monophonemic character of English diphthongs is also proved
by native speakers’ intuition who perceive these sound complexes as a
single unit element.
Besides diphthongs Russian linguists also define such a subclass of
English vowels as diphthongoids on the basis of slight articulatory insta-
5
bility in the pronunciation of [i:, u:] which becomes gradually stronger
in modern English.
The division of English vowels into monophthongs, diphthongs and
dipthongoids is very important for language teaching since there are no
such sounds in Russian. Russian sound combinations like [йа, йо, йу,
ой, ай, ау, уа] (яд, йод, юг, рой, край, мяукать, вуаль) are
biphonemic clusters of two vowels or a vowel and the sonorant [й],
when both ele- ments are pronounced with equal energy and distinction.
So special attention should be given to pronunciation teaching of
English diphthongs, presenting a phonemic entity of two elements, the
first of them being a strong and distinct nucleus and the second — a
weak and indistinct glide. The pronunciation of diphthongoids
characterized by a certain degree of instability, which is greater in
comparison with monophthongs and smaller in comparison with
diphthongs, also requires special attention.
5
But the problem phonology investigates is whether variations in
quantity are meaningful and thus can be treated as a relevant feature
when characterizing the system of English vowels.
Foreign scholars usually follow the approach of an outstanding
British phonetician D. Jones who underlines the phonological relevance
of vo- wel quantity. He states that words may be distinguished from one
another with the help of oppositions of different vowel length called
chronemes (deed — did, fool — full).
An outstanding Russian phonetician V.A. Vassilyev objects to this
point of view and considers that the difference in the quantity of vowels
should be subordinate to the difference in their quality. This conclusion
is based on two laws characterizing any language system:
(1) a relevant feature must characterize a number of units;
(2) a feature is systemic if it does not depend on the context.
The first law can be proved with the help of distinctive oppositions
containing vowels of different length. Most English vowels are
character- ized by the predominance of other distinctive features besides
quantita- tive correlation:
— in [i:, u:] vs. [ı, u] — diphthongoids vs. monophthongs;
— in [ǩ:] vs. [ǩ] — stressed vocalism (a vowel seldom occurs in
unstressed syllables) vs. unstressed vocalism (a vowel never oc-
curs in stressed syllables);
— in [a:] vs. [] — back open vs. central mid characteristics.
This gives the ground not to treat vowel length as a phonologically
relevant feature.
The second law shows that besides a great deal of other factors the
absolute length of vowels greatly depends on phonetic context. Long
vowels are the longest in terminal positions (bee, bar), they are shorter
before voiced consonants (bead, hard), and the shortest before voiceless
consonants (beet, cart). Still the words like bit and beat are perceived as
different, because vowels differ in quality: [ı] is a front retracted pure
monophthongs whereas [i:] is a diphthongized vowel.
So vowel length can’t be considered a minimal distinctive feature
since it varies under the influence of different phonetic context and
serves as an incidental feature characterizing vowel sounds of a certain
quality.
5
Such an approach to phonological relevance of the quantity of English
vowels is shared by most Russian and many British phoneticians.
The problem of vowel length also concerns the status of phoneme
[æ]. It is treated as a historically short vowel that tends to be lengthened
before lenis consonants [b, d, g, m, n, z] almost the same as long
vowels. Nowadays the most part of phoneticians considers that [æ]
belongs to the subclass of long vowels on the basis of its qualitative —
quantitative rela- tions in the opposition [æ] vs. [ǩ].
Seminar 3
5
9. Give an overview of the articulatory classification of vowels com-
pared to their phonological classification. Discuss the relevance of
the following points:
a) the stability of articulation;
b) the tongue position;
c) the vowel length;
d) the lip position;
e) the vowel tenseness and the character of the vowel end.
10. What criteria — articulatory or phonological — are more suitable
in the view of teaching pronunciation? Why?
11. State the essence of the problem of affricates in phonology. What
questions should be solved in connection with this problem?
12. What approaches regarding the problem of affricates exist in home
and foreign linguistics? Which one is the most suitable in your
opinion? Why?
13. State the essence of the problem of diphthongs in phonology. What
questions should be solved in connection with this problem?
14. Discuss the nature of English diphthongs and prove their
monopho- nemic character.
15. What can you say concerning the problem of diphthongoids?
16. Discuss the importance of the division of English vowels into
monophthongs, diphthongs and dipthongoids in connection with
language teaching.
17. Give an account of factors that can influence the quantity of the
vowel.
18. Can vowel length differentiate the meaning? Why does this
question constitute the subject of controversy among home and
foreign lin- guists?
19. What are the laws characterizing any language system according to
V.A. Vassilyev? How does this statement explain the connection
between the quantity and quality of vowels.
20. What can you say concerning the status of the phoneme [æ]?
5
Lecture 4
MODIFICATIONS AND
ALTERNATIONS OF SPEECH SOUNDS
IN THE ENGLISH LANGUAGE
5
Merging of stages usually takes place if two sounds of a different
nature are joined together: vowels and consonants, noise consonants and
sonorants, etc. In this case the end of the preceding sound penetrates
into the beginning of the following sound and they are articulated
almost si- multaneously (law [lo:]).
Interpenetration of stages usually takes place when consonants of a
similar or identical nature are joined together. In this case the end of the
first sound penetrates not only into the beginning but also into the
middle of the second sound (act [ækt], begged [begd]).
Sound variations are caused by different types of phonetic units:
seg- mental or suprasegmental.
Combinative changes are conditioned by segmental units and result
in the reciprocal influence of neighbouring sounds (tune [tju:n], in the
[in ðǩ]).
Positional changes are conditioned by suprasegmental units and re-
sult in the stylistic and intonational influence on sounds (word combina-
tions slight pressure, hot muffins may sound in colloquial speech like
['slaip 'pre∫ǩ], ['hp 'mfnz]).
The majority of sound variations in connected speech are combina-
tive, they may influence either phonemic or allophonic composition of a
word.
Phonemic variations are generally termed ‘sound alternations’.
They include changes between related phonemes and have great phono-
logical value.
Allophonic variations in the phonetic sequence are called ‘sound
mo- difications’. They are very important for practical language
teaching.
5
2.1. Modifications of consonants in connected speech
Consonants are characterized by the following types of sound modi-
fications: assimilation, accommodation, elision, and inserting.
I. Assimilation is the adaptive modification of a consonant by a
neighbouring consonant within a speech chain. There are different types
of assimilation.
1. According to the direction of sound modification assimilation is di-
vided into:
— progressive (dogs — voiced [z], cats — voiceless [s]);
— regressive (width — [d] becomes dental);
— reciprocal (tree — [t] becomes post-alveolar, [r] is partly de-
voiced).
2. According to the degree of sound modification assimilation can be:
— complete, when two sounds become completely alike or merge
into one another (sandwich ['sænnwıʤ] → ['sænwıʤ] →
['sænıʤ]);
— incomplete, when the adjoining sounds are partially alike (sweet
[w] is partially devoiced).
These types of assimilation may result in different modifications of
the place of articulation, the manner of articulation, and the force of ar-
ticulation.
1) Assimilation affecting the place of articulation includes the
following modifications of consonants:
— alveolar [t, d, n, l, s, z] become dental before interdental [ð, θ]
(eighth, breadth, on the, all the, guess that, does that);
— alveolar [t, d] become post-alveolar before post-alveolar [r] (true,
dream);
— alveolar [s, z] become post-alveolar before apical forelingual [∫]
(this shelf, does she);
— alveolar [t, d] become fricative before palatal mediolingual [j]
(graduate, congratulate);
— nasal [m, n] become labio-dental before labio-dental [f, v] (com-
fort, infant);
5
— nasal [n] becomes dental before interdental [θ] (seventh);
— nasal [n] becomes velar before backlingual [k] (think);
— nasal [n] becomes palato-alveolar before palato-alveolar [t∫, ʤ]
(pinch, change).
2) Assimilation affecting the manner of articulation includes the
follow- ing modifications of consonants:
— loss of plosion in the sequence of two stops [p, t, k, b, d, g] (and
dad, that tape, fact) or in the sequence of a stop and an affricate
(a pointed chin, a sad joke);
— nasal plosion in the combination of a plosive consonant and a
nasal sonorant (sudden, happen, at night, submarine, let me);
— lateral plosion in the sequence of an occlusive consonant and a
lateral sonorant (settle, please, apple);
— anticipating lip-rounded position in the combination of conso-
nants [t, d, k, g, s] and a sonorant [w] (quite, swim, dweller).
3) Assimilation affecting the work of the vocal cords includes the fol-
lowing modifications of consonants:
— progressive partial devoicing of the sonorous [m, n, l, w, r, j] be-
fore voiceless [s, p, t, k, f, θ, ∫] (small, slow, place, fly, sneer, try,
throw, square, twilight, pure, few, tune, at last, at rest);
— progressive voicing or devoicing of the contracted forms of the
auxiliary verbs is, has depending on the preceding phoneme
(That’s right. Jack’s gone. John’s come.);
— progressive voicing or devoicing of the possessive suffixes -’s /
-s’, the plural suffix -(e)s of nouns or the third person singular
ending -(e)s of verbs according to the phonetic context (Jack’s,
Tom’s, Mary’s, George’s; girls, boys, dishes, maps; reads,
writes, watches);
— progressive voicing or devoicing of the suffix -ed depending on
the preceding sound (lived, played, worked);
— regressive voicing or devoicing in compound words (gooseberry,
newspaper);
6
— regressive voicing or devoicing in closely connected pairs of
words, which usually include two functional words or a
combina- tion of a notional and a functional word (I have to do
this. She’s fine. Of course.).
It’s important to mention that English consonants are not subjected
to voiced-voiceless or voiceless-voiced assimilation within non-
compound words (anecdote, birthday, obstinate) or in free combinations
of two no- tional words (sit down, this book, these socks, white dress).
II. Accommodation is the adaptive modification of a consonant un-
der the influence of a neighbouring vowel which includes the following
changes:
— labialization of consonants under the influence of the following
back vowels [ɔ, o:, u, u:, a:], resulting in lip rounding (pool,
rude, ball, car);
— labialization of consonants under the influence of the following
or preceding front vowels [ı, i:], resulting in lip spreading (tea
— eat, feet — leaf , keep — leak, pill — tip);
— palatalization of consonants under the influence of front vowels
[ı, i:] (cf: part — pit, top — tip, far — feet, hard — hit, chance
— cheese).
III. Elision is a complete loss of sound in the word structure in con-
nected speech. The following examples of consonant elision are
observed in modern English:
— loss of [h] in personal and possessive pronouns he, his, her, hers
and the forms of the auxiliary verb have (What has he done?);
— loss of [l] when preceded by [o:] (always);
— loss of plosives [p, t, k, b, d, g] in clusters followed by another
consonant (next day, just one, last time, old man);
— loss of [θ, ð] in clusters with [s, z, f, v] (months, clothes, fifth, sixth);
— loss of [v] before other consonants in rapid speech (give me your
pen).
IV. Insertion is a process of sound addition to the word structure.
There are the following cases of this consonant modification type in
English:
6
— linking [r], which reveals its potential pronunciation
(carowner);
— intrusive [r] pronounced in word combinations with vowels in
the word-final and word-starting positions (chinaand glass);
— inserted [j] after word-final diphthongs gliding to [ı] (saying,
trying);
— inserted [w] after word-final diphthongs gliding to [u] (going,
al- lowing);
— inserted [t∫, ʤ] instead of word-final [t, d] before [j] (could
you).
6
II. Vowel elision (zero reduction) is the complete omission of the
unstressed vowel which is realized in connected speech under the influ-
ence of tempo, rhythm and style of speech. It usually occurs:
— in notional words within a sequence of unstressed syllables (his-
tory ['hıstǩrı] → ['hıstrı], territory ['terıtǩrı] → ['terıtrı]);
— in notional words within unstressed syllables preceding the
stressed one (correct [kǩ'rekt] → [k'rekt], suppose [sǩ'pǩuz] →
[s'pǩuz]);
— in unstressed form words within a phrase (Has he done it? [hæz hi· /
dn ıt] → [hǩz hı dn ıt] → [ǩz ı dn ıt] → [z ı dn ıt]).
/ / /
6
I. Sound alternations that are traced back to the phonemic changes
in earlier periods of language development and are known as historical.
In this case the alternating sounds are affected not by the present-day
pho- netic position or context but by certain diachronic processes which
reveal sound changes made in the course of language history. They are
now- adays reflected in English as alternations of phonemes used for
differen- tiating words, their derivatives and grammatical word-forms.
Historical alternations mark both vowels and consonants. They usually
have certain orthographic representation and may be supported by
suffixation and stress shifting.
1. Vowel alternations are exemplified by:
— distinctions of irregular verbal forms (get — got — got, know —
knew — known);
— distinctions of causal verbal forms (to rise — to raise);
— distinctions of singular and plural noun forms (goose — geese,
man — men);
— distinctions of parts of speech in etymologically correlated
words (long — length).
2. Consonant alternations represent:
— distinctions of irregular verbal forms (send — sent — sent);
— distinctions of parts of speech in etymologically correlated
words (defence — to defend);
— reduction of consonant clusters in the initial (write, know, gnat),
medial (listen, whistle) or final positions (lamb).
3. Vowel and consonant alternations are presented by distinctions of
parts of speech in etymologically correlated words (live — life,
bath — to bathe).
II. Sound alternations on the synchronic level are known as contex-
tual or contemporary. They concern the phonemic structure of mor-
phemes under the influence of other morphemes joined to them. Such
phonemic changes do not have any spelling representation and characte-
rize sounds in weak positions, namely unstressed positions for vowels
and final or pre-consonantal position for consonants.
6
The study of contextual alternations differs from the study of sound
modifications. The latter is mainly connected with the articulatory and
acoustic aspects of sound phenomena whereas the first one deals with
phonology and touches upon the problem of phoneme identification of
alternated sounds in weak positions.
Let’s consider the following example. If we take the first syllabic
vowel of the words ac'tivity and con'trast and compare it with the first
syllabic vowel of the words 'active and 'contrast, we’ll clearly see the
difference in sound representation. It is the weak position of a vowel in
the first case and its strong position in the second one.
But the question is in defining the phonemic status of the vowel in
its weak position. There are two possible variants when in the words
ac'tivity and con'trast the first syllabic vowel may be considered:
— either as the principle allophone of a neutral phoneme [ǩ];
— or as subsidiary allophone of [æ] and [ɔ] in the words 'active and
'contrast, correspondingly.
The difference is quite significant, because the sound [ǩ] may be
iden- tified either as an independent phoneme, or as a neutralized
allophone of some other phoneme. This problem still doesn’t get a
single decision in modern linguistics.
Yet in case of the English language the problem of contextual
alterna- tions and phoneme identification is said to be not so important.
Numerous phonetic simplifications of units larger than phonemes
manifested in con- nected speech don’t seem to affect the meaning of
English sentences. Omissions of speech sounds made in this or that
word for the sake of the economy of pronouncing efforts do not lead to
excessive ambiguity.
For example, the auxiliary verbs have and be, in the 3rd person singu-
lar (has, is) reduced to a single sound [z] are properly recognized by the
listener because of their syntactic function in the context. So the sound
sequences [z 'nik 'kmiŋ] or [z 'nik 'km] are easily reconstructed as ‘Is
Nick coming?’ or ‘Has Nick come?’
The same is with the possessive -’s and the plural -s of nouns pro-
nounced as [z]. In the sound sequences [ðǩ 'bɔız 'skeıt] or [ðǩ 'bɔız 'pen]
the sound [z] is correspondingly recognized as the plural or possessive
6
forms of a noun: ‘The boys skate’ or ‘The boy’s pen’.
6
The problem of phoneme identification is far more significant for
the Russian language because of the widely spread voiced — voiceless
pho- nemic consonant assimilation and vowel reduction.
6
port another view and advocate the autonomy of the phoneme and its
independence from the morpheme. They state that allomorphs of a
single morpheme may differ from each other on the synchronic level not
only in their allophonic, but also in their phonemic composition. The
content of the morpheme is constant. Speech sounds in phonologically
neutral posi- tions belong to that phoneme with whose principal variant
they com- pletely or nearly coincide. Thus:
— in ‘вода’ the first vowel sound should be assigned to phoneme [a];
— in ‘кот — код’ the sound in question belongs to phoneme [т].
III. According to the representatives of Prague phonological
school (N.S. Trubetzkoy, R. Jacobson, and others), there are types of
units higher and broader than phonemes: the so-called ‘archiphonemes’.
An archipho- neme represents a combination of distinctive features
common to two different phonemes excluding their specific features. So
in ‘кот — код’ the sound in question is neither [т] nor [д] but an
abstract unit combining their voiceless-fortis and voiced-lenis
characteristics and making them similar in neutral positions.
It should be mentioned that none of these conceptions is recognized
as ideal in modern linguistics.
Seminar 4
7
Lecture 5
SYLLABIC STRUCTURE OF ENGLISH WORDS
7
open vowels the most sonorous
close vowels
sonorants voiced
fricatives voiced
plosives
voiceless fricatives
voiceless plosives the least sonorous
p l a: n t
The most serious drawback of this theory is that many English syl-
lables contradict it. For example, in this case a sound sequence like
stops [stops] should have three syllables instead of the actual one.
Further experimental work resulted in a lot of other theories, but the
question of the articulatory and acoustic mechanism of syllable
formation is still open in phonetics. It might be fair to suppose that this
mechanism is similar in all languages and can be regarded as a phonetic
and physio- logical universal.
The theory of muscular tension by L.V. Shcherba has prevailed for
a long time in Russian linguistics. It states that the syllabic peak in most
languages is formed with the help of a vowel or sometimes a sonorant,
and the phonemes preceding or following the peak are marginal. The
syl- lable is defined as an arc of muscular tension in which the tension
of ar- ticulation increases within the range of prevocalic consonants and
then decreases within the range of postvocalic consonants. This theory
has been further modified by V.A. Vassilyev, who suggested that the
physical parameters of pitch, intensity and length also vary within the
range of the syllable. So on the speech production level the syllable can
be treated as
7
an arc of articulatory effort which combines the changes in the muscular
tension of articulation and the acoustic data.
p l a: n t
Still all the theories mentioned above analyze the syllable either on
production or perception levels. An outstanding Russian linguist and
psy- chologist N.I. Zhinkin has made an attempt to combine these levels
of analysis in the so-called loudness theory. His experiments showed
that the loudness of sounds depends on the variations of the pharyngeal
pas- sage modified by the narrowing of its walls. Thus the increase of
muscu- lar tension results in the increase of actual loudness of a sound.
So on the perception level the syllable is the arc оf loudness which
correlates with the arc of articulatory effort on the production level,
since variations in loudness are due to the work of all speech
mechanisms.
Speaking about the definition of the syllable, it is perfectly obvious
that no phonetician has so far succeeded in it. The attempts to define the
concept of the syllable resulted in the existence of different approaches.
Some linguists treat the syllable as a purely articulatory unit
universal for all languages, which lacks any functional value, because
its boundar- ies do not always coincide with those of morphemes.
Still the majority of linguists regard the syllable as the smallest pro-
nounceable unit with a certain linguistic function which refers to the
structure of a particular language. In this case the definition of the syl-
lable tends to single out the following features:
— a syllable is a chain of phonemes of varying length;
— a syllable is constructed on the basis of the contrast of its con-
stituents, usually of vowel-consonant (VC) type;
— the nucleus of a syllable is a vowel, but there are languages in
which this function is performed by a consonant;
— the presence of consonants in a syllable is optional;
— the distribution of phonemes in the syllable follows the rules of
a particular language.
7
Thus the definition of the syllable presents a sum of features charac-
teristic of this suprasegmental unit.
7
(4) syllables covered at the end, in which a consonant or a sequence of
con- sonants follow a vowel (VC(CC) type): on [ɔn], old [ǩuld], acts
[ækts].
Certain peculiarities of the system of English phonemes admit the
existence of types of syllables which consist of consonants only, with
the nucleous [l, n, m]. These are:
(1) syllables of CC type (table [teıbl], taken [teıkn], rhythm [rıðm]);
(2) syllables of CCC type (decent [di:snt], seldom [seldm]);
(3) syllables of CCCC type (students [stju:dnts];
(4) syllables of CCCCC type (functional [fŋk∫nl]).
The distribution of consonant sounds in the structure of syllables is
fixed by certain rules and restrictions:
— sonorants [w, j] are always syllable initial (wheel [wi:l], yes [jes]);
— sonorants [n, l, m] are syllabic only in unstressed final positions
if preceded by a noise consonant (bottle [bɔtl], bottom [bɔtm],
button [btn]);
— [s] is always initial in the syllables of CCCV type (straw
[stro:]);
— [s/z] are usually included in the syllables of VC(CC) type as
mor- phological indexes of the plural form of nouns or 3rd
person sin- gular form of verbs (casks [ka:sks], asks [a:sks]);
— [r] becomes syllabic in some accents (perhaps [præps]);
A single generalized formula of English syllables can be
summarized as follows: (C)V/C(C). The brackets indicate the optional
presence of con- sonants. This formula shows that the syllabic structure
of the English lan- guage consists of a nucleus which may be
accompanied with consonants.
The nucleus is the peak of the syllable which is presented by a
vowel or a sonorous consonant. Consonant(s) preceding the nucleus
make up the syllable onset. Consonant(s) following the nucleus make up
the syl- lable coda. The combination of the nucleus and the coda makes
up the rhyming property of a syllable.
The structure of the English syllable admits from 1 to 3 pre-vocalic
consonants (splay [spleı]) and from 1 to 5 post-vocalic consonants (min-
7
strels ['mınstrlz]).
7
The number of syllables in English words can vary from 1 to 8 (day
[deı], baby ['beıbı], family ['fæmılı], generation [ ʤenǩ'reı∫n], liberality
[ lıbǩ'rælıtı], responsibility [rıs ponsǩ'bılıtı], ' irresponsibility
'
[ırıs ponsǩ'bılıtı], incomprehensibility [ın'komprı hensǩ'bılıtı].
' basis of syllable formation in the English language is the open
The
type of syllable in case of long or diphthongized vowels, and the closed
type of syllable in case of short vowels. This statement brings us to the
point of syllable division.
7
The difficulty is to find the point of syllable division. It greatly de-
pends on the number of consonant phonemes following the vowel.
1) When a short stressed vowel is followed by one consonant, there
are two possibilities to determine the point of syllable division.
It may be after or inside the intervocalic consonant: city ['sıt-ı]
or ['sıŧı]. The results of instrumental analysis show that the point
of syllable division in such words is inside the intervocalic
conso- nant. It can be marked in transcription by putting a
syllable boun- dary after the consonant serving as the point of
syllable division and adding an apostrophe sign to the next
consonant in order to fill the gap in notation: ['sıt-’ı].
2) When a short stressed vowel is followed not by a single conso-
nant, but by a consonant cluster, the rule for syllable division is
different. In words like extra there may be several possibilities
to determine syllable boundaries: ['ek-strǩ], ['eks-trǩ], or ['ekst-
rǩ]. The division ['ek-strǩ] seems to be more natural.
Instrumental analysis proves that a new pronunciation effort
begins after the first consonant. Therefore such syllable division
is fixed in pro- nunciation dictionaries.
It should be kept in mind that the pronunciation of the stressed short
vowel in this case is checked, the transition from a vowel to a consonant
is very close and there is no weakening of an articulatory effort towards
the end of the syllable. Thus the syllable is closed. This rule for syllable
divi- sion in the English language is very important for language
teaching. Stu- dents should never confuse the Russian open stressed
syllable in words like си-то with the English closed stressed syllable in
words like city ['sıŧı].
III. Syllable division concerning English pre-tonic unstressed
vowels also depends on the number of consonants sepating them from
the next stressed vowel, no matter whether it is a monophthong, a
diphthong or a diphthongoid.
1) When an unstressed short vowel is separated from a succeeding
stressed one by a single consonant, the syllable it belongs to is
always open (before [bı-'fo:], idea [aı-'dıǩ]).
2) The case when vowels are separated by a cluster of two
consonants is more difficult. The point of syllable division is
7
determined with
7
the help of phoneme distribution. If a consonant cluster is
possible in the initial position, the syllable boundary lies before
the cluster and the syllable is open; if it does not, the point of
syllable division is between the consonants and the syllable is
closed.
For example, the words agree, abrupt should be divided into
syllables in the following way: [ǩ-'grı], [ǩ-'brpt], because clusters [gr],
[br] are possible at the beginning of English words (great, cry, break).
The syl- lable boundary of the word admit is between [d] and [m]:
[ǩd-'mıt] as the sound sequence [dm] doesn’t occur at the beginning of
English words.
IV. Syllable division of post tonic vowels (monophthongs,
diphthongs and diphthongoids) separated from the following vowel by a
consonant is a matter of no practical importance for language learners.
Still most lin- guists state that it should be before the consonant: history
['hıs-tǩ-rı].
8
The similar distinction is found within language units of greater
magnitude: an aim [ǩn 'eım] vs. a name [ǩ 'neım]; we loan [wı
'lǩun] vs. we’ll own [wıl 'ǩun]. These oppositional pairs present
differentia- tion of syllables concerning word combinations.
Sometimes syllable division may even be the basic ground for
sentence differentiation: I saw her rise [aı 'so: hǩ 'raız] vs. I saw her
eyes [aı 'so: hǩr 'aız]; I saw the meat [aı 'so: ðǩ 'mi:t] vs. I saw them
eat [aı 'so: ðǩm 'i:t].
3. The identificatory function of the syllable is conditioned by the pro-
nunciation of the speaker.
The listener’s ability to perceive and identify the exact meaning of
a word or a combination of words depends on the speaker’s ability to
es- tablish the correct syllabic boundary: pea stalks ['pi: 'sto:ks] vs.
peace talks ['pi:s 'to:ks]; my train [maı 'treın] vs. might rain [maıt
'reın].
The realization of the distinctive and identificatory functions of
the syllable is closely connected with the notion of juncture, kept
by the speaker and taken in by the listener.
Close juncture (conjuncture) occurs between the sounds of the
same syllable. Open juncture (disjuncture) occurs between the
sounds of two different syllables.
Some linguists state that word juncture should be marked in a pho-
netic transcription with [+]. In this case the differentiation between the
oppositional pairs will look as follows:
ice cream [aıs + kri:m] vs. I scream [aı + skri:m].
Summarizing, it’s necessary to underline that the syllable reveals its
functional value only occasionally. This means that all the functions of
the syllable can be realized only with the help of other phonological
units.
8
that parts of phonetic and orthographic syllables do not always coincide
(let-ter ['let-’ǩ], sin-ging ['sıŋ-’ıŋ).
Syllable division in writing may follow the rules which have already
been stated above. Then the division of the syllabic structure in
orthogra- phy is made according to phonological principles (fa-mi-ly,
re-gu-lar). Still such a division is not always possible. For example, in
the words body ['bɔd-’ı], money ['mn-’ı], the consonant letter
representing the point of syllable division should be added to the next
vowel letter in order to escape notation gaps: bo-dy, mo-ney.
Syllable division in writing may be also based on the morphological
principle. In this case prefixal and suffixal morphemes are divided from
the root one (ir-regul-ar), no matter whether they belong to the same
phonetic syllable or not.
Graphic representation of syllables in orthography is relevant for
language learning only when it is necessary to carry some of the letters over
to the next line. This process is usually called syllable separation.
Special attention is necessary in order to exercise it correctly. There are the
following rules:
— a word can be separated only if it consists of more than 5 letters
and contains more than one syllable;
— the number of separated letters should be more than one;
— the parts of a word subjected to separation are derivational
morhemes used in word-building, but not inflexional ones used in
word-chan- ging (be-come, friend-ship, commit-ment, sports-man,
volley-ball);
— the suffix -ing can be separated with the preceding consonant if
there is a consonant cluster before it: hand-ling;
— suffixes consisting of two syllables can’t be broken and should
be sepapated as a whole: vulner-able;
— suffixes consisting of two letters can’t be separated with the
exep- tion of -ly: surprised, teacher, graphic, but: correct-ly.
Seminar 5
8
a) the expiratory theory;
b) the sonority theory;
c) the theory of muscular tension;
d) the loudness theory.
3. Give the definition of the syllable as a sum of features,
characteristic of this suprasegmental unit.
4. What are the two aspects that determine the problem of the study
of syllable?
5. What is syllable formation in the English language based on?
6. Give an overview of different classifications of syllables types. Il-
lustrate what you are going to say.
7. Discuss the peculiarities of consonant distribution in the structure
of English syllables. Give your own examples.
8. Give a generalized formula of an English syllable. What parts does
it consist of?
9. What type of syllable makes up the basis of syllable formation in
English?
10. What is the essence of syllable division in the English language?
11. List and explain the basic rules for syllable division in the English
language.
12. Why is it sometimes difficult to define syllable boundary
within English words?
13. What functions does a syllable perform?
14. Characterize the syllabic functions one by one. Suggest evidence
for each of them with the help of examples.
15. Explain the notion of juncture. How is it connected with the
realiza- tion of syllabic functions?
16. What is the difference between phonetic and orthographic
represen- tations of syllables?
17. What methods of syllable division in writing do you know?
18. List the rules for syllable separation. Give your own examples.
8
Lecture 6
WORD STRESS IN ENGLISH
8
primary and secondary stress. Both types serve to single out stressed
9
syllables in a word, but the degree of prominence achieved by the pri-
mary stress is greater than that indicated by the secondary stress.
Notional words in every language have primary stress, which is also
termed ‘main’ or ‘nuclear’. The existence of secondary stress in polysyl-
labic words is not characteristic of all languages.
For example, English words with the number of syllables counted
one to four usually have one primary stress ('toy, 'owner, 'character,
psy'chology), but most words of more than four syllables have two
stresses: primary and secondary (pro nunci'ation). In the Russian lan-
guage polysyllabic words have ' only one primary stress
(произношéние). The most common mistake made by Russian students
consists in omitting the secondary stress in words like demonstration
[ demǩns'treı∫n] under the influence of the primary ' stress pattern
демонстрáция. Therefore spe- cial attention should be paid to this
peculiarity of English word stress in the process of language teaching.
The foresaid distribution of the degrees of word stress is backed up
by all linguists. However, the opinions of phoneticians differ concerning
the following question: how many degrees of stress are linguistically re-
levant in a word?
Russian linguists consider that unstressed syllables should not be
taken into account, whereas foreign ones state that there are actually as
many degrees of stress in a word as there are syllables.
British scholars usually distinguish three degrees of stress in a word:
— primary stress, which is the strongest;
— secondary stress, which is less prominent;
— weak stress, which is realized in unstressed syllables.
American scholars distinguish four degrees of word stress:
— primary stress;
— secondary stress;
— tertiary stress;
— weak stress.
The difference between the secondary and tertiary stresses is rather
vague and seems to be predetermined by the differences between British
and American variants of English.
9
For example, some suffixes of nouns and verbs get additional
tertiary stress in American English ('terri tory, 'dictio nary, 'adver tize).
'
Yet the tendency to use tertiary stress '
on a post-tonic syllable is also
traced in modern British English.
Due to some peculiarities of the English language, some
polysyllabic words have two primary stresses ('seven'teen,
'good-’looking, to 'mis'lead). The difference between them is marked by
the predominance of some components determining the nature of word
stress.
The first main stress, which is called ‘pre-nuclear primary stress’, is
accompanied with the change of the pitch level height. The second main
stress, which is called ‘nuclear primary stress’, is affected by a change
of pitch direction and forms the accentual nucleus of a word. There are
ca- ses when the two primary stresses may be accompanied with a
secondary one ('re organi'zation).
'
It’s necessary to mention that all the words with two primary
stresses in modern English may have variants in accentuation generally
observed in connected speech. They are usually pronounced with two
primary stresses in a careful normative copnversation and retain the
difference between the nuclear and pre-nuclear primary stress ('indi
vidu'ality, 'ir'regular). But in a rapid colloquial conversation the
' degree
of the first primary stress may be changed into the secondary or even
weak one ('indi vidu'ality, ir'regular).
'
§ 4. Phonemic distribution
in stressed syllables
9
['sbweı]). Vowels [ı, u, ǩ] tend to occur in unstressed syllables. Sylla-
bles with the syllabic sonorants [l, m, n] are never stressed.
English consonants tend to keep their distinctive features in stressed
syllables: stops have a complete closure, fricatives have full friction,
fortis-lenis features distinction is clearly defined.
9
transport ['trænspo:rt] — to transport [trǩns'po:t];
mankind ['mænkaınd] — mankind [mæn'kaınd];
blackboard ['blækbo:d] — black board ['blæk 'bo:d].
The same functions characterize the processes of word-building and
word-formation in the Russian language: зáмок — замóк, безобразнáя
— безóбразная, ногú — нóги.
9
The pattern with the primary stress on the second syllable is real-
ized in:
9
— disyllabic words with historical prefixes subjected to the restrict-
ed recessive tendency (be'cause, pro'claim, a'part, for'get);
— disyllabic verbs with the endings -ate, -ise/ize, -y (nar'rate,
com'prise, de'fy);
— words of three or four syllables with suffixes subjected to the
rhythmical tendency (phi'lology, de'mocracy, ex'perience,
o'riginate).
3. [┴ (─) ┴ (─)].
The pattern with two primary stresses is generally observed in:
— compound nouns or adjectives, consisting of two roots ('well-
'bred, 'absent-'minded, 'tea-'pot, 'ice-'cream);
— composite verbs with postpositions ('get 'up, 'come 'out, 'give 'in);
— compound words with separable prefixes ('un'fair, 'dis'appear,
'ex'minister, 're'play).
4. [┴ ┬ ─].
The pattern with primary stress followed by the secondary one is
very common among compound words as the accentuation variant
of the third pattern ('hair- dresser, 'dog- killer, 'sub structure). It is
' speech. '
often realized in connected
5. [(─) ┬ (─) (┴) ─].
The pattern with the secondary stress preceding the primary one
marks a great number of simple polysyllabic words with affixes
( intu'ition, govern'mental, pe culi'arity, repre'sent). It is as well
'
found '
in compound ' accentuation variant of the third
words as the
pattern in connected speech ( misin'terpret).
'
6. [┴ ┴ ┴ (┴)].
The pattern with three and more primary stresses is
characteristic of initial compound abbreviations ('B'B'C (British
Broadcasting Cor- poration), 'N'Y'S'E (New York Stock Exchange).
7. [┬ (─) ┬ (─) ┴ ─].
9
The pattern with two secondary stresses preceding the primary
one is found in a very small number of words with the stressed pre-
fixes, roots and suffixes ('sub'organ'ization, 'indilviduali'zation).
8. [┴ ┴ (─) ┬ ─].
The pattern with two primary stresses preceding the secondary
one is rarely found in compound words with separable prefixes
('un'trustlworthy).
The patters described above suggest the idea of great variability
in the accentuation of English words. The most widely used are pat-
terns # 1, 2, 4 and 5, which cover the main part of common English
vocabulary. Still there are a lot of words which have variants in ac-
centuation. They may differ in:
— number of stresses: UNSC [┴ ┴ ┴ ┴] or [┴ ─ ─ ┴];
— place of stress: laryngoscope [┴ ─ ─ ─] or [─ ┴ ─ ─];
— degree of stress: disability [┴ ─ ┴ ─ ─] or [┬ ─ ┴ ─ ─]; etc.
This fact underlines the instability of English accentual structure
mul-
tiplied in connected speech.
9
first syllable ('stationary, 'territory);
1
— polysyllabic words with the primary stress on the third syllable
have secondary stress on the first syllable ('proba'bility);
— polysyllabic words with the primary stress on the fourth and
fifth syllable have secondary stress on the second syllable
(arlticu'lation);
— polysyllabic words with separable prefixes with a distinct
meaning have two primary stresses ('un'known, 'dis'charge,
're'pay, 'mispro'nounce, 'pre-'war, 'ex-'wife, 'inter'view, 'anti-
ag'gressive).
Special attention should be paid to the accentual structure of com-
pound words. Variability in the accentuation of English words is multi-
plied many times because of the variability in compound structures of
the English language.
In order not to make mistakes, the following rules should be ob-
served:
— compound numerals have two primary stresses ('twenty-'four);
— compound adjectives are generally double-stressed ('well-
'known);
— compound adjectives with semantically weak second component
are single-stressed ('childlike), but they are not numerous;
— compound verbs with post-positions get two main stresses ('put 'off);
— compound nouns are usually single-stressed ('strong-box), and
thus differ from word combination with two stressed words
('strong 'box);
— compound nouns with the equal significance of both elements
are double-stressed ('ice-'cream), but they are quite rare.
1
Seminar 6
1
13. What functions does word stress perform?
14. Give a definition of the term ‘accenteme’. Where is it studied?
What types of accentemes do you know?
15. What are the functions of accentuation oppositions?
16. What tendencies effect the position of word stress in English?
17. Speak about the recessive tendency. Give examples to illustrate its
influence on borrowings in English.
18. What caused the appearance of rhythmical tendency?
19. Prove the interrelation of both tendencies in the accentual structure
of English words.
20. What peculiarity marks the accentual structure of English words
with retentive tendency?
21. Which stress tendency prevails in modern English?
22. What are the most typical stress patterns of English words? What
other stress patterns do you know? Give examples to illustrate
them.
23. Is the accentual structure of English stable? Why? / Why not?
24. What are the basic rules of word accentuation? Speak about:
a) peculiarities of word stress in simple and derivative words;
b) peculiarities of word stress in compound words.
25. What is the connection of word stress and sentence stress?
Comment on the similar and different features of these
phenomena.
26. Why does the study of the accentual structure of English words
cause difficulties for language learners?
8
Lecture 7
INTONATION IN ENGLISH
8
According to it the main unit of intonation is a clause which
presents a complex of three systemic variables: tonality, tonicity and
tone. Tona- lity marks the beginning and the end of a tone-group.
Tonicity marks the focal point of each tone-group. Tones mark the
nucleus and convey the attitude of the speaker. These parts of clauses
are connected with the help of grammatical categories and carry out the
syntactical function. Thus the statement “I’d like to” may have several
attitudinal meanings determined by the pre-nuclear and nuclear choices:
neutral (Low Fall), non-commit- tal (Low Rise), contradictory (High
Rise), reserved (Fall Rise), committal (Rise-Fall).
III. The American school of intonation founded by K. Pike
considers pitch phonemes and contours to be the main units of
intonation, which have their own meanings, but stand apart from the
communicative func- tion of intonation. This approach is treated by
most linguists as ‘me- chanical’.
IV. D. Crystal represents the extralinguistic study of intonation.
He states that it is impossible to explain intonational meaning only with
the help of grammatical or attitudinal means. He ignores the
significance of pre-head and head choices and deals only with terminal
nuclear tones, which should have both linguistic and extralinguistic
marking.
According to D. Crystal, there are nine ways of saying “Yes” as an
answer to the question “Will you marry me?”:
1) Low Fall — the most neutral tone, it reflects a detached, unemo-
tional statement of fact;
2) Full Fall — an emotionally involved tone, the involvement of
the speaker determines the onset pitch, whereas the choice of
emo- tion (surprise, excitement, irritation) depends on facial
expression of the speaker;
3) Mid Fall — a routine tone that conveys detached and unexcited
attitude;
4) Low Rise — an emotional tone, the attitude is told by the speak-
er’s facial expression: with a ‘happy’ face the tone is
sympathetic and friendly, with a ‘grim’ face it is guarded and
ominous.
5) Full Rise — an emotionally involved tone meaning disbelief or
9
shock, the extent of the emotion is determined by the width of the
tone;
9
6) High Rise — an emotional tone, often used in echoing what has
just been said, it reflects mild query or puzzlement;
7) Level tone — conveys bored, sarcastic, ironic attitude of the
speaker;
8) Fall-Rise — a strongly emotional tone, the emotion is
determined by the expression of the speaker’s face: a ‘straight’
or ‘negative’ face conveys uncertainty, doubt, or tentativeness, a
‘positive’ face conveys encouragement or urgency;
9) Rise-fall — a tone that presents strong emotional involvement,
depending on the face the attitude might be delighted, challeng-
ing, or complacent.
Thus the linguistic study of intonation in foreign linguistics is
restricted by pitch movements (melody) which determine its outer
physical expres- sion. The priority of the pitch parameter is quite
evident, but real commu- nication involves the change of other
intonation parameters as well.
§ 3. Problem of intonation
in Russian linguistics
9
prosodic components: pitch, loudness and tempo. This term is widely
used in modern linguistic literature, because it is more adequate and
causes no misunderstanding.
The prosodic components of intonation and their speech realizations
are interconnected. Every speech syllable has a special pitch colouring
and bears a definite amount of loudness. Together with the speech
tempo they form an intonation pattern which is the basic unit of
intonation.
An intonation pattern contains a nucleus which may or may not be
preceded or followed by other stressed or unstressed syllables. The
boundaries of an intonation pattern are marked by temporal pauses.
Into- nation patterns are actualized in intonation groups.
An intonation group (a speech syntagm) presents a semantically and
syntactically complete group of words which may have different length:
from one word to a group of words or a sentence.
For example: Yes.
\
I \do.
I \like it.
I like that \too.
9
The first five of these are the most important nuclear tones cha-
racteristic of English. They have different meanings. Low Fall and
High Fall usually express certainty, completeness, and
independence. Low Rise and High Rise vise versa express
uncertainty, incomplete- ness or dependence. Fall-Rise combines the
meaning of certainty with that of incompleteness, thus suggesting
that there is something else to be said. It may occur within one
syllable or spread over two or more syllables (\Fortunately I /do.).
The last three tones are not considered to be essential ones. Rise-
Fall and Rise-Fall-Rise add some refinement to speech and can be
easily replaced by basic nuclear tones without making considerable
changes in the meaning of the utterance: Rise-Fall by High Fall and
Rise-Fall-Rise by Fall-Rise. Mid-Level tone is characteristic of
spon- taneous speech when replacing the rising tone (After
'everything you’ve >said | I 'don’t want to \go there!).
2. The pitch level parameter includes variations of the normal range of
speaking voice within a given interval between its lower and upper
limits. There are three pitch levels: high, medium, and low.
High level
Medium level
Low level
3. The pitch range is the interval between two pitch levels from the
highest-pitched to the lowest-pitched syllables. The pitch range may
be normal, wide, and narrow.
↑ ↑ ↕
↓ ↑ | ↕
↓ ↓ ↕
Normal Wide Narrow
(of high, medium, low levels)
9
They may cause various semantic differences. For example, an over-
all loudness level conveys extreme emotions, such as anger, menace, or
excitement. Loudness changes are inseparably connected with pitch
vari- ations, because both of them create the effect of accentuation.
III. The tempo component of intonation implies variations in the
rate of the utterance and pausation.
a) The rate of speech is divided into normal, fast and slow.
It differs according to the importance of the parts of the
utterance, since the important ones are spoken slower, but
unimportant ones are pronounced at a greater speed.
b) Pauses are complete stops of phonation dividing a stretch of
speech into smaller units.
According to their length, the following kinds of pauses are distin-
guished:
— short pauses, which separate intonation groups within a phrase;
— longer pauses, which manifest the end of the phrase;
— very long pauses, which are used to separate phonetic wholes.
From the functional point of view there exist:
— syntactic pauses, which separate phonopassages, phrases,
intona- tion groups;
— emphatic pauses, which mark parts of the utterance especially
important for the speaker (She is the most ¦ talented actress I’ve
ever met ||);
— hesitation pauses, which are used in spontaneous speech to think
over what to say next; they may be silent (It’s rather a ...
difficult question ||) or filled (I’ll have to ... eeh ... think it over
||).
The changes of pitch, loudness and tempo are not accidental. They
are formalized in the abstracted set of intonation structures called
intona- tion patterns, which form the prosodic system of the English
language. Definite intonation patterns are actualized in real
communicative situa- tions with the help of intonation groups.
An intonation group is a word or a group of words characterized
by a certain intonation pattern complete from the point of view of
9
meaning.
9
For example, the sentence ‘I suppose he’ll be here in a moment’ may be
divided in two intonation groups: ‘I suppose’ and ‘he’ll be here in a mo-
ment’.
The structure of an intonation pattern potentially includes the
pre- head, the head, the nucleus and the tail:
— the pre-head contains unstressed and half-stressed syllables pre-
ceding the head;
— the head includes syllables from the first stressed up to the last
stressed one;
— the nucleus is the last stressed syllable presenting the change in
the pitch direction;
— the tail consists of unstressed and half-stressed syllables follow-
ing the nucleus.
The boundaries of an intonation pattern are marked by complete
stops of phonation or temporal pauses.
The abovementioned structure of an intonation pattern may be further
grouped into two larger units:
1) The pre-nuclear part of the intonation pattern is formed with the
pre-head and the head.
It can present different variations of pitch patterns, which do not
usually affect the grammatical meaning of the utterance, but often
convey meanings associated with the speaker’s attitude. There are
three common types of prе-nuclear part:
— a descending type with the pitch level gradually descending to
the nucleus;
— an ascending type with the ascending sequence of syllables;
— a level type with the syllable set of approximately the same pitch
level.
2) The terminal part of the intonation pattern consists of the
nucleus and the tail.
It is the most significant part of the intonation pattern which deter-
mines the nuclear tone and the pitch level of the rest of the utterance.
The set of English nuclear tones includes five widely used common
tones
9
(Low Fall, High Fall, Low Rise, High Rise, Fall-Rise), and three
optional tones (Rise-Fall, Rise-Fall-Rise, Mid-Level).
It’s important to mention, that every part of the intonation pattern
besides the nucleus is considered to be optional.
For example, let us consider the importance of sentence parts in the
following dialogue:
Who’s done it? — Well, that’s Jack actually.
It is obvious that in the second sentence of the dialogue the nucleus
‘Jack’ is the only part of the intonation group which is really
informative. All other parts (the pre-head ‘well’, the head ‘that’s’, the
tail ‘actually’) can be omitted in real speech, because they are not
necessary for under- standing the meaning.
1
— downward and upward curves represent the final nuclear tone;
— vertical bars indicate the temporal component (|| , | or ¦).
This representation is called ‘a stave’ or ‘a tonogram’.
§ 6. Functions of intonation
1
— to structure the information content of a text and identify new
information;
1
— to determine the speech function of a phrase and indicate
sentence types;
— to convey attitudinal meaning (surprise, annoyance, etc.);
— to structure a text and organize smaller units (phrases, intonation
groups);
— to differentiate between the meaning of the units with the
same grammatical structure and lexical composition;
— to characterize a particular style of oral speech.
For the purposes of language teaching Russian phoneticians (M.A.
So- kolova, K.P. Gintovt, and others) define the following two main
func- tions of intonation as a powerful means of communication: the
constitu- tive and the distinctive ones.
1. The constitutive function characterizes intonation as an organizing
mechanism.
On the one hand, it delimitates texts into intonation groups with
a certain structure; on the other hand, it integrates intonation groups
when forming a complete text. A broad classification of intonation
patterns, different and specific in their nature, is singled out in every
language. Their material realization helps the speaker to construct
the utterance and enables the listener to perceive it.
2. The distinctive function of intonation helps to distinguish commu-
nicative types of sentences, the actual meaning of a sentence, the
speaker’s attitude to its contents and the style of the utterance.
The distinctive function of intonation is realized in the opposition of
the same word sequences which differ in certain parameters of the
intona- tion pattern.
For example, the meaning of the phrases: ‘If Tom calls let me know
at once’ and ‘If Tom calls let me know at once’ is easily distinguished
thanks to the opposition of different intonation patterns of the first
intona- tion groups. The first sentence means that a few people are
expected to call but it is Tom who interests the speaker; the meaning of
the second one is that no one else but Tom is expected to call.
The opposition in the pitch parameters of the sentences ‘I en\joyed
it’ and ‘I en/joyed it’ shows the reserved attitude of the speaker in the
first
1
case, and the attitude implying a continuation like ‘but it could have
been a lot better’ in the second one.
1
My partner who went away on business last month, | has just arrived ||.
The division in three intonation groups means that he’s the only
busi- ness partner:
My partner, | who went away on business last month, | has just arrived ||.
Terminal tones also identify the semantic centre of the utterance and
single out the information. For example, the unusual stress of form
words instead of content ones expresses the speaker’s feelings and
attitudes:
I 'don’t \like him. (neutral)
I \don’t |like him. (annoyed)
Besides tonemes, the classification of phonological units
constituting intonation patterns includes other abstract units: intonemes,
accentemes, chronemes, etc. They also include variations.
§ 8. English rhythm
Seminar 7
1
20. What conception of the functional level of intonation is followed in
home linguistics?
21. State the value of the communicative function of intonation.
22. What functions of intonation are important for language teaching?
Characterize the role of these functions in the process of communi-
cation.
23. Suggest examples for the distinctive function of intonation.
24. Prove that intonation is a unit of phonology.
25. Give the classification of phonological units. Illustrate its main
ideas with the help of tonemes.
26. What is your understanding of intonation and its role in language
organization?
27. What types of rhythmic language organization do you know?
28. Speak on the peculiarities of English rhythm.
1
Lecture 8
STYLISTIC AND REGIONAL VARIETIES
OF ENGLISH PRONUNCIATION
1
social factors, such as functional stylistics, psycholinguistics, ethnolin-
1
guistics, anthropological linguistics, varianthology, and others. They can
study language phenomena within three levels: phonetic, lexical and
grammatical.
Speaking about phonetics it becomes obvious that pronunciation is
by no means homogeneous. It changes under the influence of numerous
fac- tors. The linguistic factors are studied in phonology, whereas the
extra- linguistic ones refer to other branches of phonetics which are
linked with sociolinguistic sciences.
The varieties of language phonetic means of different territoriality
conditioned by language communities ranging from small groups to na-
tions, are studied within phonovarianthology. The problems of
different styles of pronunciation are studied within phonostylistics. It
analyses the spoken form of language expression and deals with those
phonetic means used in some particular situations under the influence of
a certain set of extralinguistic factors.
It should be mentioned that problems of phonostylistics and
phonovari- anthology are thoroughly investigated in the book by M.A.
Sokolova men- tioned in the previous sections [19]. Here we’ll try to
give a brief overview of the main poins and add some new information
on the subject.
§ 2. Territorial varieties
of English pronunciation
1
acknowledged
1
as the standard pronunciation model. Still this standard is not homoge-
neous throughout the country and may have certain variations.
American English and British English have separated more than a
cen- tury ago. Nowadays these are the two most widely used national
variants of English, each of them possessing its own standards in all
language systems. It’s important to note that pronunciation standards are
not perma- nently fixed and undergo constant changes under the
influence of various internal and external factors. Teaching practice
should follow the rules of
the most widely accepted pronunciation model.
1
the pronunciation standard in the 19th century. It was acknowledged as
the
1
Received Pronunciation (RP). The use of this pronunciation type
marked the speaker as the representative of high society. For a long time
RP has been referred to as “King’s (Queen’s) English”, it characterized
the speech of aristocracy and the court. The spread of education gradu-
ally modified the characteristics of this accent in the direction of social
standards. Received Pronunciation was taught at public schools and
used in the best society by cultured people. It has become a social
marker, a prestigeous accent of an educated Englishman. Nowadays
only about 5% of the population in Britain speaks RP, though it is still
regarded as a conservative model for correct pronunciation, particularly
for educated formal speech.
The wide distribution of radio and television caused considerable
changes in the sound system of the present-day English, and there ap-
peared a new pronunciation model — the ВВС English. This is the pro-
nunciation of professional BBC newsreaders and announcers. It is based
on RP, but also takes into consideration modern linguistic situation and
thus becomes more flexible and true-to-life. Moreover, the wide spread
audio-visual means of mass communication make it accessible to
general public. The last edition of English Pronouncing Dictionary fixes
the BBC accent as the most broadly-based pronunciation model accent
for modern English.
The remarkable systemic modifications in the standard can be men-
tioned both in the vowel and consonant systems in the course of the last
century.
I. Vowel changes include the following:
1. increasing diphthongization of the historically long vowels [i:] and
[u:];
2. frequent lengthening of a historically short vowel [æ];
3. gradual monophthongization of some diphthongs:
— [au], [aı] followed by neutral [ǩ] are smoothed (power [pauǩ] →
[paǩ], fire [faıǩ] → [faǩ]);
— [oǩ], [uǩ] are pronounced like long [o:] or short [ɔ] with an [ǩ]-
shade (poor [puǩ] → [po:]/[pɔǩ]);
— [εǩ] is levelled to long [ǩ:] (careful [‘kεǩful] → [‘kǩ:ful]);
1
4. mutual vowel interchanges:
— between diphthongs [ou] and [ǩu] (phone [foun]/[fǩun], note
[nout]/[nǩut];
— between monophthongs [æ] and [a] (have [hæv]/[hav], dance
[dans]/[dæns]).
II. Consonant changes include the following:
1. gradual loss of the voiced/voiceless distinctions in certain positions:
— increasing devoicing of final voiced stops (dog [dog] → [dok],
cab [kæb] → [kæp]);
— voicing of intervocalic [t] (letter ['letǩ] → ['ledǩ]);
2. loss of final [ŋ] and initial [h] in rapid speech (He saw her sitting
there
[hi· →so: hз· 'sıtıŋ ðεǩ] → [i: →so: з· 'sıtın ðεǩ];
3. wide usage of typical elements of the American pronunciation:
— dark [l] instead of [l] (believe [bı'lıv] → [bı'lıv]);
— palatalized [k] in final positions (quick [kwık] → [kwık’]);
— linking and intrusive [r] (faraway, the ideaof);
III. Combinative changes generally concern the pronunciation of [j]
in certain phonetic contexts, which include:
— loss of [j] before [u:] (student ['stju:dnt] → ['stu:dnt], suit [sju:t]
→ [su:t]), sometimes accompanied with palatalization of the pre-
vious consonant (Tuesday ['tju:zdı] → ['t∫u:zdı]);
— intrusion of [j] before [u:] after [l] (illuminant [ı'lu:mınǩnt] →
[ı'lju:mınǩnt]);
— change of [j] by other sounds in original combinations [tj], [dj],
[sj] (factual ['fæktjuǩl] → ['fækt∫uǩl], graduate ['grædjuǩıt] →
['græʤuǩt], issue ['isju:] → ['i∫u:].
Other combinative changes gradually follow the general tendencies
of assimilation and reduction.
These are variations which get systemic representation in modern
British English pronunciation. There are also non-systemic variations in
standard pronunciation which appear in different accents, but they are
too numerous and need a separate consideration.
1
2.3. American English
The development of American English began with the settlement of
the first British colonists in the North American continent. In the course
of its formation American English has undergone the influence of many
other lan- guages spoken by the Native Americans (the Indians), by the
immigrants from Ireland, Spain, France, Holland, Germany, by the
Negroes. Nowadays the impact of Spanish and Chinese is easily felt in
American English.
As for American pronunciation, it’s not homogeneous at all. The
three main regional types of pronunciation are distinguished: eastern,
southern and western.
1. The Eastern type is spoken in New England and New York. It re-
sembles Southern accents of British English and includes:
— the cases of linking and intrusive [r];
— initial [hw] (which [hwıt∫]);
— monophthongization of diphthongs with [ǩ]-glide (fierce [fıǩs] →
[fi:s]).
2. The Southern type is spoken in Southern and South-Eastern states
and is characterized by a specific Southern drawl. It is a vowel
drawl, which causes:
— diphthongization of pure monophthongs (egg [eg] → [eig], yes
[jes] → [jeis]);
— monophthongization of original diphthongs (eight [eıt] → [ε:t],
drain [dreın] → [drε:n]).
3. The Western type is spoken in Western and central Atlantic states. It
is characterized by the so-called ‘Western burr’. This phenomenon
includes:
— the pronunciation of retroflexed vowels with r-colouring in the
middle of the word (bird [bǩ:rd], worm [wǩ:rm], first [fǩ:rst],
card [ka:rd], port [po:rt]);
— the pronunciation of retroflexed [r] in the final position (far [far],
here [hıǩr]).
A.D. Schweitzer offers to divide these types of pronunciation into 2
1
groups on the basis of the presence or absence of the Western burr. These
are:
1
— the non-rhotic group pronounced without Western burr, which
includes the Eastern and Southern types of pronunciation;
— General American pronunciation with Westen burr, which in-
cludes the Western type of pronunciation.
Some linguists treat General American (GA) as a standard pronun-
ciation type, because it is spoken by the majority of Americans. It is true
that GA is used in the states, which constitute about 90% of all the terri-
tory of the USA. It is also frequently heard from professional voices on
national media (radio, television, movies, CDs, etc.).
But many linguists state that no dialect can be singled out as an
Ame- rican standard, because different types of pronunciation are
constantly mixed and even professionally trained speakers retain their
regional pro- nunciation features.
The peculiar situation with the absence of the codified pronunciation
standard is intensified with the specific status of the English language in
the USA. It is not fixed in the Federal Constitution as the official lan-
guage of the United States, though it really is.
Still American pronunciation is different from RP. American
English possesses a set of systemic peculiarities both among the
segmental and suprasegmental units.
The segmental peculiarities include:
1. Specific pronunciation of vowel phonemes:
— absence of clear distinction between short and long vowels (sit/
seat [sı·t], pull/pool [pu·l];
— existence of only 5 diphthongs, compared to 8 in RP — [eı],
[aı], [oı], [au], [ou], while other diphthongs are treated as
biphonemic combinations;
— rhotic pronunciation of vowels before [r] in all positions (turn
[tǩ:rn], star [sta: r]);
— ‘nasal twang’ — nasalization of vowels preceded or followed by
nasal consonants (stain, small, name, stand, time, any, make);
— pronunciation of [æ] instead of [a] before a consonant or a
cluster (class, after, path, dance, plant, grass, bath, half);
— pronunciation of [a] instead of [o] (dog, body, shot, hot) and a
complete loss of long [o:] (cot [kat] vs. caught [kot]);
1
— monophthongization of diphthongs and diphthongization of
monophthongs, including the reverse pronunciation of [ı] and
[aı]
(civilization [ sıvılı'zeı∫n] → [ sıvılaı'zeı∫n], direct [dı'rekt] →
' '
[daı'rekt], spec ialization [' spe∫ı ǩlı'zeı∫n] → [' spe∫ıǩlaı'zeı∫n]; si-
multaneous [ sımǩl'teınjǩs] → [ saımǩl'teınjǩs ]);
' '
2. Specific pronunciation of consonant phonemes:
— loss of [t] after [n] in the middle of the word (twenty ['twenı],
wanted ['wonıd], winter ['wınǩ]);
— flapping — pronunciation of [t] like [d] in the intervocalic posi-
tion and before [l] (bitter, battle, little);
— existence of only dark shade of [l] (look [źuk], lamp [źæmp], luck
[źk]);
— omittance of [j] between a consonant or a vowel [u:] (news [nu:z],
tube [tu:b], during ['du:rıŋ]).
The supra-segmental peculiarities generally concern word stress and
include:
— placement of stress on the final syllable instead of the initial one
in words of French origin (ballet [bæ'leı]);
— placement of stress on the first element in compound words
('weekend, 'hotdog);
— existence of tertiary stress in polysyllabic words with suffixes
-ory, -ary, -mony (laboratory ['læbrǩ torı], secretary ['sekrǩ tǩrı],
ceremony ['serǩ monı]). ' '
'
American intonation patterns on the whole are similar to those of
RP. The differences generally convey emotional and attitudinal
meaning. For example, the intonation contour of a general question is
neutral if it is used in RP. In General American it conveys the meaning
of surprise or reserved curiosity:
I’ve ordered some oysters for dinner. — Do you /like them?
1
The same word or a sequence of words may be pronounced quite
dif- ferently by different speakers under different circumstances.
Thus in
1
rapid colloquial speech the conjuction and is frequently pronounced like
[n], but the same word might be pronounced like [ǩnd] in slow everyday
speech or even like [ænd] in a careful serious conversation. In other
words, the pronunciation of speech sounds is greatly determined by the
style of pronunciation. Stylistic variations of pronunciation are quite nu-
merous. They are studied within phonostylistics — a comparatively
new branch of linguistics which has links both with phonetics and
stylistics.
purpose of communication
1
basis of different spheres of discourse.
1
But there are certain lacks in this approach, because oral texts
referred to different functional styles may have identical phonetic
features. Thus, an extract from a piece of prose or an advertisement does
not reveal any phonostylistic differences if read aloud with the same
pragmatic aim.
II. Another group of phoneticians suggests that the classification of
pro- nunciation styles should be based on different degrees of formality
and fami- liarity between the speaker and the listener. For example, J.A.
Dubovsky suggests the following phonetic styles: informal ordinary,
formal neutral, formal official, informal familiar, declamatory.
The degree of familiarity may be also combined with the number of
listeners. Thus, L.V. Shcherba suggests the existence of only two styles
of pronunciation: the colloquial style characteristic of people’s quiet talk,
and the full style used in distinct public speech. On the same basis A.D.
Jones distinguishes: the rapid familiar style, the slower colloquial style,
the natural style addressed to a fair-sized audience, the acquired style of
the stage, and the acquired style of singing.
The theories stated above are suitable for presenting texts for
descrip- tion and analysis, but they still don’t create a symmetrical
classification of speech acts.
III. A different view is presented by the linguists, who consider the
problem of phonostylistic classification in connection with style-
forming factors. For instance, M.A. Sokolova singles out five
intonational styles according to the purpose of communication:
informational style, aca- demic (scientific) style, publicistic style,
declamatory style, conversational (familiar) style. The use of each of
these styles results in evident varia- tions of suprasegmental phonetic
units.
This approach makes up a rather adequate system of phonostylistic
varieties, but still differentiation of intonation according to the purpose
of communication is not enough. There are other factors that affect
intona- tion in various situations. Moreover, the changes of the
segmental units are not clearly defined.
It’s possible to say that the question of phonostylistic classification
is still open in modern linguistics, as the peculiarities of different styles
of pronunciation have not yet been sufficiently investigated. But no
matter which classification is taken into account, it’s always necessary
to remem- ber that any style is seldom realized in its pure form. Every
1
oral text presents a fusion of styles and includes different phonostylistic
characteristics.
1
Seminar 8
1
14. Comment on the main peculiarities of American pronunciation.
Dis- cuss specific features in:
a) vowel pronunciation;
b) consonant pronunciation;
c) placement of stress and intonation patterns.
15. Is it possible to consider certain peculiarities observed in the pro-
nunciation of foreign speakers as the rise of new variants of Eng-
lish? Explain your point of view.
16. What is the object of phonostylistics?
17. Name the main components of speech communication and prove
their interrelation.
18. State the difference between the style-forming and style-differenti-
ating factors.
19. Give the classification of style-forming factors. Which factor is the
most significant? Why?
20. Give an overview of other style-forming factors.
21. What variations do the style-differentiating factors imply? Give a
brief characterization.
22. Is it possible to determine the style of speech according to phonetic
variations only? Why? / Why not?
23. Speak about different classifications of phonetic styles. Discuss
this problem in connection with:
a) functional styles;
b) degrees of formality and familiarity;
c) style-forming factors.
24. Which of these classifications is the most relevant in your opinion?
Give your reasons.
25. Comment on the following statement: ‘any style is seldom realized
in its pure form’.
1
P A R T II
Practical Exercises
1
Exercise Block 1
#1. Write the 3rd person singular forms of the verbs and transcribe
them. State the connection of phonetics and grammar.
loves poil place tick
like put type rule
deny punish see touch
teach dig read rely
#2. Write the three forms of the verbs and transcribe them. Under-
line the interchanging vowel and consonant sounds. Prove that
phonetics is connected with grammar.
become drive kneel shake
bite feel lean shoot
build find leap sink
catch forgive lie spill
choose grind mean swear
creep hang ride throw
dig hide run wind
#3. Write the plural forms of the nouns and transcribe them. State
the connection of phonetics and grammar.
girl wife month leaf
cat dog mouse book
box goose boy tooth
woman house postman army
1
#4. Read the following sentences. Prove that phonetics is connected
with grammar through intonation.
1) I’m a journalist. — You are a journalist? — I’m really a profes-
sional!
2) As a matter of fact, I find this subject quite interesting.
3) Morning came at last; the rain fell again, and the wind howled.
4) What’s your opinion on this subject?
5) Betty went to school at 7.30. — Betty went to school? Oh, she
went to school so early!
6) It is a nice country house, quite perfect and pretty, very small
and plain, and well deserving a visit.
7) You see, I promised Ben to meet him.
8) He went by train and I went by bus, so he got there earlier and I
saw more of the country.
9) Do you expect to stay here for a long time?
10) Poodle? What poodle? Oh, that little creature! Like it? It’s yours!
#5. Read the words and word-combinations. Place the accent marks.
State the connection of phonetics and lexicology.
ability-to-pay — ability to pay early-warning — early warning
blueprint — blue print face-down — face down
bull’s-eye — bull’s eye heavy-weight — heavy weight
blackmail — black mail hot-house — hot house
cache-drive — cash drive mad-doctor — mad doctor
earles-penny — earl’s penny to redbook — red book
1
a transfer — to transfer to converse — conversation
a present — to present to transform — transformation
an advice — to advise lead (руководство) — lead (свинец)
a breath — to breathe tear (разрыв) — tear (слеза)
a song — to sing row (ряд) — row (шум)
a house — to house bow (лук) — bow (поклон)
#7. Read the tongue-twisters. What sounds are used to create the
effect of alliteration? State the connection of phonetics and
stylistics.
1) Peter Piper picked a peck of pickled peppers
If Peter picked a peck of pickled peppers
Where is the peck of pickled peppers Peter Piper picked?
2) Robert Rowley rolled a round roll around,
A round roll Robert Rowley rolled around;
Where’s the round roll Robert Rowley rolled around?
3) If one doctor doctors another doctor, does the doctor who doc-
tors the doctor doctor the doctor the way the doctor he is
doctor- ing doctors? Or does he doctor the doctor the way the
doctor who doctors doctors?
4) Sudden swallows swiftly skimming,
Sunset’s slowly spreading shade,
Silvery songsters sweetly singing
Summer’s soothing serenade.
1
She lays eggs for gentlemen;
1
Sometimes nine, and sometimes ten.
Hickety, pickety, my black hen!
Cock-a-doodle-do!
3) Shoo, fly, don’t bother me, shoo, fly, don’t bother me,
Shoo, fly, don’t bother me, for I belong to somebody.
I feel, I feel, I feel, I feel like a morning star,
I feel, I feel, I feel, I feel like a morning star.
4) Rain, rain, rain, April rain,
You are feeding seed and grain,
You are raising plants and crops
With your gaily sparkling drops.
1
‘Have you been a good girl?’
‘Have you been a good girl?’
I went to the Zoo, and they waited to say:
‘Have you been a good girl?’
‘Have you been a good girl?’
Well, what did they think I went there to do?
And why should I want to be bad at the Zoo?
And should I be likely to say if I had?
So that’s why it’s funny of Mummy and Dad,
This asking and asking, in case I was bad,
‘Well? Have you been a good girl, Jane?’
#11. Make sure that you remember all organs of speech. Indicate
the corresponding parts of the sound producing mechanism
in the following pictures:
1
1) Power mechanism
1
Exercise Block 2
#1. Read and transcribe the following groups of words. Compare
the contrasted consonant phonemes.
rip — rib, tap — tab; live — life, save — safe;
bet — bed, sight — side; said — zed, racing — raising;
dog — dock, bug — buck; rich — ridge, search — surge;
verse — worse, vet — wet; sun — sung, ran — rung.
#2. Read and transcribe the following words. State the difference in
corresponding paired consonant allophones.
pork — rope fork — corn chance — cheese
bark — robe drive — vim just — gist
side — done they — bathe dim — lamp
set — ton his — zone tin — sent
dog — gone rouge — genre sing — sink
all — leave right — trap when — twelve
#3. Read and transcribe the following pairs of words. Do the dis-
criminative sounds present different phonemes or variants of
the same phoneme? Prove your opinion.
main — mine buck — book bill — pill tie — fie
1
kiss — case choice — voice word — ward
bid — bead believe — belief
1
sounds present different phonemes or allophones? Explain
your point of view. Which variant do you consider to be prin-
cipal? Why?
fly — roof thorn — eighth dweller — doer
tree — ray scream — say blow — bottle
sweat — wait fat — tame past — happen
1
— [k]: thick, cat, cask, a black cap;
— [l]: dull, light, lure;
— [p]: park, play, ape, a cold pie;
— [n, ð]: go on, no way, on the shelf, this time;
— [r]: ripe, far, war and peace.
#7. Use the procedure of commutation test for the following words.
Find minimal pairs with different meaning. State the type of
phonological opposition in each case.
kite but bat
time show veil
bake pit tea
#8. Read the following pairs of words. Transcribe the sounds cor-
responding with the letters in bold. State the number of phono-
logical oppositions in every pair.
pool — pull far — four
bay — may fast — vast
pay — they my — may
pay — bay seat — seem
chop — top fit — feet
Exercise Block 3
#1. Make sure that you remember all English consonant phonemes.
Fill in the following table.
1
degree of noise noise consonants sonorants
occlusive-
occlusive constrictive
manner of articulation constrictive occlusive constrictive
(plosives) (fricatives)
(affricates)
work of the vocal cords and force voiced voiceless voiced voiceless voiced voiceless
of articulation lenis fortis lenis fortis lenis fortis
bilabial
labial
labio-dental
interdental
place of articulation
1
alveolar
forelingual
post-alveolar
lingua
l
palato-
alveolar
palatal
mediolingual
backlingual velar
glottal
position of the soft palate oral nasal oral
#2. Write down a complete description of every English consonant
phoneme. Enclose in brackets the characteristics, which are
not phonologically relevant.
Example: [p] noise, occlusive, plosive, bilabial (voiceless), fortis,
(oral).
[b] noise, occlusive, plosive, bilabial (voiced), lenis,
(oral).
#3. Read and transcribe the following words with occlusive fortis
stops. Observe different degrees of aspiration.
port paper cart school
cut talk top proper
poker pit cost take
speech stop poke porter
cook poor taxi bat
#4. Read and transcribe the following pairs of words. Avoid palata-
lization of initial plosives and fricatives. Observe slight palata-
lization of affricates before front vowels.
part — peel cart — key thus — theme
guest — game bag — big chose — cheese
tooth — teeth far — feet chest — chin
door — day hut — heat just — gist
#5. Read the following groups of words. Mind full voicing of initial
and intervocalic plosives and fricatives. Observe their partial
devoicing in final position.
veal — cover — dove
that — mother — with
zebra — bosom — doze
giraffe — pleasure — garage
bad — rubber — mob
dig — lady — bed
goal — eager — bag
1
#6. Read the following words and give their broad and narrow tran-
scription. Make distinctions between dark [l] in final positions
and before consonants, and light [l] before vowels and [j].
let leave help
tell fall value
all silk salt
#7. Read the following groups of words. Give their broad and
narrow transcription. Observe longer pronunciation of nasal
sonorants in final positions, before voiced consonants and
vowels. Keep the usual shorter pronunciation before voiceless
consonants.
sing — singing — sink
sun — sunny — send — sent
dim — lambs — mole — lamp
men — mend — many — meant
long — longer — think
#8. Read the following pairs of words. Transcribe the opposed
sounds. Discuss their relevant features.
bet — bed ten — men
make — mate tale — sale
can — cat tan — pan
tame — lame tin — bin
tool — fool teal — veal
#9. Make sure that you remember English vowel phonemes. Fill in
the following table.
horizontal movements of the tongue
vertical variations in the
front- back-
movements of height of vertical front central back
retracted advanced
the tongue movements
narrow variant
close (high)
broad variant
narrow variant
mid (half-open)
broad variant
narrow variant
open (low)
broad variant
1
#10. Write down a complete description of every English vowel pho-
neme. Enclose in brackets the characteristics, which are not
phonologically relevant.
Example: [ı] front-retracted, close, broad variant, (unrounded,
short) monophthong.
[i:] front, close, narrow variant, (unrounded, long)
diphthongoid.
[eı] diphthong, the nucleus is front, mid, narrow vari-
ant (unrounded).
#11. Read the following groups of words. Give their broad and nar-
row transcription. State differences in vowel length, tenseness
and checkness of corresponding vowels. Say if they are
relevant for phonological distinctions.
pity — pig — pit bore — bored — bought
bed — bet look — good
sad — sat mud — cut
car — card — cart fur — firm — first
mog — mock away — teacher
see — seed — seat go — goal — goat
who — soon — soup how — howl — house
lay — laid — late here — real — fierce
tie — tied — tight care — scared — scarce
toy — toys — voice cure — cured
1
Exercise Block 4
#1. Read the following words. Say what type of sound juncture is
affected. Classify types of consonant modifications. Mark them
with the help of corresponding signs.
try rotten team sixths
fried burden sit sty
press symphony miss Spain
tree John’s pit sky
small thanks tip twice
snake moon feet sweater
dry who hit question
draw cool cheese playing
horseshoe tall leap staying
cattle bar lie bark
little meet punch cargo
1
thank you cold pie that one
could you at last wrong one
in case come from rounding
big bag in fact for a month
big bat already for me
#5. Read and transcribe the following poems. Find and explain cases
of sound modifications. Mark them with the help of correspon-
ding signs.
1) Spring is here,
The glorious spring,
When young lams gamble
And little birds sing.
The fields are all green,
The trees are in bud.
Away with the snow
The rain and the mud.
2) On top of a bus in spring time,
Along the country lane,
The trees all bright with blossom,
I hear the bird refrain.
1
I see a field where lambs play,
And peeping through the grass
The little yellow primroses
Nod their petals their path.
Yes, spring time is the best time,
Everything is so gay ...
As over the hill and down the lane
The bus goes on its way.
1
#8. Find and transcribe the alternated vowel and consonant phonemes.
Give your own examples of vowel + consonant alternations.
live — life
bath — bathe
breath — breathe
loss — lose
Exercise Block 5
#1. Define the syllabic type and structure of the following words.
Underline the peak of the syllable.
ear clench spray
mat twists at
must strength act
place pie asks
1
#3. Divide the following words into three groups. Work out the
rules determining a syllable boundary for each group. Identify
the syllable boundary.
farther abduct finish
mother dreamer beloved
answer drummer lady
baby afraid scanner
beggar corner admire
#6. Study the following examples of open juncture. Turn them into
those with close juncture. Transcribe both examples and mark
them with [+].
an ice house — a nice house plum pie — plump eye
it slips — its lips fine day — find A
keep sticking — keeps ticking a name — an aim
one zone — one’s own my claim — Mike lame
1
#7. Separate the following words in orthography if it is possible. Use
the rules for syllable separation.
agreeable plumber
bored submit
writing shopgirl
brotherhood mosaic
desks swiftly
overcome postman
Exercise Block 6
#2. Accent the following polysyllabic words with two or three de-
grees of stress. Show the differences between British and Ame-
rican pronunciation models.
accelerate justify adversary
testify economize oratory
memorize functionary abdicate
ceremony enumerate nationalize
demonstrate verify legitimate
1
#3. Study the columns of the accentuation oppositions. Transcribe
and accent the words. What phonological functions of word
stress are realized? How is it connected with the phonemic
com- position of a word?
accent — to accent progress — to progress
addict — to addict forecast — to forecast
contrast — to contrast conflict — to conflict
record — to record abstract — to abstract
produce — to produce perfect — to perfect
increase — to increase patent — to patent
#5. Read the following words. Put down stress marks and state the
origin of words. Comment on the realization of rhythmical ten-
dency in English accentuation.
psychology satisfactory terrorist
umbrella stiletto violoncello
décolleté impression development
1
administration personal archaeology
characteristic infantile distance
parenthesis phenomenon volcano
#7. Arrange the following words into three groups. Put down accent
marks. Comment on the distribution of stress.
chairman give in bad-tempered
ex-husband pre-packed wristwatch
narrow-minded farther-in-law intercultural
bathroom overdone watch out
twenty-eight short-sighted underfeed
irregular vice-president kind-
hearted before-mentioned fall down
immortal
beat back misbehave pass over
girlfriend headache illiterate
good-looking well-informed baseball
inartistic sub-editor unknown
1
beamsman seventy-four ninety-five
1
ultramodern cupboard non-stop
radio-active antiseptic bring down
#9. Read the following words and put down stress marks. Bring
together the facts you know about stress patterns of English
words. Assign the following words to the appropriate pattern
and make up a logical classification.
market proceed luxuriant
international total pronounce
familiar misprint grumble again
location vice-admiral mother
aflame original over-serious
ready alike evaporate pre-heat
colour advise historian ultracritical
reason withdraw academy fortification
engine withhold political recognition
picture divide hard-working academician
1
refuge disturb light-blue patriotic
1
monitor fixate open-handed recommend
cinema dictate armchair originality
faculty surprise gas-stove composition
policy rely give up repetition
origin articulate go out constitution
symbolize biology get lost scientific
situate geometry non-final experimental
celebrate philosophy illegal accidental
clarify metallic immaterial nationality
therapist psychologist inoffensive hospitality
execute geography irrational CIS
institute photographer ex-president WHO
classroom efficient remake WTO
roundabout essential reorganize UNSC
oval-shaped habitual underdone OSCE
before courageous antiwar IMF
behave delicious subsection LSE
Exercise Block 7
1
4) Becky likes wild violets. — No, you are wrong. She doesn’t
like wild violets. She likes garden violets.
5) Helen’s daughter is a doctor. Betty’s son isn’t a doctor. He is a
sailor.
#2. Read and intone the following sentences. Observe intonation of
enumeration.
1) June, July and August are summer months.
2) The dog is in the garden and the cat is on the chair.
3) I’m a part-time worker. Monday, Wednesday and Friday are
my working days.
4) One, two, three, four, five
Tommy caught a fish alive.
Six, seven, eight, nine, ten
Johny let it go again.
#4. Read and intone the sentences given below. Observe differences
in the intonation of general and special questions.
1) Is this a cotton dress? — No, it isn’t. It’s a synthetic one.
2) What is there in the bag? — It is a new collection of nursery
rhymes.
1
3) Do you really like her new style?! — I really do. She looks
quite nice.
4) Where is Mr. Blake? Is he in the park? — No, he isn’t. Mr.
Blake is at the Institute.
5) Whose things are these? — Tom’s. These are Tom’s things.
6) Will you put onions in the soup? — No, I won’t. I don’t like
onions. I’ll take a clove of garlic.
7) Who is that lean man? — That’s Roger. He’s been in
hospital for three weeks already.
8) How many guests are there at the party? — There are six of
them.
#9. Read the following sentences. Make the words and phrases set
in italics sound emotional with the help of special nuclear
tones. Intone the sentences and state the attitude conveyed.
1) What are you going to do now? — What am I going to do...? I
don’t know yet.
2) I thought you two are going to marry. — No, you’re talking
nonsense!
3) Are you going to visit Mike in hospital? — Of course I will!
1
4) What an extraordinary piece of luck! To see Ben in a place like
this!
1
P A R T III
Suplementary
Material
1
Supplement 1
Proverbs, sayings, tongue twisters
1. Vowel drills
[o:]
— Paul snores worse than a horse,
We close the door when he snores,
There ought to be laws to prevent such snores.
— Better unborn than untaught.
[ǩu]
— Don’t poke your nose in the things you don’t know.
— When in Rome, do as the Romans do.
— Ask no questions and you’ll be told no lies.
[au]
— Snow came in the night without a sound,
Like a white cloud trembling down to the ground.
— Out of sight, out of mind.
[aı]
— A stitch in time saves nine.
— Strike while the iron is hot.
[a:]
He laughs best who laughs last.
[ɔ]
— Honesty is the best policy.
— A little pot is soon hot.
— A watched pot never boils.
— Joy and sorrow are as near as today and tomorrow.
1
[]
— When your work is done, come out in the sun, and have
some fun.
— Some love onions for lunch or supper,
But when one has stuffed oneself with onions,
One isn’t much loved, is one?
— The tongue is not steel, but it cuts.
— What is done, cannot be undone.
[ǩ:]
— First come, first served.
— An early bird catches the worm.
— As the workman so the work.
— A bird in the hand is worth two in the bush.
[eı]
— He, who makes no mistakes, makes nothing.
— Haste makes waste.
[u:]
— Exceptions prove the rule.
— Too good to be true.
[e]
— Better late than never.
— So many men — so many minds.
— All’s well that ends well.
[i:]
Each teacher needs to be free to teach as he pleases.
[æ]
Handsome is as handsome does.
[ı]
There is no use crying over spilt milk.
1
[e] — [æ]
The devil is not so black as he is painted
[i:] — [ı]
— A friend in need is a friend indeed.
— Honey is sweet, but the bee stings.
— Eat at pleasure, drink with measure.
— People meet, but mountains never greet.
— Still waters run deep.
[a:] — []
— Well begun is half done.
— What the heart thinks, the tongue speaks.
[u:] — [u]
— The proof of the pudding is in the eating.
— Too many cooks spoil the broth.
[u] — [ju:]
— No news — good news.
— A new broom sweeps
clean. [ɔ] — [o:]
— Never put off till tomorrow what you can do today.
— You can bring the horse to water, but you can’t make it
drink.
[aı] — [eı]
— Get a name to rise early and you may lie all day.
— Make hay while the sun shines.
2. Consonant drills
[k]
— Critical cricket critic.
— Can you can a can as a canner can can a can?
1
[b]
Betty Botter bought a bit of butter,
But the butter Betty bought was bitter,
Betty bought another bit of butter
To make the bitter butter better,
But the butter Betty bought was also bitter.
[w
] Why do you cry, Willy? Why do you cry?
Why, Willy? Why, Willy? Why, Willy, why?
1
[θ] — [ð]
The thirty-three thieves thought
That they thrilled the throne throughout Thursday.
[k] — [g]
How many cookies could a good cook cook,
If a good cook could cook cookies?
A good cook could cook as much cookies
As a good cook who could cook cookies.
[h] — [g]
How much ground would a groundhog hog,
If a groundhog could hog ground?
A groundhog would hog all the ground he could hog,
If a groundhog could hog ground.
[t] — [tw]
Mr. Tongue Twister tried to train his tongue
To twist and turn, and twit and twat, to learn the letter ‘T’.
[d] — [b]
In a double bubble gum bubbles double.
[f] — [l]
Love is a feeling you feel when you feel
You’re going to feel the feeling you’ve never felt before.
1
— Spades for digging, pens for writing,
Ears for hearing, teeth for biting,
Eyes for seeing, legs for walking,
Tongues for tasting and for talking.
— Big bells ring a long full song,
Ding-dong, ding-dong!
Hear the ringing, hear the song,
Ting-a-ling, ding-ding, dong-dong.
— One-one was a race horse,
Two-two was one too,
One-one won one race,
Two-two won one too.
— Yellow butter, purple jelly, red jam, black bread.
Spread it thick, say it quick!
Yellow butter, purple jelly, red jam, black bread.
Spread it thicker, say it quicker!
Yellow butter, purple jelly, red jam, black bread.
Don’t speak with your mouth full!
— If you understand, say ‘understand’,
If you don’t understand, say ‘don’t understand’.
But if you understand and say ‘don’t understand’,
How do I understand that you understand? Understand?!
— I know a boy named Tate
Who dined with his girl at eight.
I’m unable to state what Tate ate at eight
Or what Tate’s tête à tête ate at eight.
— There was an old man with a nose,
Who said: “If you choose to suppose
That my nose is too long,
You are certainly wrong.”
That remarkable man with a nose.
— Dr. Johnson and Mr. Jackson after a great consideration,
Came to the conclusion that the Indian nation
Is back in education because the chief occupation is cultivation.
1
Supplement 2
Phonetic phenomena
2. Aspiration.
Occlusive stops [p, t, k] in the initial position in a stressed syllable
are accompanied with aspiration. Aspiration is a strong puff of
breath in a voiceless interval after the explosion of [p, t, k]. There
are three degrees of aspiration:
1) it is very strong before a long vowel or a diphthong (port, pale);
2) it is weaker before a short vowel (pit);
3) it is less noticeable before an unstressed vowel (joker) or in the
final position (look).
When [p, t, k] are preceded by [s] (sky) or followed by a noise
conso- nant (looked), there’s hardly any aspiration at all. For
example:
tall, tale, till, baker.
[to:l], [teıl], [tıl], ['beıkǩ]
1 1 2 3
3. Loss of plosion.
Occlusive consonants [p, b, t, d, k, g] lose plosion if they are
1
followed by another occlusive or affricates [t∫, ʤ]. The first
plosive loses its
1
explosion and becomes unreleased, instead of the release a pause is
heard. They also lose plosion when preceded by [s].
Eg: and dad [ǩnd dæd], that tape [ðæt teıp], fact [fækt], scale [skeıl]
4. Lateral plosion.
A plosive, preceding the lateral sonorant, becomes laterally exploded:
it has the explosion during the pronunciation of the sonorant [l]. The
re- lease before [l] is made by a sudden lowering of the sides of the
tongue, and the air escapes along the lowered sides with lateral
plosion.
Eg. please [pli:z], cattle [kætl], apple [‘æpl]
5. Nasal plosion.
When a plosive is followed by the syllabic [m, n], it has no release
of its own, and the so-called nasal plosion is produced. A plosive
be- comes nasally exploded: its explosion is produced during the
pronun- ciation of the sonorant [m] or [n].
Eg. happen [‘hæpn], kitten [‘kıtn], submarine [ısbmǩ’ri:n]
6. Assimilation.
The articulation of one sound affects the articulation of the neigh-
bouring one assimilating the latter. There are four types of assimila-
tion:
1) assimilation affecting the direction;
2) assimilation affecting the place of obstruction;
3) assimilation affecting the position of the lips;
4) assimilation affecting the work of the vocal cords.
The first type is divided into three subtypes: progressive, regressive,
and double (reciprocal) assimilation.
a) Progressive assimilation happens when the preceding sound af-
fects the articulation of following one, and the preceding sound
1
remains unchanged. For example:
1
looked, opened, cats, dogs.
→ → → →
[lukt], ['ǩupnd], [kæts], [dɔgz]
7. Wrong assimilation.
Foreign speakers shouldn’t voice the voiceless consonant which is
followed by the voiced one. They correspondingly shouldn’t
devoice the voiced consonant which is followed by the voiceless
one. For example
sit down, these socks.
[sıt daun], [ði:z sOks]
= = = =
8. Syllabic sonorants.
In unstressed final positions sonorants [l, m, n] become syllabic if
preceded by a noise consonant. For example:
cattle, sudden, rhythm.
['kætl], ['sdn], ['rıðm]
· · ·
1
9. Linking.
Instances of linking occur at word boundaries between two vowels
or a consonant with a following vowel. Here belongs the
phenomenon of linking [r] which reveals its potential pronunciation.
Eg: thisisa boy; carowner,
Supplement 3
Stave representation of intonation
1
Notional parts of speech (nouns, main verbs, adjectives, adverbs,
nu- merals, interrogative and demonstrative pronouns, interjections) are
gene- rally stressed. Functional parts of speech (modal verbs, auxiliary
verbs, link verbs, articles, prepositions, conjunctions, particles, personal
and posses- sive pronouns) are generally unstressed. But all modal and
auxiliary verbs are stressed in the negative form. Sometimes structural
parts of speech may be stressed for additional emphasis and emotional
colouring.
There are six main types of nucleus necessary for practical language
learning: Low Fall, Low Rise, High Fall, High Rise, Fall-Rise and Rise-
Fall.
Low Fall starts at the middle and then falls to a very low pitch level.
It is used in statements and special questions.
Eg: \ No.
Low Rise starts from a very low pitch level and then continues to a
medium one. It is used in general questions and the tags of disjunctive
questions.
Eg: / No.
In High Fall the voice falls all the way down from a higher to a very
low pitch level.
Eg: \ No.
In High Rise the voice rises from a medium pitch level and then
moves up to the top.
Eg: / No.
1
In Fall-Rise the voice first falls from a medium to a low pitch level
and then rises to a moderately medium pitch. It may be used within one
syllable or spread over two or more syllables. It is used in requests.
Eg: \/ No.
\
Ne/ver.
\
Generally / I do.
•
•••
In Rise-Fall the voice rises from a medium to a higher pitch level and
then quickly falls to a low pitch.
Eg: /\ No.
Supplement 4
Step-by-step phonetic analysis
1
This is a book, isn’t it?
1. Step one. Transcribe the sentence and show its end by putting down
two vertical lines at the end.
Eg: [ðıs ız ǩ buk ıznt ıt || ]
2. Step two. Define the communicative and syntactical type of the sen-
tence, i.e. see whether it is a statement, an order, a request, an excla-
mation, a question (state the type of a question); and consequently
define the nuclear tone of the sentence.
Eg: it is a disjunctive question; the rising tone is used in the second
part (tail), the falling tone is used in the first part (statement).
3. Step three. Divide the sentence into sense-groups if possible, and
separate them from each other by a vertical line. Separate parts of
sense-groups by a wavy line in case there is a very short meaningful
pause.
Eg: [ðıs ız ǩ buk | ıznt ıt || ]
4. Step four. Define the nucleus of the sentence or of every sense-
group. Put down the necessary tone mark before the stressed
syllable of the nuclear word(s).
Eg: [ðıs ız ǩ \ buk | / ıznt ıt || ]
5. Step five. See if there is a word to emphasize in the sentence (sense-
groups). If there is one, put the necessary mark before its stressed
syllable (↑ or ↓) to show the emphasis.
Eg: There are no emphasized words in the sentence.
6. Step six. Define all the other stressed words in the sentence and put
down stress marks (') before their stressed syllables.
['ðıs ız ǩ \ buk | / ıznt ıt || ]
7. Step seven. Intone the sentence graphically at the stave.
1
8. Step eight. Define all the phoneme clusters in the words and at word
boundaries. Mark sound modifications with the help of conventional
symbols.
∩
['ðısızǩ \ buk | /
ızntıt || ]
3 3 3
1
BIBLIOGRAPHY
1
16. Леонтьева С.Ф. Теоретическая фонетика современного английского языка
(учебник на английском языке). — М., 2004.
17. Лукина Н.Д. Практический курс фонетики английского языка. — М.,
2003.
18. Михальчук В.А., Лисенко М.В. Практическая фонетика английского язы-
ка. — СПб., 2001.
19. Семнадцать акцентов. Варианты разговорного английского / под ред.
А.И. Пахотина. — М., 2004.
20. Соколова М.А., Афонская И.А., Ковалева Л.Б., Крылова Н.И. Теоретиче-
ская фонетика английского языка: практикум. — М., 1997.
21. Соколова М.А., Гинтовт К.П., Кантер Л.А., Крылова Н.И., Тихонова И.С.,
Шабадаш Г.А. Практическая фонетика английского языка. — М., 1997.
22. Соколова М.А., Гинтовт К.П., Тихонова И.С., Тихонова Р.М. Теоретиче-
ская фонетика английского языка (на английском языке). — М., 1991.
23. Торсуев Г.П. Фонетика английского языка. — М., 1950.
24. Трубецкой Н.С. Основы фонологии. — М., 1960.
25. Шахбагова Д.А. Фонетическая система английского языка в синхронии и
диахронии (на материале британского, американского, австралийского,
ка- надского вариантов английского языка). — М., 1992.
26. Швейцер А.Д. Литературный английский язык в США и Англии. — М.,
1971.
27. Шевякова В.Е. Коррективный фонетический курс английского языка. —
М., 1968.
28. Arnold G.F. Stress in English Words. — Amsterdam, 1957.
29. Backer A. Ship or Sheep? — Cambridge Univ. Press, 1985.
30. Bloch M.Y. A Course in Theoretical English Grammar. — M., 2000.
31. Bloch M.Y., Semionova T.N., Timifeyeva S.V. Theoretical English Grammar.
Seminars. — M., 2004.
32. Brazil D., Hewings M., Cauldwell R. The Communicative Value of Intonation
in English. — Cambridge Univ. Press, 1997.
33. Cruttenden A. Intonation. 2nd ed. — Cambridge Univ. Press, 1986, 1997.
34. Crystal D., Davy D. Investigating English Style. — Lnd: Longman, 1969.
35. Crystal D. Prosodic Systems and Intonation in English. — Cambridge Univ.
Press, 1969.
36. Cunningham S., Moor P. Cutting Edge. — Edinburgh, 2003.
1
37. Fries Ch. Linguistics and Reading. — N.Y., 1965.
38. Gimson A.C. An Introduction to the Pronunciation of English. — Lnd., 1981.
39. Ginsburg R.S., Khidekel S.S., Knyazeva G.Y., Sankin A.A. A Course in
Modern English Lexicology. — M., 1966.
40. Gussenhoven C. The Phonology of Tone and Intonation (Research Surveys in
Linguistics). — Cambridge Univ. Press, 2004.
41. Halliday M.A.K. Intonation and Grammar in British English. — The
Hague: Mouton, 1967.
42. Halliday M.A.K. Intonation Systems in English: Patterns of Language. —
Lnd., 1966.
43. Hayes B. Introductory Phonology. — Oxford: Blackwell Publishers, 2008.
44. Hughes A., Trudgill P. English Accents and Dialects. — Lnd., 1979.
45. Jespersen O. English Phonetics. — Copenhagen, 1950.
46. Jones D. An Outline of English Phonetics. 9th Ed. — Leipzig: Teubner, 1918, 1960.
47. Jones D. English Pronouncing Dictionary. — Cambridge, 2007.
48. Kenyon J.S. American Pronunciation. — Michigan: Ann Arbor, 1953.
49. Kingdon R. The Groundwork of English Intonation. — Lnd., 1958.
50. Ladd D.R. Intonational Phonology. — Cambridge Univ. Press, 1996.
51. Laver J. Principles of Phonetics. — Cambridge Univ. Press, 1994.
52. Merkulova Y.M. English for University Students: Introduction to Phonetics. —
Spb., 2000.
53. Mortimer C. Elements of Pronunciation. — Cambridge, 1995.
54. Pike K.L. The Intonation of American English. — Michigan: Ann Arbor,
1945.
55. Pike K. Phonetics. — Michigan: Ann Arbor, 1943.
56. Roach P. English Phonetics and Phonology: A Practical Course. 4th Ed. —
Cam- bridge Univ. Press, 2009.
57. Sweet H. A Primer of Phonetics. — Oxford, 1929.
58. Trudgill P., Hannah J. International English. — Lnd., 1982.
59. Vassilyev V.A. English Phonetics: A Theoretical Course. — M., 1970.
60. Wells J.C. Accents of English. Vol.3. Beyond the British Isles. — Cambridge:
Cambridge Univ. Press, 1982.
61. Wells J.C. English Intonation: An Introduction. — Cambridge Univ. Press, 2006.
62. Wells J.C. Longman Pronouncing Dictionary. — N.Y., 2006.
1
Учебное издание
ТЕОРЕТИЧЕСКАЯ ФОНЕТИКА
АНГЛИЙСКОГО ЯЗЫКА
1
Подписано в печать 17.08.2011. Формат 60 88/16. Печать офсетная.
Усл. печ. л. 10,3. Уч.-изд. л. 6,4. Тираж 1000 экз. Изд. № 2318.
1
ИЗДATEЛ@CTBO «ФЛИHTA/
cyyecmвyem na кnu+nom pыnкe c 1996 s. Bo «Фnunme» e+e-
soðno выxoðum бonee 2QQ кnus. Nзðamenbcmвo cneyuanuзupyemcя na
выnycкe numepamypы ðnя вyзoв в ocnoвnom symanumapnoso npo-
Øunя: yneбnuкu,yneбnыe u memoðunecкue nocoбuя, xpecmomamuu,
coвpemennыe cnoвapu u cnpaвonnuкu. Bcя yneбnaя numepamypa
noðsomoвnena в coomвemcmвuu c socyðapcmвennыmu oбpaзoвa-
menbnыmu cmanðapmamu. Gpeðu aвmopoв — вeðyyue ynenыe u
npenoðaвamenu uз Mocквы, ðpysux sopoðoв Poccuu u зapyбe+bя.
Бonbnuncmвo yneбnuкoв u yneбnыx nocoбuǔ npoxoðяm зкc-
nepmuзy u umeαm spuØы Munucmepcmвa oбpaзoвanuя u nayкu
PФ unu coomвemcmвyαyux yneбno-memoðunecкux
oбaeðunenuǔ.
BEДY1ИE TEMATИuECKИE HAMPABЛEHИЯ:
• Peuь, 'sыk, o6щeниe
• Pитopиka
• Pyccka' литepaтypa и литepaтypobeдeниe
• 3apy6eжнa' литepaтypa
• Pycckий 'sыk kak инocтpaнный
• Ncтopи' жypнaлиcтиkи
• Лaтинckий 'sыk (cepи' LINGUA LATINA)
• Nнocтpaнный 'sыk
• McиxoлoFи', пeдaFoFиka
• BaлeoлoFи'
• @иsнec
• MoлитoлoFи', coциoлoFи', филocoфи', kyльтypoлoFи'
• 3koнomиka
• Юpиcпpyдeнци'
• Maтemaтиka, фиsиka, инфopmaтиka, тexниueckиe нaykи
Пpu#nauaen н compyднuuecmвy
• 6и6лиoтekи, kниFoтopFobыe opFaниsaции
• abтopob yue6нoй литepaтypы
Bыnonняen зaнaзы
пo иsдaнию moнoFpaфий, нayuныx тpyдob и дpyFиx пeuaтныx
pa6oт нa дoFobopныx нauaлax.
HAШИ KOOPДИHATЫ:
Ten./Øaкc: (495) 334-82-65,men.: (495) 336-Q3-11
E-mail: flinta@mail.ru,WebSite: www.flinta.ru