Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 7

AUTHOR’S COPY | AUTORENEXEMPLAR

Rev. Neurosci., Vol. 22(3): 365–371, 2011 • Copyright © by Walter de Gruyter • Berlin • Boston. DOI 10.1515/RNS.2011.024

Spontaneously hypertensive rat (SHR) as an animal model for


ADHD: a short overview

Alfredo Meneses1,*, Georgina Perez-Garcia, Teresa disorders (Millan, 2008). ADHD represents a focal point in
Ponce-Lopez1, Ruth Tellez1, Andrea Gallegos-Cari and this context.
Carlos Castillo2,*
1
Department of Pharmacobiology, CINVESTAV, Tenorios Modeling ADHD
235, Granjas Coapa, Mexico City 14330, Mexico
2
Escuela Superior de Medicina, Instituto Politécnico Interestingly, a Medline search (September, 2010) for ‘ADHD
Nacional, Plan de San Luis y Díaz Mirón, Colonia Santo and drugs and animal models’ yielded 71 papers, including
Tomás, México City 11340, México 22 reviews. More recently (March or April, 2011) the same
*Corresponding authors phrasing elicited 77 or 81 papers, including 23 or 24 reviews.
e-mail: ameneses@msn.com; drcarloscastillo@hotmail.com As regarding the latter there are many excellent papers, and
hence the aim of this work is to present a brief overview with a
particular focus on the spontaneously hypertensive rat (SHR)
Abstract as an animal model for ADHD. Hence, available evidence is
analyzed and discussed. Russell (2007) summarized that con-
Diverse studies indicate that attention-deficit hyperactivity sistent with ADHD being a developmental disorder, animal
disorder (ADHD) is associated with alterations in encoding models are either genetic (SHR), dopamine transporter (DAT)
processes, including working or short-term memory. Some knock-out mice, SNAP-25 mutant mice, mice expressing a
ADHD dysfunctional domains are reflected in the spontane- mutant thyroid receptor or have suffered an insult to the cen-
ously hypertensive rat (SHR). Because ADHD, drugs and tral nervous system during the early stages of development
animal models are eliciting a growing interest, hence the (anoxia, 6-hydroxydopamine). By contrast, neuropsychologi-
aim of this work is to present a brief overview with a focus cal and imaging studies indicate that ADHD is associated with
on the SHR as an animal model for ADHD and memory alterations on information encoding processes such as those
deficits. Thus, this paper reviews the concept of SHR as a observed on children with these conditions (Denckla, 1996).
model system for ADHD, comparing SHR, Wistar-Kyoto and The dysfunctional areas identified by diagnostic schemes
Sprague-Dawley rats with a focus on the hypertension level include executive and attention-related abilities (poor concen-
and working, short-term memory and attention in different tration), increased motor activity (restlessness), and cognitive
behavioral tasks, such as open field, five choice serial reac- and behavioral impulsivity (ill-considered responses) (Oades
tion time, water maze, passive avoidance, and autoshaping. In et al., 2005). ADHD alterations include deficit in behavioral
addition, drug treatments (d-amphetamine and methylpheni- inhibition (Barkley, 1997), prefrontal cortex (PFC) and to its
date) are evaluated. striatum connections (Brennan and Arnsten, 2008), dysfunc-
tion in dopaminergic, noradrenergic (Oades et al., 2005), cho-
Keywords: attention; attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder linergic (Terry et al., 2000) and serotonergic pathways (Wilens
(ADHD); autoshaping; d-amphetamine; hyperactivity; long- et al., 2008). It should be noted that approximately one-half of
term memory (LTM); memory; rats; short-term memory ADHD patients could have working memory problems (Paule
(STM); spontaneously hypertensive rat (SHR); Sprague- et al., 2000) and as a group variability is constant.
Dawley; Wistar-Kyoto.

ADHD and animal models


Introduction
Some ADHD characteristic symptoms have been observed in
Growing interest on memory dysfunction in diverse dis- animal models (Paule et al., 2000), including the SHR, which
orders, including Alzheimer ’s disease, schizophrenia, has been proposed as an animal model for ADHD (Oades,
Parkinson disease, diabetes, attention-deficit hyperactiv- 1987; Wultz et al., 1990; Paule et al., 2000; Sagvolden, 2000;
ity disorder (ADHD), post-traumatic stress disorder, etc., Adriani et al., 2003; Russell, 2003; nevertheless, see van
suggests that memory is either a promiscuous or specific den Bergh et al., 2006; Ferguson et al., 2007; Robertson et
function of the brain. Wherever the nature and function of al., 2008) for its characterization as a disorder (Oades, 1987;
this cognitive process might be, the study of memory for- Russell, 2000; Sagvolden, 2000; Sagvolden et al., 2005;
mation and diverse brain disorders has been producing key Sagvolden and Xu, 2008), and to determine memory altera-
information. In this regard, animal models are very impor- tions related to hypertension (Meneses and Hong, 1998; Terry
tant, particularly as animal models for, not of, psychiatric et al., 2000), but not exclusively. Importantly, Wistar-Kyoto

AUTHOR’S COPY | AUTORENEXEMPLAR


AUTHOR’S COPY | AUTORENEXEMPLAR

366 A. Meneses et al.

(WKY) rats are considered the control inbreed strain against water maze acquisition in both WKY and SHR rats (De Bruin
SHR, and Sprague-Dawley (SD) rats are considered as an et al., 2003). Certainly, the uses of the WKY strain as the ‘con-
outbreed strain with different genetic backgrounds (Oades, trol’ for the SHRs have been questioned (Terry et al., 2000;
1987; Sagvolden, 2000; Russell, 2007). Actually male SD rats Ferguson et al., 2007; Robertson et al., 2008). Although SHRs
with chronic hypertension showed that severe hypertension are a genetic model with neural disturbances, they are not nec-
for over 10 months is not sufficient to impair spatial learn- essarily or exclusively related to hypertension per se (Meneses
ing and memory in the water maze in otherwise normal rats and Hong, 1998; Diana, 2002; De Bruin et al., 2003).
(Kadish et al., 2001). And an animal model of ADHD is repre-
sented by SD-derived Naples High Excitability rats (Viggiano
et al., 2002, 2003). Likewise, male middle-aged WKY and WKY and SHR behavioral characteristics
SHR groups chronically treated with nimodipine (a calcium
Interestingly, questions have been raised about the extent to
channel blocker) showed improved memory relative to their
which SHRs represent ADHD (van den Bergh et al., 2006),
control groups, whereas in the latter the drug decreased hyper-
and that WKY rats are not appropriated controls of SHRs in
tension and maintained it at normal levels during 4 weeks, in
numerous behavioral features (Clements and Wainwright,
the former it worked only during the first 2 weeks (Meneses et
2006; Ferguson et al., 2007; Grundt et al., 2009), including
al., 1997; Meneses and Hong, 1998). In another study, it was
locomotor activity. Indeed, whereas SHRs are hyperactive,
observed that acute hypertension in male rats induced by the
WKY rats are hypoactive (Alsop, 2007), even the latter have
deoxycorticosterine acetate-salt method produced impairment
been used as an animal model of depression (Robertson et al.,
of short-term memory (STM) in the passive avoidance task,
2008). In addition, SHRs regarding WKY rats present sev-
but did not affect spatial learning and memory in the water
eral genetic and neurochemical differences (Johnson et al.,
maze task (Ghavipanjeh et al., 2010). SHR features hyperac-
1995; Drolet et al., 2002; see also Grundt et al., 2009). For
tivity, impulsivity, poor sustained attention and learning and
instance, SHRs have a Y chromosome locus that increases
memory (Meneses et al., 1996; Meneses and Hong, 1998;
blood pressure and this locus requires an androgen receptor
Diana, 2002; Heal et al., 2008; Meneses et al., 2009; Wells et
and testosterone for maximum expression as well as having
al., 2010; but see Robertson et al., 2008). SHR also presents
significantly higher steroid sulfatase activity in testes, adre-
disturbances in glutamate, dopamine and norepinephrine func-
nal gland, liver, and hypothalamus (Johnson et al., 1995).
tion, which parallel evidence that ADHD patients could have
Sometimes even WKY rats could present hypertension
defects on neuronal circuits required for reward-guided asso-
(Louis and Howes, 1990) and even though medial temporal
ciative learning and memory formation (Oades et al., 2005;
lobe functioning appears to be normal in SHRs exhibiting an
Heal et al., 2008; Wells et al., 2010; but see Robertson et al.,
ADHD-like phenotype, WKY hypertensive (WKHT) male
2008), including dopamine and norepinephrine release and
rats display both hippocampal functioning deficits and signs
impaired second messenger systems (Russell, 2000). of bilateral hippocampal cell loss (Wells et al., 2010).

SHR and memory tasks Memory, behavioral tasks and cognitive demand

Operant behavioral tasks modeling learning, STM and simple Deficits in temporal processing have been implicated in
discriminations have been useful to detect behavioral features ADHD. Thus, in a study, adult males and females of SHR,
of ADHD and normalization of performance on working mem- WKY and SD strains were compared in two timing tasks,
ory (WM) and STM by stimulant drugs such as methylpheni- temporal response differentiation (TRD) and differential rein-
date (Paule et al., 2000). Similarly, open field test, Pavlovian/ forcement of low rates (DRL) (Ferguson et al., 2007), indicat-
instrumental autoshaping (Meneses and Hong, 1998) and spa- ing that strains did not differ in task acquisition; however, the
tial memory in the water maze (Terry et al., 2000) tasks have steady state of TRD and DRL performance of the SHR and
been useful to detect the effects of drugs on adult male SHRs. WKY strains was less accurate, which was related to increased
For example, in the water maze, autoshaping (Meneses et al., burst (non-timing related) responses. Subsequently, methyl-
1997) and open field tests (Heal et al., 2008; Wells et al., 2010), phenidate (1.0–12.0 mg/kg) or d-amphetamine (0.1–2.0 mg/kg)
with 3, 6, 9, 12, 18, or 24 months (Meneses et al., 1997) or pretesting treatment showed that both drugs disrupted non-
3 months (Heal et al., 2008; Wells et al., 2010) old male SHRs, significantly TRD and DRL performances. Strain differences
have shown an age-related impairment on learning, memory, were generally maintained throughout the drug and extinction
and spontaneous locomotor activity relative to normotensive sections of the study. Ferguson et al. (2007) concluded that their
WKY saline rats. Even male SHR and WKY rats have shown results do not support continued use of the SHR as a model for
memory deficits relative to SD rats in the water maze task ADHD. Additionally, Robertson et al. (2008) showed that the
(Wells et al., 2010) or SD rats in an autoshaping test (Meneses STM capabilities of male SHRs, that are necessary for WM
et al., 2011a). Although the memory deficits are partially or performance, appear to be intact, and the evidence of executive
completely reversed by the calcium channel blocker nimo- deficits related to WM functions was limited (observable only
dipine in autoshaping task (Meneses et al., 1997), uridine and in the massed trials and were also evident in the WKY control
choline supplementation normalizes attention of SHR in the group) in the radial water maze configured as a T-maze and
five choice serial reaction time (5-CSRT) test and improves delayed matching-to-place task. Once again, these findings do

AUTHOR’S COPY | AUTORENEXEMPLAR


AUTHOR’S COPY | AUTORENEXEMPLAR

ADHD, SHR, D -amphetamine and memory 367

not support the use of SHRs as a model to address the con- strategy than SHRs or WKY rats during an early probe test
dition termed ADHD (Robertson et al., 2008). Nonetheless, on day 8. During a later probe test on day 24, WKY rats pro-
Sagvolden (2000), who pioneered the testing of SHRs in gressed significantly from using a place strategy to a response
behavioral and memory tasks, has highlighted that a good strategy. Throughout all probe tests, special strategy was used
model mimics the fundamentals of the behavioral characteris- predominately by SHRs and a habit strategy by WKHT rats
tics of ADHD, conforms to a theoretical rationale for it and is (Wells et al., 2010). Thus, SHRs exhibited deficits in dorsal
able to predict aspects of ADHD. Importantly, although other striatum-related habit learning, whereas WKHT rats exhib-
strains and species could be hyperactive and/or show atten- ited deficits in hippocampus-related spatial learning (Wells
tion deficits following genetic, environmental or pharmaco- et al., 2010). It should be nevertheless noted that when the
logical interventions, the SHRs are presently the only strain integration of information derived from sign (i.e., the action
that have shown the major behavioral symptoms of ADHD, of stimulus-stimulus, classical conditioning) and goal track-
suggesting that this does not mean that research with other ing (i.e., response-stimulus, instrumental/operant condition-
models cannot provide valuable information (Sagvolden and ing) settings (Meneses, 2003; Tellez and Meneses, 2009;
Xu, 2008). For instance, male SHRs, WKY and SD rats were Tellez et al., 2010; Meneses et al., 2011a,b,c) are tested on an
assessed in spatial memory as measured of their performance autoshaping task, the adult male SHR-saline group showed
in a win-shift version of the water radial arm maze and the poor STM and long-term memory (LTM) relative to SD and
results showed that all strains made more errors on trial 4, WKY vehicle rats (Meneses et al., 2011a). By contrast, male
when the mnemonic demand was highest, and they showed a SD and WKY rats treated with d-amphetamine displayed bet-
similar response when the delay was increased from 60 s to ter STM and LTM, compared with SD-vehicle, WKY-vehicle
2 h on week 3 (Clements and Wainwright, 2006). Both SHRs or SHR-d-amphetamine groups; and the latter showed better
and WKY rats made more reference memory errors than SD STM and LTM relative to the SHR-vehicle group (Johnson et
rats; however, SHRs showed minimal improvement over al., 1995). That this autoshaping protocol reflects STM and
weeks. According to the same authors, the increase in errors LTM is supported by the evidence that it engages hippocam-
could be due to a greater inclination of SHRs and WKY pus (mediating declarative memory) and striatum (mediating
rats to use a chaining strategy of entering consecutive arms stimulus-response ‘habit’ formation) (Meneses, 2003; Tellez
than SD rats. Furthermore, the number of incomplete arm and Meneses, 2009; Tellez et al., 2010), respectively, and by
entries into reference memory arms decreased over weeks the forgetting curve (i.e., decreased performance), observable
in WKY and SD rats, but increased in SHRs, suggesting an when autoshaping training and testing sessions are interrupted
increased impulsivity of SHRs at the later stages of testing. by a period larger than 48–72 h (Tellez and Meneses, 2009).
Although, based on number of errors, the data indicate that Likewise, the contingent or 6 h later (following training), inhi-
SHRs could have memory deficits, the data relating to arm bition of hippocampal protein synthesis or new mRNA did
entries suggest that the minimal improvement in SHRs over not affect autoshaping STM but it specifically impaired LTM
weeks could have been due to greater impulsivity in the later performance (Izquierdo et al., 1998; Meneses, 2003, 2007a;
weeks, rather than defective memory. Thus, these findings Tellez and Meneses, 2009; Tellez et al., 2010; Meneses et al.,
are consistent with SHRs having impairments with a selec- 2011b,c); therefore, the procedure seems to be valid in the
tion of appropriate behavioral responses in a goal-directed temporal and molecular bases for STM and LTM study. As
task (Clements and Wainwright, 2006). Moreover, male autoshaping STM and LTM are mediated by the PFC and hip-
SHRs vs. WKY and SD rats were compared in the operant pocampus, respectively (Meneses, 2003; Liy-Salmeron and
delayed non-matching-to-position (DNMTP) test for study- Meneses, 2007, 2008; Meneses, 2007a,b; Tellez and Meneses,
ing STM, 5-CSRT task for assessing selective visual atten- 2009; Tellez et al., 2010; Meneses et al., 2011b,c), which is
tion processes, and the water maze for spatial learning and consistent with the evidence that SHRs reflect diminished
mnemonic capabilities, and the findings revealed impaired habituation and severe deficits in attention, learning, and
attention (De Bruin et al., 2003). Both the SHRs and WKY memory (see above), which are probably related to (at least)
rats showed deficits in spatial learning when compared with a dysfunctional PFC. Nevertheless, when WM was assessed
the SD rats. Thus, SHRs have a deficiency in visual selective in the water maze with different strategic demands, including
attention and spatial learning. the delayed matching-to-place task (DMP), the performance
of male SHRs did not differ from that of either of SD or WKY
rats (Robertson et al., 2008). As already mentioned, Robertson
Spatial vs. habit learning and memory et al. (2008) concluded that their findings do not provide fur-
ther support of STM impairments in the SHRs, and hence this
Similarly, the male WKHT (hypertensive WKY) rats, SHR strain is not appropriated in the study of ADHD in humans.
and WKY strains show similar levels of amygdala-related Notwithstanding, consistent with the notion of SHRs show-
stimulus-reward learning during conditioned cue preference ing STM and LTM impairments, others authors have reported
testing (Wells et al., 2010). In the ambiguous T-maze task, that this strain present poor memory in several memory tasks,
which dissociates between spatial (i.e., hippocampus-depen- including the water maze (Terry et al., 2000), novel object
dent) and habit learning (i.e., hippocampus-independent), recognition (Tian et al., 2009), STM in the DMP version of
WKHT rats significantly used more a response (indicative the Morris water maze (Clements et al., 2003), odor-delayed
of habit learning) vs. a place (indicative of spatial learning) win-shift (nonspatial working and reference memory), win-

AUTHOR’S COPY | AUTORENEXEMPLAR


AUTHOR’S COPY | AUTORENEXEMPLAR

368 A. Meneses et al.

stay (habit learning), and attentional set-shifting (Kantak et (Hoyer et al., 1994). Antagonists for these serotonergic recep-
al., 2008) and other tasks (Sagvolden and Xu, 2008). Taken tors could have no effects, improve memory, and/or reverse
together, these data support the notion that SHRs have poor amnesia (Meneses, 1999, 2003). The implications for this
STM and LTM relative to (in increasing order) WKY<SD rats information is more important in the context that stimulants
and provide further support to the notion that this strain repre- are currently the most common treatment for ADHD, and pre-
sents a useful tool in the study of memory deficits on ADHD. clinical and clinical studies (Zeeuws and Soetens, 2007) sug-
Certainly, to clarify the discrepant findings further studies are gest that methylphenidate and d-amphetamine could involve
necessary, which would be very insightful to investigate, e.g., activation of presynaptic inhibitory autoreceptors, reducing
loci and mechanisms. dopaminergic and noradrenergic activity (Bymaster et al.,
2002; Rosa et al., 2008). Thus, stimulants have paradoxical
calming effects in ADHD and stimulating effects on normal
Locus and memory processes individuals, and produce hyperactivity in rats. Notably low
doses of stimulants are clinically relevant (Tian et al., 2009),
That the above mentioned d-amphetamine improvement on which supports the focus on attention and improves executive
autoshaping STM and LTM might occur via stimulation of function in both normal and ADHD subjects (Stevenson and
(at least) the PFC is supported by evidence that projections Wolraich, 1989; Arnsten, 2006). Nonetheless, in humans the
present in this area (de Almeida et al., 2008; González-Burgos d-amphetamine-facilitatory effect on verbal memory does not
and Feria-Velasco, 2008) play an important role in the pro- involve modulation of the initial encoding or STM processes
cessing of mnemonic information (de Almeida et al., 2008; (Rosa et al., 2008). In the above mentioned study (Meneses et
González-Burgos and Feria-Velasco, 2008; Olvera-Cortés al., 2011a), the d-amphetamine dose previously tested com-
et al., 2008), particularly STM (Liy-Salmeron and Meneses, prises the highest possible and because at 48 h all three strains
2008). Inhibition of protein and mRNA synthesis on the PFC responded similarly, as a consequence, the use of lower doses
prevents STM without affecting LTM (Liy-Salmeron and or other times would be more insightful. For instance, low
Meneses, 2007), and as already mentioned SHRs present doses of methylphenidate reduce locomotor activity in rats
poor STM and LTM regarding SD and WKY rats, and acute and increase norepinephrine and dopamine release (Arnsten,
post-training d-amphetamine administration reverses such an 2006) in the PFC, whereas provoking only subtle effects on
effect (Meneses et al., 2011a). Hence, STM and PFC seem subcortical catecholamine release. Even some commonalities
to be affected in SHRs, which might be due to functional between ADHD and schizophrenia highlight the functional
and/or structural changes. For instance, microarray analysis importance of dopaminergic and glutamatergic neurotrans-
revealed that male SHRs relative to WKY rats up- and down- mission dysfunction in PFC and deficit memory (Marsh and
regulated genes in the PFC (Qiu et al., 2010), including genes Williams, 2006). As a consequence, several neurotransmis-
in charge of monoamines, signal transduction, etc. Inasmuch sion systems might be altered in ADHD and/or represent
as WM depends upon the integrity of the PFC (Solanto, viable therapeutic alternatives. Certainly, the monoamines
1998; Castner et al., 2004; Kibby and Cohen, 2008; Qiu et represent promising means of modulating PFC dysfunction to
al., 2010) and as this brain structure is dysfunctional in sub- ameliorate cognitive deficits in disorders such as ADHD and
jects with ADHD (Stevenson and Wolraich, 1989; Kibby and schizophrenia (Goldman-Rakic, 1996; Robbins and Arnsten,
Cohen, 2008); hence, poor STM and dysfunctional PFC in 2009).
SHRs would be useful to model parts of ADHD. Concerning
d-amphetamine and memory tasks, it should be noted that the
nature of memory task, timing, dosing and locus of admin- SHR and ADHD
istration determine if it produces improvement, impairment
or no effects (Podolski, 1998; Moncada and Viola, 2007). As mentioned in the Introduction section, ADHD is a com-
Actually, post-training administration of d-amphetamine plex central nervous system disorder characterized by
produces a dose-dependent enhancement of memory on hyperactivity, inattention, and impulsivity, and SHRs do not
autoshaping (Oscos et al., 1985, 1988; Rodriguez et al., 2008), consistently display symptoms of ADHD across the different
retention in a passive avoidance (Beatty and Rush, 1983) or tests (Chamberlain et al., 2006; van den Bergh et al., 2006);
selectively enhances spatial memory (Calabrese, 2001). The however, this strain could be useful in the study of memory
pharmacological, neural, and molecular mechanisms remain alterations observed in ADHD patients. An important prob-
unclear and future research should also explore their neural lem in this context is that WKY rats sometimes represent
and cognitive mechanisms. For instance, propranolol alone a poor control group with regard to SHRs (Chamberlain et
has no effects on retention, but antagonizes the d-amphet- al., 2006; van den Bergh et al., 2006). Nevertheless, SHRs
amine memory-enhancing effect and elevates noradrenaline as a model of ADHD offer insights about its memory com-
release (Lee and Ma, 1995). It should nevertheless be noted ponent. Thus, WKY and SD groups treated with d-amphet-
that propranolol impaired working and emotional memory amine display better STM and LTM (24 h) than SHR-vehicle
(Chamberlain et al., 2006); indeed, it is useful in the treatment or SHR-amphetamine groups (Meneses et al., 2011a). In
of post-traumatic stress disorder (Glannon, 2006). In addition, fact, the finding that poor STM in SHRs was normalized by
propranolol is commonly identified as a β-adrenergic antago- d-amphetamine tends to support the link between the PFC
nist, it also displays affinity for 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B receptors and memory. These results are novel in the sense that they

AUTHOR’S COPY | AUTORENEXEMPLAR


AUTHOR’S COPY | AUTORENEXEMPLAR

ADHD, SHR, D -amphetamine and memory 369

demonstrate that associative STM and LTM are impaired in Brennan, A.R. and Arnsten, A.F. (2008). Neuronal mechanisms
SHRs and that d-amphetamine reversed it. Likewise, these underlying attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: the influence
data to some extent confirm the previous baseline comparison of arousal on prefrontal cortical function. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.
in all outlined rats on tasks assessing timing and motivation 1129, 236–245.
Bymaster, F.P., Katner, J.S., Nelson, D.L., Hemrick-Luecke, S.K.,
and pretraining d-amphetamine administration (Ferguson et
Threlkeld, P.G., Heiligenstein, J.H., Morin, S.M., Gehlert, D.R.,
al., 2007) or learning of reinforced repetitions vs. reinforced and Perry, K.W. (2002). Atomoxetine increases extracellular lev-
variations (Mook and Neuringer, 1994). Perhaps, the more els of norepinephrine and dopamine in prefrontal cortex of rat: a
relevant issue that these data illustrate is that WKY rats show potential mechanism for efficacy in attention deficit/hyperactiv-
poor STM and LTM relative to SD rats, certainly WKY rats ity disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 27, 699–711.
are better than SHRs. Without a doubt SHRs represent a good Calabrese, E.J. (2001). Dopamine: biphasic dose responses. Crit.
model for the study of memory deficits in ADHD, primarily Rev. Toxicol. 31, 563–583.
in the context of models for particular risk factors or symp- Castner, S.A., Goldman-Rakic, P.S., and Williams, G.V. (2004).
toms, and responsiveness to specific mechanisms of drug Animal models of working memory: insights for targeting cogni-
tive dysfunction in schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology (Berl.)
action or other treatment modes (Millan, 2008) or biomarkers
174, 111–125.
useful for the diagnosis of ADHD, and also for understanding Chamberlain, S.R., Müller, U., Blackwell, A.D., Robbins, T.W., and
the pathological mechanisms for development of therapeutics Sahakian, B.J. (2006). Noradrenergic modulation of working
venues. memory and emotional memory in humans. Psychopharmacology
As different subtypes of ADHD could represent distinct (Berl.) 188, 397–407.
neurobehavioral disorders of childhood onset with sepa- Clements, K.M. and Wainwright, P.E. (2006). Spontaneously hyper-
rate etiologies and as the SHR model has some advantages, tensive, Wistar-Kyoto and Sprague-Dawley rats differ in perfor-
these fulfill some of the validation criteria (Sagvolden et mance on a win-shift task in the water radial arm maze. Behav.
al., 2008, 2009), including memory deficits, and as a conse- Brain Res. 167, 295–304.
Clements, K.M., Girard, T.A., Xing, H.C., and Wainwright, P.E.
quence it seems reasonable to use this strain and appropriate
(2003). Spontaneously hypertensive and Wistar-Kyoto rats dif-
genetic and behavioral control groups. Further experiments
fer in delayed matching-to-place performance and response to
are necessary to test lower doses of d-amphetamine and dietary long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids. Dev. Psychobiol.
other drugs on SHRs and their molecular mechanisms, par- 43, 57–69.
ticularly in the context of alterations in SHRs and ADHD de Almeida, J., Palacios, J.M., and Mengod, G. (2008). Distribution
related to a deficit in behavioral inhibition, dysfunctional of 5-HT and DA receptors in primate prefrontal cortex: implica-
PFC and striatum connections, as well as dysfunctional tions for pathophysiology and treatment. Prog. Brain Res. 172,
dopaminergic, noradrenergic, cholinergic, and serotonergic 101–115.
neurotransmission. Finally, SHRs seem to be a useful tool De Bruin, N.M., Kiliaan, A.J., De Wilde, M.C., and Broersen, L.M.
to model ADHD dysfunctional domains on memory. (2003). Combined uridine and choline administration improves
cognitive deficits in spontaneously hypertensive rats. Neurobiol.
Learn. Mem. 80, 63–79.
Denckla, M.B. (1996). Biological correlates of learning and atten-
Acknowledgments tion: what is relevant to learning disability and attention-deficit
hyperactivity disorder? J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 17, 114–119.
The authors wish to thank Sofia Meneses-Goytia for revising the
Diana, G. (2002). Does hypertension alone lead to cognitive decline
English language. Our work is supported, in part, by CONACYT
in spontaneously hypertensive rats? Behav. Brain Res. 134,
grant 80060. 113–121.
Drolet, G., Proulx, K., Pearson, D., Rochford, J., and Deschepper,
C.F. (2002). Comparisons of behavioral and neurochemical
References characteristics between WKY, WKHA, and Wistar rat strains.
Neuropsychopharmacology 27, 400–409.
Adriani, W., Caprioli, A., Granstrem, O., Carli, M., and Laviola, G. Ferguson, S.A., Paule, M.G., Cada, A., Fogle, C.M., Gray, E.P., and
(2003). The spontaneously hypertensive-rat as an animal model Berry, K.J. (2007). Baseline behavior, but not sensitivity to stim-
of ADHD: evidence for impulsive and non-impulsive subpopula- ulant drugs, differs among spontaneously hypertensive, Wistar-
tions. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 27, 639–651. Kyoto, and Sprague-Dawley rat strains. Neurotoxicol. Teratol.
Alsop, B. (2007). Problems with spontaneously hypertensive rats 29, 547–561.
(SHR) as a model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/ Ghavipanjeh, G.R., Alaei, H., Khazaei, M., Pourshanazari, A.A., and
HD). J. Neurosci. Methods 162, 42–48. Corrected and repub- Hoveida, R. (2010). Effect of acute and chronic hypertension on
lished in: J. Neurosci. Methods (2007) 166, XV–XXI. short- and long-term spatial and avoidance memory in male rats.
Arnsten, A.F. (2006). Stimulants: therapeutic actions in ADHD. Pathophysiology 17, 39–44.
Neuropsychopharmacology 31, 2376–2383. Glannon, W. (2006). Psychopharmacology and memory. J. Med.
Barkley, R.A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and Ethics 32, 74–78.
executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1996). Regional and cellular fractionation of
Psychol. Bull. 121, 65–94. working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 93, 13473–13480.
Beatty, W.W. and Rush, J.R. (1983). Retention deficit after d-am- González-Burgos, I. and Feria-Velasco, A. (2008). Serotonin/dop-
phetamine treatment: memory defect or performance change? amine interaction in memory formation. Prog. Brain Res. 172,
Behav. Neural Biol. 37, 265–275. 603–623.

AUTHOR’S COPY | AUTORENEXEMPLAR


AUTHOR’S COPY | AUTORENEXEMPLAR

370 A. Meneses et al.

Grundt, A., Grundt, C., Gorbey, S., Thomas, M.A., and Lemmer, Meneses, A., Castillo, C., Ibarra, M., and Hong, E. (1996). Effects of
B. (2009). Strain-dependent differences of restraint stress- aging and hypertension on learning, memory and activity in rats.
induced hypertension in WKY and SHR. Physiol. Behav. 97, Physiol. Behav. 60, 341–345.
341–346. Meneses, A., Terrón, J.A., Ibarra, M., and Hong, E. (1997). Effects
Heal, D.J., Smith, S.L., Kulkarni, R.S., and Rowley, H.L. (2008). of nimodipine on learning in normotensive and spontaneously
New perspectives from microdialysis studies in freely-moving, hypertensive rats. Behav. Brain Res. 85, 121–125.
spontaneously hypertensive rats on the pharmacology of drugs Meneses, A., Perez-Garcia, G., Liy-Salmeron, G., Ponce-Lopez,
for the treatment of ADHD. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 90, T., Tellez, R., and Flores-Galvez, D. (2009). Associative learn-
184–197. ing, memory and serotonin: a neurobiological and pharmaco-
Hoyer, D., Clarke, D.E., Fozard, J.R., Hartig, P.R., Martin, G.R., logical analysis. In: Models of Neuropharmacology. L.L. Rocha
Mylecharane, E.J., Saxena, P.R., and Humphrey, P.P. (1994). Arrieta and V. Granados-Soto, eds. (Trivandrum, Kerala, India:
International Union of Pharmacology classification of recep- Transworld Research Network), pp. 169–182.
tors for 5-hydroxytryptamine (Serotonin). Pharmacol. Rev. 46, Meneses, A., Ponce-Lopez, T., Tellez, R., Gonzalez, R., Castillo, C.,
157–203. and Gasbarri, A. (2011a). Effects of d-amphetamine on short- and
Izquierdo, I., Medina, J.H., Izquierdo, L.A., Barros, D.M., de Souza, long-term memory in spontaneously hypertensive, Wistar-Kyoto
M.M., and Mello e Souza, T. (1998). Short- and long-term mem- and Sprague-Dawley rats. Behav. Brain Res. 216, 472–476.
ory are differentially regulated by monoaminergic systems in the Meneses, A., Perez-Garcia, G., Ponce-Lopez, T., Tellez, R., and
rat brain. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 69, 219–224. Castillo, C. (2011b). Serotonin transporter and memory.
Johnson, M.L., Ely, D.L., and Turner, M.E. (1995). Steroid sulfatase Neuropharmacology [Epub ahead of print]; DOI:10.1016/j.
and the Y chromosome hypertensive locus of the spontaneously neuropharm.2011.01.018. PMID: 21276807.
hypertensive rat. Steroids 60, 681–685. Meneses, A., Pérez-García, G., Ponce-Lopez, T., and Castillo, C.
Kadish, I., van Groen, T., and Wyss, J.M. (2001). Chronic, severe (2011c). 5-HT6 Receptor memory and amnesia: behavioral phar-
hypertension does not impair spatial learning and memory in macology – learning and memory processes. Int. Rev. Neurobiol.
Sprague-Dawley rats. Learn. Mem. 8, 104–111. 96, 27–47.
Kantak, K.M., Singh, T., Kerstetter, K.A., Dembro, K.A., Mutebi, Millan, M.J. (2008). The discovery and development of pharma-
M.M., Harvey, R.C., Deschepper, C.F., and Dwoskin, L.P. (2008). cotherapy for psychiatric disorders: a critical survey of animal
Advancing the spontaneous hypertensive rat model of attention and translational models and perspectives for their improve-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Behav. Neurosci. 122, 340–357. ment. In: Animal and Translational Models for CNS Drug
Kibby, M.Y. and Cohen, M.J. (2008). Memory functioning in chil- Discovery, R. McArthur and F. Borsini, eds. (Amsterdam:
dren with reading disabilities and/or attention deficit/hyperactiv- Elsevier), pp. 1–57.
ity disorder: a clinical investigation of their working memory Moncada, D. and Viola, H. (2007). Induction of long-term memory
and long-term memory functioning. Child Neuropsychol. 14, by exposure to novelty requires protein synthesis: evidence for a
525–546. behavioral tagging. J. Neurosci. 27, 7476–7481.
Lee, E.H. and Ma, Y.L. (1995). Amphetamine enhances memory Mook, D.M. and Neuringer, A. (1994). Different effects of amphet-
retention and facilitates norepinephrine release from the hip- amine on reinforced variations versus repetitions in spontane-
pocampus in rats. Brain Res. Bull. 37, 411–416. ously hypertensive rats (SHR). Physiol. Behav. 56, 939–944.
Liy-Salmeron, G. and Meneses, A. (2007). Role of 5-HT1–7 receptors Oades, R.D. (1987). Attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity
in short- and long-term memory for an autoshaping task: intra- (ADDH): the contribution of catecholaminergic activity. Prog.
hippocampal manipulations. Brain Res. 1147, 140–147. Neurobiol. 29, 365–391.
Liy-Salmeron, G. and Meneses, A. (2008). Effects of 5-HT drugs Oades, R.D., Sadile, A.G., Sagvolden, T., Viggiano, D., Zuddas, A.,
in prefrontal cortex during memory formation and the ketamine Devoto, P., Aase, H., Johansen, E.B., Ruocco, L.A., and Russell,
amnesia-model. Hippocampus 18, 965–974. V.A. (2005). The control of responsiveness in ADHD by cate-
Louis, W.J. and Howes, L.G. (1990). Genealogy of the spontane- cholamines: evidence for dopaminergic, noradrenergic and inter-
ously hypertensive rat and Wistar-Kyoto rat strains: implications active roles. Dev. Sci. 8, 122–131.
for studies of inherited hypertension. J. Cardiovasc. Pharmacol. Olvera-Cortés, M.E., Anguiano-Rodríguez, P., López-Vázquez,
16 (Suppl. 7), S1–S5. M.A., and Alfaro, J.M. (2008). Serotonin/dopamine interaction
Marsh, P.J. and Williams, L.M. (2006). ADHD and schizophrenia in learning. Prog. Brain Res. 172, 567–602.
phenomenology: visual scanpaths to emotional faces as a poten- Oscos, A., Camacho, J.L., Meneses, A., and Aleman, V. (1985).
tial psychophysiological marker? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30, The post-trial effect of amphetamine in memory and in cerebral
651–665. protein amino acid incorporation in the rat. In: Contemporary
Meneses, A. (1999). 5-HT system and cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Psychology: Biological Processes and Theoretical Issues, J.M.
Rev. 23, 1111–1125. McGaugh, ed. (Amsterdam: North-Holland), pp. 123–129.
Meneses, A. (2003). A pharmacological analysis of an associative Oscos, A., Martinez, J.L., Jr., and McGaugh, J.L. (1988). Effects
learning task: 5-HT1A to 5-HT7 receptor subtypes function on a of post-training d-amphetamine on acquisition of an appetitive
Pavlovian/instrumental autoshaped memory. Learn. Mem. 10, autoshaped lever press response in rats. Psychopharmacology
363–372. (Berl.) 95, 132–134.
Meneses, A. (2007a). Stimulation of 5-HT1A, 5-HT1B, 5-HT2A/2C, Paule, M.G., Rowland, A.S., Ferguson, S.A., Chelonis, J.J., Tannock,
5-HT3 and 5-HT4 receptors or 5-HT uptake inhibition: short- and R., Swanson, J.M., and Castellanos, F.X. (2000). Attention defi-
long-term memory. Behav. Brain Res. 184, 81–90. cit/hyperactivity disorder: characteristics, interventions and
Meneses, A. (2007b). Do serotonin1–7 receptors modulate short- and models. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 22, 631–651.
long-term memory? Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 87, 561–572. Podolski, I.Ya. (1998). Possibility of “superfast” consolidation of
Meneses, A. and Hong, E. (1998). Spontaneously hypertensive rats: long-term memory. Membr. Cell Biol. 11, 743–752.
a potential model to identify drugs for treatment of learning dis- Qiu, J., Hong, Q., Chen, R.H., Tong, M.L., Zhang, M., Fei, L., Pan,
orders. Hypertension 31, 968–972. X.Q., Guo, M., Guo, X.R., and Chi, X. (2010). Gene expression

AUTHOR’S COPY | AUTORENEXEMPLAR


AUTHOR’S COPY | AUTORENEXEMPLAR

ADHD, SHR, D -amphetamine and memory 371

profiles in the prefrontal cortex of SHR rats by cDNA microar- Solanto, M.V. (1998). Neuropsychopharmacological mechanisms of
rays. Mol. Biol. Rep. 37, 1733–1740. stimulant drug action in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a
Robbins, T.W. and Arnsten, A.F. (2009). The neuropsychopharma- review and integration. Behav. Brain Res. 94, 127–152.
cology of fronto-executive function: monoaminergic modula- Stevenson, R.D. and Wolraich, M.L. (1989). Stimulant medi-
tion. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 32, 267–287. cation therapy in the treatment of children with attention
Robertson, B.A., Clements, K.M., and Wainwright, P.E. (2008). The deficit hyperactivity disorder. Pediatr. Clin. North Am. 36,
working memory capabilities of the spontaneously hypertensive 1183–1197.
rat. Physiol. Behav. 94, 481–486. Tellez, R. and Meneses, A. (2009). Drug treatment and age differ-
Rodriguez, J.S., Boctor, S.Y., Phelix, C.F., and Martinez, J.L., Jr. entially affected memory formation and forgetting, Chicago, IL,
(2008). Differences in performance between Sprague-Dawley October 17–21, 2009. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 885.2.
and Fischer 344 rats in positive reinforcement tasks. Pharmacol. Tellez, R., Rocha, L., Castillo, C., and Meneses, A. (2010).
Biochem. Behav. 89, 17–22. Autoradiographic study of serotonin transporter during memory
Rosa, D.V., Souza, R.P., Souza, B.R., Guimarães, M.M., Carneiro, formation. Behav. Brain Res. 212, 12–26.
D.S., Valvassori, S.S., Gomez, M.V., Quevedo, J., and Romano- Terry, A.V., Jr., Hernandez, C.M., Buccafusco, J.J., and Gattu, M.
Silva, M.A. (2008). DARPP-32 expression in rat brain after an (2000). Deficits in spatial learning and nicotinic-acetylcholine
inhibitory avoidance task. Neurochem. Res. 33, 2257–2262. receptors in older, spontaneously hypertensive rats. Neuroscience
Russell, V.A. (2000). The nucleus accumbens motor-limbic inter- 101, 357–368.
face of the spontaneously hypertensive rat as studied in vitro by Tian, Y., Wang, Y., Deng, Y., and Maeda, K. (2009). Methylphenidate
the superfusion slice technique. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24, improves spatial memory of spontaneously hypertensive rats:
133–136. evidence in behavioral and ultrastructural changes. Neurosci.
Russell, V.A. (2003). Dopamine hypofunction possibly results from a Lett. 461, 106–109.
defect in glutamate-stimulated release of dopamine in the nucleus van den Bergh, F.S., Bloemarts, E., Chan, J.S., Groenink, L., Olivier,
accumbens shell of a rat model for attention deficit hyperactiv- B., and Oosting, R.S. (2006). Spontaneously hypertensive rats do
ity disorder – the spontaneously hypertensive rat. Neurosci. not predict symptoms of attention-deficit. Pharmacol. Biochem.
Biobehav. Rev. 27, 671–682. Behav. 83, 380–390.
Russell, V.A. (2007). Neurobiology of animal models of attention- Viggiano, D., Vallone, D., Welzl, H., and Sadile, A.G. (2002). The
deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Neurosci. Methods 161, 185– Naples high- and low-excitability rats: selective breeding, behav-
198. Corrected and republished in: J. Neurosci. Methods (2007) ioral profile, morphometry, and molecular biology of the meso-
166, I–XIV. cortical dopamine system. Behav. Genet. 32, 315–333.
Sagvolden, T. (2000). Behavioral validation of the spontaneously Viggiano, D., Vallone, D., Ruocco, L.A., and Sadile, A.G. (2003).
hypertensive rat (SHR) as an animal model of attention-deficit/ Behavioural, pharmacological, morpho-functional molecular
hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD). Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24, studies reveal a hyperfunctioning mesocortical dopamine system
31–39. in an animal model of attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder.
Sagvolden, T. and Xu, T. (2008). Amphetamine improves poor sus- Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 27, 683–689.
tained attention while d-amphetamine reduces overeactivity and Wells, A.M., Janes, A.C., Liu, X., Deschepper, C.F., Kaufman, M.J.,
impulsiveness as well as improves sustained attention in an ani- and Kantak, K.M. (2010). Medial temporal lobe functioning
mal model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). and structure in the spontaneously hypertensive rat: comparison
Behav. Brain Funct. 4, 3. with Wistar-Kyoto normotensive and Wistar-Kyoto hypertensive
Sagvolden, T., Russell, V.A., Aase, H., Johansen, E.B., and Farshbaf, strains. Hippocampus 20, 787–797.
M. (2005). Rodent models of attention-deficit/hyperactivity dis- Wilens, T.E., Klint, T., Adler, L., West, S., Wesnes, K., Graff, O., and
order. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1239–1247. Mikkelsen, B. (2008). A randomized controlled trial of a novel
Sagvolden, T., Dasbanerjee, T., Zhang-James, Y., Middleton, F., and mixed monoamine reuptake inhibitor in adults with ADHD.
Faraone, S. (2008). Behavioral and genetic evidence for a novel Behav. Brain Funct. 4, 24.
animal model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder predom- Wultz, B., Sagvolden, T., Moser, E.I., and Moser, M.B. (1990). The
inantly inattentive subtype. Behav. Brain Funct. 4, 56. spontaneously hypertensive rat as an animal model of attention-
Sagvolden, T., Johansen, E.B., Wøien, G., Walaas, S.I., Storm- deficit hyperactivity disorder: effects of methylphenidate on
Mathisen, J., Bergersen, L.H., Hvalby, O., Jensen, V., Aase, H., exploratory behavior. Behav. Neural Biol. 53, 88–102.
Russell, V.A., et al. (2009). The spontaneously hypertensive rat Zeeuws, I. and Soetens, E. (2007). Verbal memory performance
model of ADHD – the importance of selecting the appropriate improved via an acute administration of d-amphetamine. Hum.
reference strain. Neuropharmacology 57, 619–626. Psychopharmacol. 22, 279–287.

AUTHOR’S COPY | AUTORENEXEMPLAR

Вам также может понравиться