Вы находитесь на странице: 1из 13

Prevalence and Assessment of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in Primary Care Settings Ronald T. Brown, Wendy S. Freeman, James M.

Perrin, Martin T. Stein, Robert W. Amler, Heidi M. Feldman, Karen Pierce and Mark L. Wolraich Pediatrics 2001;107;43DOI: 10.1542/peds.107.3.e43

This information is current as of May 29, 2006

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is located on the World Wide Web at: http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/107/3/e43

PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned, published, and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright 2001 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.

Downloaded from www.pediatrics.org by on May 29, 2006

AMERICAN ACADEMY OF PEDIATRICS Prevalence and Assessment of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in Primary Care Settings
Ronald T. Brown, PhD*; Wendy S. Freeman, PhD*; James M. Perrin, MD; Martin T. Stein, MD; Robert W. Amler, MD; Heidi M. Feldman, MD, PhD ; Karen Pierce, MD#; and Mark L. Wolraich, MD**
ABSTRACT. Research literature relating to the prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and co-occurring conditions in children from primary care settings and the general population is reviewed as the basis of the American Academy of Pediatrics clinical practice guideline for the assessment and diagnosis of ADHD. Epidemiologic studies revealed prevalence rates generally ranging from 4% to 12% in the general population of 6 to 12 year olds. Similar or slightly lower rates of ADHD were revealed in pediatric primary care settings. Other behavioral, emotional, and learning problems significantly co-occurred with ADHD. Also reviewed were rating scales and medical tests that could be employed in evaluating ADHD. The utility of using both parent- and teacher-completed rating scales that specifically assess symptoms of ADHD in the diagnostic process was supported. Recommendations were made regarding the assessment of children with suspected ADHD in the pediatric primary care setting. Pediatrics 2001;107(3). URL: http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/ full/107/3/e43; prevalence, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, primary care.
ABBREVIATIONS. ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; TRI, Technical Resources International; DSM, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual; DISC, Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children; EEG, electroencephalogram; ERP, event-related potential.

ttention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) has defining features of inattention, overactivity, and impulsivity.1 It is the most frequently encountered childhood-onset neurodevelopmental disorder in primary care settings. Symptoms frequently co-occur with other emotional, behavioral, and learning problems, including oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder, depression, anxiety, and learning disabilities. The cause of ADHD is unknown, and multiple pathways may lead to the phenotypic expression of the disorder.2 Public awareness of ADHD has increased, and the disorder represents a public health concern with sigFrom the *Department of Pediatrics, Medical University of South Carolina, Charleston, South Carolina; Massachusetts General Hospital for Children, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts; University of California at San Diego, San Diego, California; US Department of Health and Human Services, Atlanta, Georgia; University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania; #Childrens Memorial Hospital, Chicago, Illinois; and **Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee. Received for publication Dec 18, 2000; accepted Dec 18, 2000. PEDIATRICS (ISSN 0031 4005). Copyright 2001 by the American Academy of Pediatrics.

nificant effects on childrens functioning across multiple areas.2 Referrals to health care professionals for children suspected of having the disorder continue at a high rate, and changes in the health care system in the United States have placed increasing demands on primary care pediatricians to diagnose and manage the disorder. It is now recognized that ADHD is a chronic condition that will persist over the life span.2 The American Academy of Pediatrics Committee on Quality Improvement Subcommittee on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder synthesized a clinical practice guideline for the diagnosis and evaluation of children with ADHD.3 This report will highlight the empirical literature review on which this practice guideline is based. The subcommittee worked with Technical Resources International (TRI), Washington, DC, under the auspices of the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, to develop an evidence base addressing questions regarding the prevalence, co-occurring conditions, and diagnostic tests for ADHD. For a full account of the literature review, see the technical review compiled by Green, Wong, Atkins, Taylor, and Feinleib.4 Given the widespread attention ADHD has received, it is important to examine the epidemiology of this disorder and methods to assess it. Because of the paucity of data regarding preschoolers and adolescents with ADHD, the literature review focused on studies involving elementary-school-aged children. Specifically, 4 key questions provided the framework for the development of the technical review.4 They are as follows: 1. What is the prevalence of ADHD and co-occurring behavioral, emotional, and learning disorders in the general population of 6 to 12 year olds in the United States? 2. What is the prevalence of ADHD and co-occurring conditions in 6 to 12 year olds coming to primary care providers in the United States? 3. How accurate and reliable are behavior rating instruments in screening for ADHD? 4. How useful are medical screening tests in diagnosing ADHD? For the review, 507 articles and 10 published rating scale manuals were compiled from empirical articles; traditional databases (Medline, PsychINFO); reference lists in review papers; references from the Practice Parameters for the Assessment and Treatment of
1 of 11

http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/107/3/e43 PEDIATRICS Vol. 107 No. 3 March 2001 Downloaded from www.pediatrics.org by on May 29, 2006

Children, Adolescents, and Adults with AttentionDeficit/Hyperactivity Disorder5; recently published journal articles; citations suggested by members of the American Academy of Pediatrics; and a database of bibliographies on studies involving the Child Behavior Checklist rating scale.6 A physician and psychologist specializing in ADHD independently rated each article and manual for sound empirical evidence addressing the 4 questions. Criteria used to evaluate the appropriateness of a study are presented in Table 1. Application of the inclusion and exclusion criteria yielded 87 articles and 10 manuals for inclusion in the review.4
PREVALENCE OF ADHD

Most studies of ADHD have come from referral populations seen in tertiary care centers. They, therefore, reflect unknown sampling biases and cannot provide estimates of rates of ADHD in unreferred populations. Thus, to address the epidemiology of ADHD, we reviewed the prevalence of ADHD in communities, schools, and primary care settings. Also investigated was the prevalence of co-occurring conditions in the general population and primary care settings.
Prevalence of ADHD and Co-occurring Conditions in the Community Samples

Studies in which diagnostic instruments were administered to 6- to 12-year-olds from representative


TABLE 1. Inclusion Criteria for Studies Factor Definition of ADHD and comorbidities

community samples or school settings were considered.716 All used Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM) criteria for ADHD, although different editions were used. Three studies used DSM-III,17 6 used DSM-III-R,18 1 used DSM-III-R and DSM-IV,1 and 1 used DSM-IV. See Table 2 for the criteria employed in the various editions. For studies using DSM-III and DSM-III-R criteria to diagnose ADHD, prevalence rates ranged from 4%12 to 26%.11 However, 9 of the 10 investigations revealed prevalence rates between 4% and 12% (median: 5.8%). The investigation reporting the outlier prevalence rate of 26% examined the smallest sample of children, and all children in that investigation were from a single, inner-city elementary school.11 These methodological limitations may have contributed to a spuriously inflated estimate for the prevalence of ADHD in the general population. Across studies, setting (ie, community vs school), gender, and diagnostic nomenclature (DSM-III vs DSM-III-R criteria), all affected the prevalence rates. Specifically, the mean prevalence rates of ADHD were higher in community samples (10.3% for community samples vs 6.9% for school samples), higher among males (9.2% for males vs 3.0% for females), and higher among children who were diagnosed according to DSM-III-R criteria (10.3% for DSM-III-R vs 6.8% for DSM-III criteria). At the time that the American Academy of Pediatrics practice guideline was synthesized and the technical report was compiled, sufficient research

Criteria ADHD with criteria from DSM-III, DSM-III-R, and DSM-IV Co-occurring conditions: learning disabilities, depression, anxiety, conduct disorder, and oppositional defiant disorder Combinations of the 5 co-occurring conditions allowed in Questions 1 and 2 (prevalence questions) None of the 5 co-occurring conditions for Questions 3 and 4 (assessment questions) Boys and girls 6 to 12 years old Representative population Non-referred populations (for prevalence estimates) Absence of moderate to severe mental retardation, pervasive developmental disorders, and severe psychiatric disorders North American studies for Questions 1 and 2 (prevalence questions) Any countries for Questions 3 and 4 (assessment questions) All settings allowed in Questions 3 and 4 (assessment questions) Limited settings for Questions 1 and 2 (prevalence questions): general population (Question 1)community and school surveys; primary care setting (Question 2) pediatricians and family or general practice physicians Selected behavior checklists and rating scales (parent and teacher reports) Accuracy for ADHD: sensitivity, specificity, positive predictive value Accuracy for referral population (for broad-band checklists) Effect size for discriminating referred from non-referred samples Selected medical and neurologic screening tests: electroencephalography, lead concentration level, thyroid hormone level, imaging tests, continuous performance tests, hearing and vision screening Prevalence of abnormal findings Data from peer-reviewed published studies Literature published 1980 to 1997 English language Diagnostic (not treatment) outcomes

Patient population

Setting and provider population

Behavior screening tests for ADHD Outcomes of interest for behavior screening tests (Question 3) Medical screening tests for ADHD (Question 4) Outcomes of interest for medical screening tests (Question 4) Criteria for admissible evidence and bibliographic database boundaries

American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 3rd ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1980. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 3rd ed, rev. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1987. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994.

2 of 11

TECHNICAL REPORT: ADHD IN PRIMARY CARE SETTINGS Downloaded from www.pediatrics.org by on May 29, 2006

had not yet accumulated on the prevalence of ADHD using DSM-IV criteria. One study that was available, however, was an investigation by Wolraich et al19 conducted with 4323 school children ranging from kindergarten to fifth grade. In addition to assessing DSM-IV symptoms, Wolraich and colleagues examined the level of impairment in childrens academic and behavior functioning. Consistent with the data provided by others,8,15 the investigators found an overall prevalence rate of 6.8% when each of the ADHD subtypes (primarily inattentive type, combined type, and primarily hyperactive/impulsive type) was considered. Of interest is the finding that, when DSM-IV impairment criteria were not considered, 16% of the sample qualified for a diagnosis, with many more males than females meeting criteria. This finding underscores the importance of incorporating functional impairment when making a diagnosis of ADHD. Without due consideration of the DSM-IV functional impairment criterion, the frequency of ADHD diagnosis may be spuriously high. Wolraich and colleagues19 offered support for the DSM-IV diagnostic subtypes of ADHD, with most of the children meeting criteria for either the predominantly inattentive subtype (3.2%) or the combined subtype (2.9%). More studies need to be conducted to determine the distribution of children with ADHD across the subtypes. Three studies20 22 examined prevalence rates of ADHD among elementary school children in the general population who had been screened for symptoms of behavior problems. Formal diagnostic instruments were administered only to children who were identified in the initial screening procedure. The average prevalence rate approached 4%. A high proportion of children with ADHD have other related conditions, and several investigators have assessed co-occurring conditions. In the technical review, Green et al4 presented studies assessing co-occurring ADHD and oppositional defiant disorder,12,14,16 conduct disorder,7,12,14,16 and depressive and anxiety disorders.14,21 Calculations of the mean prevalence rates across studies were highest for oppositional defiant disorder (35.2%), followed by conduct disorder and anxiety disorders (25.7% and 25.8%), and depressive disorders (18.2%). In the 1 investigation that employed DSM-IV criteria, Wolraich and colleagues19 found that rates of co-occurring oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder, anxiety disorder, and depressive disorder were similar to those found in the studies previously mentioned. Of interest, Wolraich et al found markedly different patterns of co-occurring disorders according to diagnostic subtype. When they examined the inattentive subtype, significantly fewer children had co-occurring disruptive behavior disorders (ie, oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder) compared with the hyperactive/impulsive and combined subtypes. In addition, children with the predominantly hyperactive/impulsive subtype evidenced fewer co-occurring internalizing (ie, anxiety, depressive) and learning problems. Thus, externalizing disorders (ie, oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder) seem to be associated with the hyper-

active/impulsive dimension of ADHD; internalizing disorders (ie, anxiety, depression, learning disabilities) seem to be more frequently associated with the inattention dimension of the disorder. The rate of co-occurrence between ADHD and learning disabilities is difficult to establish because few studies have employed DSM criteria for learning disorders. As a result, many studies were not included in this review. August and Garfinkel7 examined the prevalence of specific reading disability among children with ADHD. For children to be classified as having a specific reading disability, children had to be reading disabled relative to their peers of the same chronological age and relative to their general level of intellectual functioning. Twenty-two percent of their sample of children with ADHD met these criteria. In the study by Wolraich and colleagues,19 only 11% of the children classified as having ADHD according to DSM-IV criteria were reported to have learning disabilities. The lower rate of learning disabilities in that study may reflect the use of a restrictive methodology to classify children as learning disabled,4 with the classification simply based on teachers indication of whether the child had a learning disability diagnosis. Clearly, additional research regarding the co-occurrence of ADHD and learning disabilities is needed.
Prevalence of ADHD and Co-occurring Conditions in the Pediatric Office Setting

Significant changes in the health care system have placed increasing demands on pediatric primary care providers to assess and manage children who present with ADHD symptoms. Some research has suggested that childrens symptom display may differ as a function of psychiatric versus pediatric clinic setting.23 Numerous studies have examined the symptoms of children with ADHD evaluated in psychiatric settings, but not enough attention has been devoted to children evaluated in primary care offices. Two groups of investigators reported on the prevalence of ADHD in the primary care setting,24 26 and their findings were presented in the technical review. Lindgren et al26 examined 457 consecutive 6- to 12year-old patients from primary care settings. Prevalence of ADHD under different inclusion criteria (ie, 8 vs 10 symptoms) was estimated. Symptoms were assessed from parents reports based on DSM-III-R criteria. As might be expected, prevalence rates differed according to cutoff criteria. A prevalence of 11.2% occurred when the cutoff was 8 symptoms. A lower prevalence rate (3.7%) was revealed with the more conservative 10-symptom cutoff. In contrast to concerns expressed in the popular media that this disorder is overdiagnosed by pediatricians,27 the data here suggest that prevalence rates of ADHD in primary care settings are similar to rates in general population studies. In an epidemiologic study conducted in Pittsburgh by Costello and colleagues,24 25 300 children were assessed from a pool of 789 patients from 2 health maintenance organization clinics. The Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children (DISC),28 a structured
3 of 11

http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/107/3/e43 Downloaded from www.pediatrics.org by on May 29, 2006

TABLE 2.

Diagnostic Criteria for ADHD Across Versions of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual

* American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 3rd ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, 1980 American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 3rd ed, rev. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, 1987 American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, 1994

4 of 11

TECHNICAL REPORT: ADHD IN PRIMARY CARE SETTINGS Downloaded from www.pediatrics.org by on May 29, 2006

psychiatric interview assessing DSM-III criteria, was administered by a psychiatric social worker. When considering parent reports on the DISC, findings were that 1.4% of the children met DSM-III criteria for a diagnosis of attention-deficit disorder with hyperactivity, and 0.2% met criteria for attention-deficit disorder without hyperactivity. Fewer cases were identified using childrens reports on the DISC, compared with parent reports, indicating that reliance only on childrens reports may show the prevalence rate to be falsely low. This finding underscores the importance of collecting parent interview data. The prevalence rates reported by Costello and colleagues24 25 were much lower than those reported by Lindgen et al,26 probably because older editions of the DSM yielded lower rates of the disorder. Lindgren et al26 and Costello et al25 showed prevalence rates of co-occurring conditions with ADHD ranging from 9% to 38% across disorders. The most prevalent co-occurring conditions were oppositional defiant disorder and anxiety disorder, each with a prevalence rate of 38%. The prevalence rates of depression and conduct disorder (each at 9%) were lower in the primary care setting than those found in the studies of ADHD in the general population. Children considered to have more severe psychiatric disturbances, like depression and conduct disorder, may be triaged to child psychiatric clinics as opposed to pediatric primary care settings. Nonetheless, coexisting conditions are clearly evident among children seen in the primary care setting and merit the careful attention of the primary care provider.
Summary

associations with the hyperactive/impulsive dimension of ADHD, whereas internalizing and learning problems are more strongly associated with the inattentive dimension of the disorder. Thus, as recommended in the clinical practice guideline,3 the evaluation of ADHD should include evaluation for other conditions that may co-occur with the disorder.
ASSESSMENT OF ADHD

Assessment of ADHD in the primary care settings may include both behavior rating scales and medical and laboratory tests. The utility of these instruments in the identification of ADHD was examined.
Behavior Rating Scales

Examination of the data revealed that prevalence rates of ADHD generally range from 4% to 12% of the elementary school population when the DSM-III, DSM-III-R, or DSM-IV is used,1,1718 with higher rates of the disorder among males and with higher rates reported from school settings than community settings. Only 2 studies provided information on the prevalence of ADHD in primary care settings, with 1 reporting 11% of children met criteria for ADHD, and the second reporting that less than 2% of children met criteria for the disorder. Additional research examining the prevalence of this disorder in pediatric settings is needed. Overall, the findings regarding the prevalence of ADHD support the American Academy of Pediatrics clinical practice guideline3 assertion that it is reasonable for primary care pediatricians to initiate an evaluation for ADHD when children present with symptoms that include inattention, hyperactivity, impulsivity, academic underachievement, and behavior problems. ADHD frequently co-occurs with additional emotional, behavioral, and learning problems in community and primary care settings, with disruptive behavior disorders being most common, followed by internalizing and learning problems. Finally, the investigation that examined conditions co-occurring with the DSM-IV subtypes supported the current nosology that conceptualizes ADHD as a 2-dimensional disorder.19 Interestingly, co-occurring disruptive behavior problems seem to have more frequent

A widely employed tool for the assessment of ADHD has been the use of behavior rating scales and checklists. These scales can be completed by parents, teachers, and other informants. The informant is asked to summarize the extent to which the child exhibits particular behaviors over a specified time. The advantages of behavior rating scales include ease of administration, cost effectiveness, and the range of information provided by multiple informants. These instruments can be classified into either broad-band checklists or ADHD-specific measures. ADHD-specific measures are those that specifically assess the core symptoms of the disorder, whereas broad-band checklists measure a variety of child behavior problems. Regarding broad-band rating scales, information obtained may include scores that sum across all types of child behavior problems (ie, total global scale scores), scores that sum across types of internalizing problems such as depression and anxiety (ie, internalizing scale scores), and scores that sum across types of externalizing problems such as aggression and conduct problems (ie, externalizing scale scores). Some measures provide scores for subscales that assess adaptive behavior. To be included in the technical review,4 it was required that the ADHD-specific and broad-band rating scales included a parent version of the scale and had normative data available. Also, broad-band checklists had to include subscales designed to measure symptoms associated with ADHD. The rating scales reviewed are listed in Table 3. Effect sizes were calculated for each of these rating subscales. An effect size refers to the difference in mean scores between 2 populations (eg, children referred with ADHD vs nonreferred children) divided by an estimate of the individual standard deviation. A larger effect size is more desirable because it suggests less overlap between the 2 populations. Hence, an effect size of 1.0 or less would reflect substantial overlap between the distribution of scores across the 2 populations. In contrast, an effect size of 3.0 suggests little overlap between scores attained for the 2 populations, with the mean scores for the 2 populations falling 3 standard deviation units apart. Consequently, one could consider the 2 populations relatively distinct. A higher effect size is indicative of greater sensitivity and specificity of the measure. For example, assuming that the populations with and without the
5 of 11

http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/107/3/e43 Downloaded from www.pediatrics.org by on May 29, 2006

TABLE 3.

Broad-Band Checklists: Ability to Detect Referred Versus Nonreferred Participants* Study Behavior Rating Scale Age Gender Effect Size 1.4 1.3 1.2 1.1 1.0 2.3 2.0 1.2 1.0 1.0 0.9 1.4 2.9 2.0 1.1 1.1 0.7 0.7 1.6 1.2 1.1 1.2 1.2

Total Scales CBCL/4-18-R CBCL/4-18-R Achenbach, 1991 CBCL/TRF-R CBCL/TRF-R Naglieri, LeBuffe, and Pfeiffer, 1994 DSMD Conners, 1997 CPRS-R:L CTRS-R:L Externalizing Scales Achenbach, 1991 CBCL/4-18-R CBCL/4-18-R Achenbach, 1991 CBCL/TRF-R CBCL/TRF-R Naglieri, LeBuffe, and Pfeiffer, 1994 DSMD Conners, 1997 CPRS-R:L-DSM-IV CTRS-R:L-DSM-IV Internalizing Scales Achenbach, 1991 CBCL/4-18-R CBCL/4-18-R Achenbach, 1991 CBCL/TRF-R CBCL/TRF-R Naglieri, Lebuffe, and Pfeiffer, 1994 DSMD Adaptive Functioning Scales Achenbach, 1991 CBCL/4-18-R CBCL/4-18-R Achenbach, 1991 CBCL/TRF-R CBCL/TRF-R Achenbach, 1991

411 411 511 511 512 411 411 511 511 512 411 411 511 511 512 411 411 511 411

M F M F MF MF MF M F M F MF MF MF M F M F MF M F M F

Adapted from Green M, Wong M, Atkins D, Taylor J, Feinleib M. Diagnosis of Attention-Deficit/ Hyperactivity Disorder (Technical Review #3). Rockville, MD: Agency for Health Care Policy and Research; 1999. * CBCL/4-18-R Child Behavior Checklist for Ages 4 18, Parent Form, Revised; TRF-R Teacher Report Form, Revised; DSMD Devereaux Scales of Mental Disorders; CPRS-R:L 1997 Revision of the Conners Parent Rating Scale, Long Version; CTRS-R:L 1997 Revision of the Conners Teaching Rating Scale, Long Version. Achenbach TM. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry; 1991. Achenbach TM. Manual for the Teachers Report Form and 1991 Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry; 1991. Naglieri JA, Lebuffe PA, Pfeiffer SI. Devereaux Scales of Mental Disorders. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Brace; 1994.

disorder are normally distributed and have equal variances, and considering the case whereby sensitivity equals specificity, an effect size of 3.0 would be associated with a sensitivity and specificity of 0.94 and a false-positive and false-negative rate of 6%.29 In contrast, an effect size of 1.0, under the same conditions, would be associated with a sensitivity and specificity of 0.71, which is associated with a false-positive and false-negative rate of 29%.29 Broad-band rating scales and checklists were evaluated for their ability to discriminate children referred for ADHD from their nonreferred peers. Table 3 presents the broad-band rating scale studies that allowed the calculation of effect sizes for total problem, internalizing, externalizing, and adaptive functioning indices or subscales. The average effect size across broad-band measures using total global scale scores was 1.5. Effect sizes using domain scales (ie, the internalizing, externalizing, and adaptive functioning scales) generally ranged from 0.7 to 1.4, with the exception of the externalizing scales on the parent and teacher forms of the Conners Scales,30 which had significantly better discriminatory power. Taken together, the findings do not provide sufficient evi6 of 11

dence to support a reliance on either broad-band checklist total problem indices or scales assessing externalizing, internalizing, or adaptive behavior to screen for or diagnose ADHD. However, these scales may be used for other purposes such as screening for co-occurring problems in other areas (eg, anxiety, depression, conduct problems). Also available to practitioners are several rating scales that specifically assess symptoms related to ADHD. The studies that were reviewed used ADHD-specific measures to discriminate between children diagnosed with ADHD and typically developing children. Because few studies used control groups with other psychiatric disorders (eg, learning disabilities, conduct disorder), it is difficult to evaluate the efficacy of these rating scales in differentiating children with ADHD from those with other psychiatric diagnoses. Table 4 presents studies of ADHD-specific rating scales. These studies allowed the calculation of effect sizes for global ADHD symptoms and subscales that assess specific types of ADHD symptoms (ie, inattention, impulsivity, overactivity). The overall range of effect sizes varied across measures. Effect sizes for global ADHD symp-

TECHNICAL REPORT: ADHD IN PRIMARY CARE SETTINGS Downloaded from www.pediatrics.org by on May 29, 2006

TABLE 4.

ADHD-Specific Checklists: Ability to Detect ADHD Versus Normal Controls* Study Behavior Rating Scales Total ADHD Symptoms Scales CPRS-R:L-ADHD Index CTRS-R:L-ADHD Index CPRS-R:L-DSM-IV Symptoms CTRS-R:L-DSM-IV Symptoms SSQ-O-I SSQ-O-II Hyperactivity Subscales ACTeRS DSM-III SNAP Checklist DSM-III-R SNAP Checklist DSM-III-R SNAP Checklist CATQ-HI Inattention Subscales ACTeRSParent Version DSM-III SNAP Checklist DSM-III-R SNAP Checklist DSM-III-R SNAP Checklist Impulsivity Subscales DSM-III SNAP Checklist DSM-III-R SNAP Checklist DSM-III-R SNAP Checklist Age 617 617 617 617 611 611 614 712 711 711 7 614 712 711 711 712 711 711 Gender MF MF MF MF F F MF MF M F M MF MF M F MF M F Effect Size 3.1 3.3 3.4 3.7 1.3 2.0 1.5 5.1 3.1 3.7 4.1 2.0 4.2 3.5 4.0 5.5 4.7 4.0

Conners, 1997

Breen, 1989

Ullmann, Sleator, & Sprague, 1997 Atkins, Pelham, & Licht, 1985 Horn, Wagner, & Ialongo, 1989 Horn, Wagner, & Ialongo, 1989 Tarnowski, Prinz, & Nay, 1986 Ullmann, Sleator, & Sprague, 1997 Atkins, Pelham, & Licht, 1985 Horn, Wagner, & Ialongo, 1989 Horn, Wagner, & Ialongo, 1989 Atkins, Pelham, & Licht, 1985 Horn, Wagner, & Ialongo, 1989 Horn, Wagner, & Ialongo, 1989

Adapted from Green et al. Diagnosis of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (Technical Review 3). Rockville, MD: Agency for Health Care Policy and Research; 1999. * CPRS-R:L 1997 Revision of the Conners Parent Rating Scale, Long Version; CTRS-R:L 1997 Revision of the Conners Teaching Rating Scale, Long Version; SSQ-O-I Barkleys School Situations QuestionnaireOriginal Version, Number of Problem Settings Scale; SSQ-O-II Barkleys School Situations QuestionnaireOriginal Version, Mean Severity Scale; ACTeRS ADD-H: Comprehensive Teacher Rating Scale; CATQ-HI Conners Abbreviated Teacher QuestionnaireHyperactivity Index.

toms ranged from 1.3 to 3.7. The Conners scales30 yielded the highest effect sizes, and the School Situations Questionnaire31 the lowest effect sizes. When specific symptoms were examined separately, the effect sizes were again variable. Overall, the effect sizes were slightly greater for subscales assessing specific symptoms compared with indices of combined ADHD symptoms. Across symptoms, effect sizes ranged from 3.1 to 5.5 when considering the Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham (SNAP) Checklist32,33 and the Conners Abbreviated Teacher Questionnaire Hyperactivity Index,34 with significantly lower effect sizes by the inattention and hyperactivity subscales of the Attention-Deficit Disorder Comprehensive Teacher Rating ScaleParent version.35
Summary

means to gather data regarding the display of the core symptoms of ADHD. In addition, the collection of behavior ratings from teachers and caregivers will fulfill the DSM requirement that there be cross-situational evidence of the disorder. Although rating scales are convenient for use in the pediatric office setting, we caution against their use in isolation. Information collected via rating scales must be supplemented with a clinical history, including age of onset and duration of symptoms, and careful interview, which includes an assessment of the functional consequences of the behaviors.
Medical and Laboratory Screening Tests

Overall, rating scales of specific ADHD symptoms were more useful in diagnosis than global indices on broad-band checklists. Among the ADHD-specific rating scales that were reviewed, the ADHD Index and the DSM-IV Symptoms Scale of the 1997 revision of the Conners Rating Scale30 and the Hyperactivity and Inattention Subscales of the SNAP Checklist32 performed well in discriminating between children with ADHD and normal controls. It should be noted, however, that while parent- or teacher-completed broad-band scales are not recommended to specifically diagnose ADHD, global rating scales may be useful to screen for co-occurring problems. Given the recommendations set forth in the practice guideline3 that the assessment of ADHD requires evidence of symptomatology from caregivers and school personnel (ie, teachers), we endorse the use of behavior rating scales as a time-efficient and cost-effective

Several medical screening tests and laboratory measures have been used to evaluate children with suspected ADHD. These tests include blood lead levels, thyroid function, radiographic assessment, electroencephalography, neurologic screening examinations, and continuous performance tasks, as well as other miscellaneous laboratory assessments. The association between elevated lead levels and delays in cognitive functioning, including attention problems, has been consistently reported.36 37 This begs the question regarding the utility of lead level measurements in the assessment of ADHD. Six studies were reviewed, with no statistically significant associations in 3 of the investigations.38 40 One study reported a positive association between lead level and behavior problems.15 Two studies examined children screened for disruptive behavior problems and found associations between elevated lead levels and behavior problems.41 42 However, because these studies did not assess ADHD, the extent to which their findings may be applied to children with this
7 of 11

http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/107/3/e43 Downloaded from www.pediatrics.org by on May 29, 2006

disorder is unknown. These findings suggest an association between elevated lead levels and a range of behavior problems including inattention. However, the routine use of lead screening as a diagnostic indicator for ADHD is not supported. A key issue here is that by elementary school age, children who have had lead effects will almost always have normal lead levels. Abnormal thyroid function can produce a variety of behavior effects in children, ranging from impairments in concentration to severe neuropsychological deficits.43 Four studies of thyroid function were reviewed, and none of the 4 showed an association between abnormal thyroid levels and ADHD. In 3 of the studies, none of the children with ADHD had clinically significant thyroid dysfunction.44 46 In a study by Weiss, Stein, Trommer, and Refetoff47 2% of the ADHD cohort had abnormal thyroid levels compared with 1% of the comparison control group. In total, these studies fail to support the use of thyroid function tests to screen for ADHD. Recent attention also has been devoted to investigating whether there are morphologic differences between the brain structures of individuals with ADHD relative to their normally developing peers. The technical review4 examined 9 studies that used either computerized tomography or magnetic resonance imaging to compare children with ADHD with a comparison control group.48 56 No differences were found between children with ADHD and comparison controls in 2 of the investigations.50,56 In the other 7 studies, asymmetries, differences in shape or volume of the ventricles, and differences in brain size occurred between ADHD and normally developing children. In each of these studies, structures in the children with ADHD were smaller than those of comparison controls. These studies are provocative and will likely direct new research that has the potential to shed light on the pathogenesis of this disorder. However, because other child psychiatric control groups (eg, children with learning problems or other disruptive behavior disorders) have not been included, the specificity of these findings to ADHD is not clear. Furthermore, although some studies have revealed significant group differences, the degree of within group variance and overlap between groups make imaging of little use for individual diagnostic purposes. That is to say, the imaging findings do not discriminate adequately between children with ADHD and those without. For these reasons, the use of imaging procedures is not currently supported as a diagnostic tool for assessment of ADHD. One of the most widely researched medical tests for evaluating children with ADHD is the electroencephalogram (EEG) to examine event-related potentials (ERPs). Eight studies met criteria for inclusion in Green and colleagues4 technical review.10,57 63 Overall, no major EEG abnormalities (ie, evidence of seizure activity) were found for children with ADHD. Several investigations reported minor differences in ERPs functioning, including longer latencies at the P3 site,57 longer latencies of certain waves for brainstem auditory-evoked potentials,58 more slow waves and fewer -waves,59 and asymmetry in peak
8 of 11

amplitude evoked-response potentials.10 These findings are variable and do not provide any compelling evidence for a particular EEG pattern for patients with ADHD. Over the years, various neuropsychological screens, or soft sign assessments were believed to shed light on the pathogenesis of the ADHD disorder. Five studies using such assessments met criteria for inclusion in the technical review.4 Reeves and colleagues64 found that children with ADHD evidenced higher rates of neurodevelopment abnormalities than comparison control children on 9 tests of sensorimotor coordination, but no differences between groups were found for prenatal or perinatal problems or speech problems. Trommer and colleagues65 found that children with ADHD evidenced a greater number of errors on a psychomotor task designed to assess inattention. However, the ranges for the number of errors exhibited by the ADHD and control group were similar, casting doubt on the clinical significance of this group difference. The remaining 3 studies66 68 revealed no differences between ADHD and comparison control children on various neurodevelopment tasks (eg, the Revised Neurologic Examination for Subtle Signs, Mazes subtest of the Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children). These findings do not support the use of neurodevelopment measures for diagnosis of ADHD. Some isolated studies measuring neurotransmitters (eg, serotonin levels, dopamine receptors, epinephrine), hormones (eg, growth hormone releasing factor), and proteins69 74 were also reviewed in the TRI report.4 Each study reported findings suggestive of possible biological differences between children with ADHD and controls, but the findings were too sparse and preliminary to indicate a definitive relationship. Finally, studies examining computerized and pencil and paper tests of sustained attention and impulsivity (eg, continuous performance tests)7,33,75 84 were reviewed in the TRI report.4 These measures poorly discriminated children with ADHD from their normally developing peers. Both indices of inattention and indices of impulsivity provided by continuous performance tasks were poor predictors of ADHD, with most effect sizes lower than 1.0.4 Thus, the power of discrimination of these tests is not sufficient to support their use in the assessment and diagnosis of ADHD.
Summary

Many medical tests and laboratory assessments have been investigated in relation to ADHD. Across studies that included blood lead levels, morphologic features, and thyroid abnormalities, no compelling evidence supported an association between abnormalities on these various tests and the presence of ADHD. Morphologic studies offered some preliminary support for brain-related differences between children with and without the disorder, but additional studies with control groups of children with other psychiatric and developmental disorders and larger sample sizes will be necessary before brain morphology becomes useful in diagnosing ADHD.

TECHNICAL REPORT: ADHD IN PRIMARY CARE SETTINGS Downloaded from www.pediatrics.org by on May 29, 2006

Based on these data, it is recommended in the practice guideline3 that laboratory and medical diagnostic tests discussed above not be used routinely in determining a diagnosis of ADHD.
CONCLUSIONS

Children with ADHD are frequently encountered in the primary care setting. It is important that the diagnosis of this condition by primary care providers be based on procedures supported by evidence from empirical investigations. Here findings were highlighted from a comprehensive review of the literature regarding the prevalence of ADHD and co-occurring conditions in community and primary care settings, as well as evidence regarding the utility of behavior rating scales and medical tests in the assessment process. In community samples of school-aged children, the prevalence rates of the disorder generally ranged from 4% to 12%, with similar or lower rates in pediatric samples. In the literature examined for the purpose of the technical review,4 the most frequently co-occurring disorder was oppositional defiant disorder (33%), followed by conduct disorder and anxiety disorder (each at 25%). Approximately 20% had co-occurring depressive disorders, and 12% to 22% had learning disabilities. Anxiety, depression, and learning disabilities were recently found to cooccur more frequently in children with the inattentive subtype of ADHD, and disruptive behavior disorders co-occurred more frequently in children presenting with hyperactive/impulsive symptoms. With regard to assessment, behavior rating scales and medical screening tests have been investigated. ADHD-specific scales are reliable and valid for the assessment of the disorder, but global or broad-domain scales are not. There is no compelling evidence to support the use of medical and laboratory tests in the identification of ADHD. The findings of this review have significant implications for practice and research. Given the prevalence rates of ADHD, primary care pediatricians should be prepared to identify children with the disorder. Clinicians should use ADHD-specific rating scales completed by caregivers and teachers in their efforts to identify children suspected for ADHD. Ratings from multiple informants should be employed to ascertain the DSM-IV criterion of crosssituational symptom display. Broad-band scales may be useful in the identification of problems or symptoms that may co-occur with ADHD, but their use in diagnosing ADHD is not supported. Also, the use of medical and laboratory tests to diagnose ADHD is not indicated. In addition to behavior rating scales, a thorough history of symptoms and the effect of these symptoms on the childs current functioning should be evaluated. Examination of the literature summarized in the technical review4 reveals important areas for additional investigation. For example, most of the literature used DSM-III and DSM-III-R criteria to identify children with ADHD. Fewer studies using DSM-IV criteria have addressed the prevalence and assessment of ADHD. These questions need to be exam-

ined with DSM-IV criteria and parameters associated with the specific ADHD subtypes. Most research conducted to date has predominantly reported on samples of boys with ADHD. Investigations targeting girls with this disorder are sorely needed. As well, research has primarily focused on elementaryschool-aged children; there is a need for investigations examining this disorder among preschoolers and adolescents. Finally, the studies reviewed used control groups composed of either nonreferred children or typically developing comparison controls. Greater efforts are needed to evaluate the specificity of ADHD-specific rating scales in discriminating between children with ADHD and those with other psychiatric disorders where ADHD does not co-occur.
REFERENCES
1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994 2. National Institutes of Health. Diagnosis and Treatment of AttentionDeficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Washington, DC: US Government Printing Office; 1998. NIH Consensus Statement 16(2) 3. American Academy of Pediatrics. Clinical practice guideline: diagnosis and evaluation of the child with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2000;105:1158 1170 4. Green M, Wong M, Atkins D, Taylor J, Feinlieb M. Diagnosis of AttentionDeficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. (Technical Review #3). Rockville, MD: Agency for Health Care Policy and Research; 1999 5. American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Practice parameters for the assessment and treatment of children, adolescents, adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36:85121 6. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock CS. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist and Child Behavior Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry; 1991 7. August GJ, Garfinkel BD. Behavioral and cognitive subtypes of ADHD. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1989;28:739 748 8. Cohen P, Cohen J, Kasen S, et al. An epidemiological study of disorders in late childhood and adolescence: I. Age and gender-specific prevalence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1993;34:851 867 9. King C, Young RD. Attentional deficits with and without hyperactivity: Teacher and peer perceptions. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1982;10:483 495 10. Kuperman S, Johnson B, Arndt S, Lindgren L, Wolraich M. Quantitative EEG differences in a nonclinical sample with ADHD and undifferentiated ADD. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1996;35:1009 1017 11. Newcorn JH, Halperin JM, Schwartz S, et al. Parent and teacher ratings of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms: implications for case identifications. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1994;15:86 91 12. Pelham WE Jr, Gnagy EM, Greenslade KE, Milich R. Teacher ratings of DSM-III-R symptoms for the disruptive behavior disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1992;31:210 218 13. Shaffer D, Fisher P, Dulcan MK, et al. The NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children, Version 2.3 (DISC 2.3): descriptive, acceptability, prevalence rates, and performance in the MECA Study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1996;35:865 877 14. Shekim WO, Kashani J, Beck N, et al. The prevalence of attention deficit disorders in a rural Midwestern community sample of nine-year-old children. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 1985;24:765770 15. Tuthill RW. Hair lead levels related to childrens classroom attentiondeficit behavior. Arch Environ Health. 1996;51:214 220 16. Wolraich ML, Hannah JN, Pinnock TY, Baumgaertal A, Brown J. Comparison of diagnostic criteria for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a county-wide sample. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1996; 35:319 324 17. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 3rd ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1980 18. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 3rd ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1987 19. Wolraich ML, Hannah JN, Baumgaertal A, Feurer ID. Examination of

http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/107/3/e43 Downloaded from www.pediatrics.org by on May 29, 2006

9 of 11

20.

21.

22.

23.

24. 25. 26.

27. 28.

29. 30. 31. 32.

33.

34.

35. 36. 37.

38. 39.

40.

41.

42.

43. 44.

45.

46.

47. 48.

DSM-IV criteria for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a county-wide sample. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1998;19:162168 August GJ, Ostrander R, Bloomquist MJ. Attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder: an epidemiological screening method. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 1992;62:387396 Bird HR, Canino G, Rubio-Stipec M, et al. Estimates of the prevalence of childhood maladjustments in a community survey in Puerto Rico. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988;45:1120 1126 Costello EJ, Angold A, Burns BJ, et al. The Great Smokey Mountains study of youth: goals, design, methods, and the prevalence of DSMIII-R disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;53:1129 1136 Loney J, Milich R. Hyperactivity, inattention, and aggression in clinical practice. In: Routh D, Wolraich M, eds. Advances in Development and Behavioral Pediatrics. Vol. 3. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press; 1982:113147 Costello EJ, Costello AJ, Edelbrock C, et al. Psychiatric disorders in pediatric primary care. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988;45:11071116 Costello EJ, Edelbrock C, Costello AJ, et al. Psychopathology in pediatric primary care: the new hidden morbidity. Pediatrics. 1988;82:415 424 Lindgren S, Wolraich M, Stromquist A, et al. Diagnosis of attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder by primary care physicians. Paper presented at the Mental Health Services for Children and Adolescents in Primary Care Settings: A Research Conference; 1989; New Haven, CT Brown RT, Sawyer MG. Medications for School-Aged Children: Effects on Learning and Behavior. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1998 Costello AJ, Edelbrock CS, Kalas R, Kessler MK, Klaric SA. National Institute of Mental Health Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children. Bethesda, MD: National Institute of Mental Health; 1982 Hasselblad V, Hedges LV. Meta-analysis of screening and diagnostic tests. Psychol Bull. 1995;117:167178 Conners CK. Conners Rating Scales Revised: Instruments for Use With Children and Adolescents. New York, NY: Multi-Health Systems, Inc; 1997 Barkley RA, Murphy KR. Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Clinical Workbook. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1998 Atkins MS, Pelham WE, Licht MH. A comparison of objective classroom measures and teacher ratings of attention deficit disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1985;13:155167 Horn WF, Wagner AE, Ialongo N. Sex differences in school-aged children with pervasive attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1989;17:109 124 Tarnowski KJ, Prinz RJ, Nay SM. Comparative analysis of attentional deficits in hyperactive and learning-disabled children. J Abnorm Psychol. 1986;95:341345 Ullman RK, Sleator EK, Sprague RL. ACTeRS: Teacher and Parent Forms Manual. Champaign, IL: MeriTech, Inc; 1997 Needleman HL, Gatsonis CA. Low-level lead exposure and the IQ of children: a meta-analysis of modern studies. JAMA. 1990;263:673 678 Pocock SJ, Smith M, Baghurst P. Environmental lead and childrens intelligence: a systematic review of the epidemiological evidence. BMJ. 1994;309:1189 1197 Barlow PJ. A pilot study on the metal levels in the hair of hyperactive children. Med Hypotheses. 1983;309 318 Gittelman R, Eskenazi B. Lead and hyperactivity revisited: an investigation of nondisadvantaged children. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1983;40: 827 833 Kahn CA, Kelly PC, Walker WO Jr. Lead screening in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and developmental delay. Clin Pediatr. 1995;34:498 501 Silva PA, Hughes P, Williams S, Faed JM. Blood lead, intelligence, reading attainment, and behavior in 11-year-old children in Dernedin, New Zealand. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1988;29:4352 Thomson GOB, Raab GM, Hepburn WS, et al. Blood lead levels and childrens behavior: results from the Edinburgh lead study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1989;30:515528 Becker JB, Breedlove SM, Crews D. Behavioral Endocrinology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1992 Elia J, Gulotta C, Rose SR, Marin G, Rappaport JL. Thyroid functions and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1994;33:169 172 Spencer T, Biederman J, Wilens T, Guite J, Harding M. ADHD and thyroid abnormalities: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1995; 36:879 885 Stein MA, Weiss RE, Refetoff S. Neurocognitive characteristics of individuals with resistance to thyroid hormone: comparisons with individuals with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1995;16:406 411 Weiss RE, Stein MA, Trommer B, Refetoff S. Attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder and thyroid function. J Pediatr. 1993;123:539 545 Castellanos FX, Geidd JN, Marsh WL, et al. Quantitative brain magnetic

49.

50.

51.

52.

53.

54.

55.

56.

57.

58.

59.

60.

61.

62.

63.

64.

65. 66.

67.

68. 69.

70.

71.

72.

73.

resonance imaging in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;53:607 616 Filipek PA, Semrud-Clikeman M, Steingard RJ, et al. Volumetric MRI analysis comparing subjects having attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder with manual controls. Neurology. 1997;48:589 601 Harcherik DF, Cohen DJ, Ort S, et al. Computed tomographic brain scanning in four neuropsychiatric disorders in childhood. Am J Psychiatry. 1985;146:731734 Hynd GW, Semrud-Clikerman M, Lorys AR, Novey ES, Eliopulos D. Brain morphology and developmental dyslexia and attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder. Arch Neurol. 1990;47:919 926 Hynd GW, Semrud-Clikeman M, Lorys AR, et al. Corpus callosum morphology and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: morphometric analysis of MRI. J Learn Disab. 1991;24:141146 Hynd GW, Hern KL, Novey ES, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and asymmetry of the caudate nucleus. J Child Neurol. 1993;8: 339 347 Lyoo IK, Noam GG, Lee CK, Lee HK. The corpus callosum and lateral ventricles in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a brain magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol Psychiatry. 1996;40: 1060 1063 Semrud-Clikeman M, Filipek PA, Biederman J, et al. Attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder: magnetic resonance imaging morphometric analysis of the corpus callosum. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1994;33:875 881 Shaywitz BA, Shaywitz SE, Byrne T, Cohen DJ, Rothman S. Attentiondeficit disorder: quantitative analysis of CT. Neurology. 1983;33: 1500 1503 Holcomb PJ, Ackerman PT, Dykman RA. Cognitive event-related brain potentials in children with attention and reading deficits. Psychophysiology. 1985;22:656 667 Lahat E, Airtal E, Barr J, Berkovitch M, Arlagoroff A, Aladjern M. BAEP studies in children with attention deficit disorders. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1995;37:119 123 Matsuura M, Okubo Y, Toru M, et al. A cross-national EEG study of children with emotional and behavioral problems: a WHO collaborative study in the Western Pacific Region. Biol Psychiatry. 1993;34:59 65 Newton JE, Oglesby DM, Ackerman PT, Dykman RA. Visual slow brain potentials in children with attention deficit disorder. Integr Physiol Behav Sci. 1994;29:39 54 Robaey P, Breton F, Dugas M, Renault B. An event-related potential study of controlled and automatic processes in 6 8-year-old boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1992;82:330 340 Satterfield JH, Schell Am, Nicholas TW, Satterfield BT, Freese TE. Ontogeny of selective attention effects on event-related potentials in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and normal boys. Biol Psychiatry. 1990;28:879 903 Valdizan JR, Andreau AC. Test of repeated operations and logistic regression as to the efficacy of brain mapping. Clin Electroencephalogr. 1993;24:697703 Reeves JC, Werry JS, Elkind GS, Zametkin A. Attention deficit, conduct, oppositional and anxiety disorders in children: II. Clinical characteristics. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1987;26:144 155 Trommer BL, Hoeppner JA, Lorber R, Armstrong KJ. The go-no-go paradigm in attention deficit disorder. Ann Neurol. 1998;24:610 614 Accardo PJ, Tomazic T, Morrow J, Haake C, Whitman BY. Minor malformations, hyperactivity and learning disabilities. Am J Disab Child. 1991;145:1184 1187 Gillberg C, Carlstrom G, Rasmussen P, Waldenstrom E. Perceptual, motor and attentional deficits in seven-year-old children: neurological screening aspects. Acta Paediatr. 1983;72:119 124 Vitiello B, Stoff D, Atkins M, Mahoney A. Soft neurological signs and impulsivity in children. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1991;11:112115 Cacabelos R, Albarran M, Dieguez C, et al. GRF-induced GH response in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Methods Find Exp Clin Pharmacol. 1990;12:79 85 Cook EH, Stein MA, Ellison T, Unis AS, Leventhal BL. Attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder and whole-serotonin levels: effects of comorbidity. Psychiatry Res. 1995;57:1320 Hole R, Lingjaerde O, Morkrid L, et al. Attention deficit disorders: a study of peptide-containing urinary complexes. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1988;9:205212 LaHoste GJ, Swanson JM, Wigal SB, et al. Dopamine D4 receptor gene polymorphism is associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 1996;1:121124 Pliszka SR, Maas JW, Javors MA, Rogeness GA, Baker J. Urinary catecholamines in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder with and with-

10 of 11

TECHNICAL REPORT: ADHD IN PRIMARY CARE SETTINGS Downloaded from www.pediatrics.org by on May 29, 2006

74.

75.

76. 77.

78.

out comorbid anxiety. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1994;33: 11651173 Warren RP, Odell JD, Warren WL, Burger RA, Maciulis A, Torres AR. Is decreased blood plasma concentration of the complement C4B protein associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1995;34:1009 1014 Barkley RA, Grodzinsky GM. Are tests of frontal lobe functions useful in the diagnosis of attention deficit disorders? Clin Neuropsychol. 1994; 8:121139 Breen MJ. Cognitive and behavioral differences in ADHD boys and girls. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1989;30:711716 Carter CS, Krener P, Chaderjian M, Northcutt C, Wolfe V. Abnormal processing of irrelevant information in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Res. 1995;56:59 70 Cohen ML, Kelly PC, Atkinson AW. Parent, teacher, child: a trilateral approach to attention deficit disorder. Am J Dis Child. 1989;143: 1229 1233

79. Dykman RA, Ackerman PT. Attention-deficit disorder and specific reading disability: separate but often overlapping disorders. J Learn Dis. 1991;24:96 103 80. Fischer M, Newby RF, Gordon M. Who are the false negatives on continuous performance tests? J Clin Child Psychol. 1995;24:427 433 81. Grant ML, Ilai D, Nussbaum NL, Bigler ED. The relationship between continuous performance tasks and neuropsychological tests in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1990;142:731734 82. Halperin JM, Newcorn JH, Matier K, Sharma, V, McKay KE, Schwartz S. Discriminant validity of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1993;32:1038 1043 83. Loge DV, Staton D, Beatty WW. Performance of children with ADHD on tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1990;29:540 545 84. Seidel WT, Joshko M. Assessment of attention in children. Clin Neuropsychol. 1991;5:53 66

http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/107/3/e43 Downloaded from www.pediatrics.org by on May 29, 2006

11 of 11

Prevalence and Assessment of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in Primary Care Settings Ronald T. Brown, Wendy S. Freeman, James M. Perrin, Martin T. Stein, Robert W. Amler, Heidi M. Feldman, Karen Pierce and Mark L. Wolraich Pediatrics 2001;107;43DOI: 10.1542/peds.107.3.e43 This information is current as of May 29, 2006
Updated Information & Services References including high-resolution figures, can be found at: http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/107/3/e43 This article cites 68 articles, 13 of which you can access for free at: http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/107/3/e43#BIBL This article has been cited by 2 HighWire-hosted articles: http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/107/3/e43#otherarticle s This article, along with others on similar topics, appears in the following collection(s): Office Practice http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/collection/office_practice Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures, tables) or in its entirety can be found online at: http://www.pediatrics.org/misc/Permissions.shtml Information about ordering reprints can be found online: http://www.pediatrics.org/misc/reprints.shtml

Citations

Subspecialty Collections

Permissions & Licensing

Reprints

Downloaded from www.pediatrics.org by on May 29, 2006

Вам также может понравиться